Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 171
Filter
1.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 17906, 2020 10 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33087857

ABSTRACT

Cannibalistic necrophagy is rarely observed in social hymenopterans, although a lack of food could easily favour such behaviour. One of the main supposed reasons for the rarity of necrophagy is that eating of nestmate corpses carries the risk of rapid spread of pathogens or parasites. Here we present an experimental laboratory study on behaviour indicating consumption of nestmate corpses in the ant Formica polyctena. We examined whether starvation and the fungal infection level of the corpses affects the occurrence of cannibalistic necrophagy. Our results showed that the ants distinguished between corpses of different types and with different levels of infection risk, adjusting their behaviour accordingly. The frequency of behaviours indicating cannibalistic necrophagy increased during starvation, although these behaviours seem to be fairly common in F. polyctena even in the presence of other food sources. The occurrence and significance of cannibalistic necrophagy deserve further research because, in addition to providing additional food, it may be part of the hygienic behaviour repertoire. The ability to detect infections and handle pathogens are important behavioural adaptations for social insects, crucial for the fitness of both individual workers and the entire colony.


Subject(s)
Ants/physiology , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Cadaver , Perception/physiology , Social Behavior , Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Animals , Cannibalism , Health Behavior , Starvation/psychology
2.
Nat Metab ; 2(10): 1096-1112, 2020 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33046910

ABSTRACT

Steroid hormones are crucial regulators of life-stage transitions during development in animals. However, the molecular mechanisms by which developmental transition through these stages is coupled with optimal metabolic homeostasis remains poorly understood. Here, we demonstrate through mathematical modelling and experimental validation that ecdysteroid-induced metabolic remodelling from resource consumption to conservation can be a successful life-history strategy to maximize fitness in Drosophila larvae in a fluctuating environment. Specifically, the ecdysteroid-inducible protein ImpL2 protects against hydrolysis of circulating trehalose following pupal commitment in larvae. Stored glycogen and triglycerides in the fat body are also conserved, even under fasting conditions. Moreover, pupal commitment dictates reduced energy expenditure upon starvation to maintain available resources, thus negotiating trade-offs in resource allocation at the physiological and behavioural levels. The optimal stage-specific metabolic shift elucidated by our predictive and empirical approaches reveals that Drosophila has developed a highly controlled system for ensuring robust development that may be conserved among higher-order organisms in response to intrinsic and extrinsic cues.


Subject(s)
Drosophila/metabolism , Starvation/metabolism , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Body Size , Drosophila Proteins/metabolism , Eating , Ecdysteroids/pharmacology , Glycogen/metabolism , Insulin-Like Growth Factor Binding Proteins/metabolism , Larva , Life History Traits , Lipid Metabolism , Metabolomics , Models, Theoretical , Pupa/metabolism , Starvation/psychology , Triglycerides/metabolism
3.
J Nerv Ment Dis ; 208(9): 654-657, 2020 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32868687

ABSTRACT

The aim of this article is to emphasize that starvation is an important potential consequence of psychosis and to provide recommendations for management of this condition. A review of the literature on food refusal and starvation in patients with psychotic illnesses was performed. Our search strategy returned 54 articles with one article meeting inclusion criteria. Additional independent research returned an additional four cases of patients with psychosis engaging in self-starvation. The cases of several patients from our institution who engaged in self-starvation behaviors as a result of psychosis are also presented. The management and outcomes of each of these 10 patients are discussed. Starvation secondary to psychosis is an important but underappreciated consequence of psychosis that can lead to serious adverse outcomes in these patients. Few cases have been reported in the literature. More study is warranted to develop evidence-based management guidelines.


Subject(s)
Feeding and Eating Disorders/psychology , Psychotic Disorders/psychology , Schizophrenic Psychology , Adult , Aged , Antipsychotic Agents/therapeutic use , Delusions/psychology , Feeding and Eating Disorders/etiology , Feeding and Eating Disorders/therapy , Female , Fluid Therapy , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Paranoid Disorders/psychology , Parenteral Nutrition , Psychotic Disorders/complications , Psychotic Disorders/drug therapy , Schizophrenia/complications , Schizophrenia/drug therapy , Starvation/etiology , Starvation/psychology , Starvation/therapy , Young Adult
4.
Br J Psychiatry ; 213(4): 579-586, 2018 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30160644

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The Chinese Great Famine caused widespread starvation in 1959-1961. Its long-term association with depressive symptoms has not been studied.AimsTo estimate the burden of depressive symptoms and the association of famine exposure with depressive symptoms. METHOD: The China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study is a nationwide representative survey of 17 708 Chinese adults aged ≥45. Propensity score matching and modified Poisson regression were used to evaluate the association between self-reported famine exposure in early life and depressive symptoms among the overall participants. Such associations were also assessed by developmental stage using modified Poisson regression and logistic regression. RESULTS: The prevalence of depressive symptoms was 26.2% (95% CI 25.1-27.3%) in 2011. As defined by loss of family members because of starvation, 11.6% (95% CI 10.1-13.1%) of this population experienced severe famine. When compared with participants who did not experience starvation, those who had experienced severe famine during fetal, mid-childhood, young-teenage and early-adulthood stages had 1.87 (95% CI 1.36-2.55), 1.54 (95% CI 1.23-1.94), 1.47 (95% CI 1.09-2.00) and 1.77 (95% CI 1.42-2.21) times higher odds of having depressive symptoms in late adulthood, respectively. The first two trimesters of pregnancy were a critical time window during the fetal stage when severe famine had a stronger association with depressive symptoms. Famine during infant, toddler, preschool or teenage stages was not associated with depressive symptoms. Overall, famine contributed to 13.6% of the depressive symptom burden in this population. CONCLUSIONS: The Chinese Great Famine contributed substantially to the burden of depressive symptoms in China.Declaration of interestNone.


Subject(s)
Depression/epidemiology , Starvation/epidemiology , Starvation/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Child , Child, Preschool , China/epidemiology , Depression/etiology , Female , Health Surveys , Humans , Logistic Models , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Pregnancy , Prenatal Exposure Delayed Effects/epidemiology , Prevalence , Propensity Score , Retirement , Young Adult
5.
J Neurosci ; 38(20): 4641-4654, 2018 05 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29712787

ABSTRACT

Animal behavior is critically dependent on the activity of neuropeptides. Reversals, one of the most conspicuous behaviors in Caenorhabditis elegans, plays an important role in determining the navigation strategy of the animal. Our experiments on hermaphrodite C. elegans show the involvement of a neuropeptide FLP-18 in modulating reversal length in these hermaphrodites. We show that FLP-18 controls the reversal length by regulating the activity of AVA interneurons through the G-protein-coupled neuropeptide receptors, NPR-4 and NPR-1. We go on to show that the site of action of these receptors is the AVA interneuron for NPR-4 and the ASE sensory neurons for NPR-1. We further show that mutants in the neuropeptide, flp-18, and its receptors show increased reversal lengths. Consistent with the behavioral data, calcium levels in the AVA neuron of freely reversing C. elegans were significantly higher and persisted for longer durations in flp-18, npr-1, npr-4, and npr-1 npr-4 genetic backgrounds compared with wild-type control animals. Finally, we show that increasing FLP-18 levels through genetic and physiological manipulations causes shorter reversal lengths. Together, our analysis suggests that the FLP-18/NPR-1/NPR-4 signaling is a pivotal point in the regulation of reversal length under varied genetic and environmental conditions.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT In this study, we elucidate the circuit and molecular machinery required for normal reversal behavior in hermaphrodite Caenorhabditis elegans We delineate the circuit and the neuropeptide receptors required for maintaining reversal length in C. elegans Our work sheds light on the importance of a single neuropeptide, FLP-18, and how change in levels in this one peptide could allow the animal to change the length of its reversal, thereby modulating how the C. elegans explores its environment. We also go on to show that FLP-18 functions to maintain reversal length through the neuropeptide receptors NPR-4 and NPR-1. Our study will allow for a better understanding of the complete repertoire of behaviors shown by freely moving animals as they explore their environment.


Subject(s)
Caenorhabditis elegans Proteins/physiology , Caenorhabditis elegans/physiology , Neuropeptides/physiology , Receptors, Neuropeptide Y/physiology , Receptors, Neuropeptide/physiology , Animals , Animals, Genetically Modified , Behavior, Animal , Caenorhabditis elegans Proteins/genetics , Calcium/metabolism , Interneurons/physiology , Locomotion/physiology , Mutation/genetics , Mutation/physiology , Neuropeptides/genetics , Optogenetics , Receptors, Neuropeptide/genetics , Receptors, Neuropeptide Y/genetics , Sensory Receptor Cells , Signal Transduction/genetics , Signal Transduction/physiology , Starvation/genetics , Starvation/psychology
7.
Int J Epidemiol ; 47(1): 109-120, 2018 02 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29126190

ABSTRACT

Background: Existing studies of the 1944-45 Dutch famine found little evidence of the association between early life malnutrition and midlife cognition. Methods: Among 2446 rural participants born between 1958 and 1963 in the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study, we examined effects of exposure to China's 1959-61 Great Leap Forward famine during prenatal and early postnatal life, on four cognitive measures in 2011 (baseline) and changes in cognition between 2011 and 2013 (first follow-up). We obtained difference-in-differences (DID) estimates of the famine effects by exploiting temporal variation in the timing and duration of famine exposure across six birth cohorts born between 1958 and 1963, together with geographical variation in famine severity at the prefecture level. Results: After adjusting for gender, marital status and provincial fixed effects, we found that the 1961 cohort who experienced full-term prenatal and partial-term postnatal exposures to famine had lower scores on the Telephone Interview of Cognitive Status (TICS), a test of drawing pentagons, and general cognition at age 50 years compared with the unexposed 1963 cohort. Adjusting for education, the famine effects on drawing pentagons and general cognition were fully attenuated, but the effect on TICS persisted. We also found a robust negative famine effect on the longitudinal change in general cognition during the 2-year follow-up in the 1959 cohort. Conclusions: Severe nutritional deprivation during prenatal and postnatal periods has a lasting impact on cognitive performance in Chinese adults in their early 50s.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/epidemiology , Starvation/psychology , Aged , China/epidemiology , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Female , Humans , Life Change Events , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Pregnancy , Prenatal Exposure Delayed Effects/epidemiology , Rural Health/statistics & numerical data , Starvation/epidemiology
8.
Clin Psychol Psychother ; 24(1): 1-21, 2017 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27739190

ABSTRACT

The need for novel approaches to understanding and treating anorexia nervosa (AN) is well recognized. The aim of this paper is to describe an integrative bio-psycho-social theory of maintaining factors in AN. We took a triangulation approach to develop a clinically relevant theory with face validity and internal consistency. We developed theoretical ideas from our clinical practice and reviewed theoretical ideas within the eating disorders and wider bio-psycho-social literature. The synthesis of these ideas and concepts into a clinically meaningful framework is described here. We suggest eight key factors central to understanding the maintenance and treatment resistance of anorexia nervosa: genetic or experiential predisposing factors; dysfunctional feelings processing and regulation systems; excessive vulnerable feelings; 'feared self' beliefs; starvation as a maladaptive physiological feelings regulation mechanism; maladaptive psychological coping modes; maladaptive social behaviour; and unmet physical and psychological core needs. Each of these factors serves to maintain the disorder. The concept of universal physical and psychological core needs can provide an underpinning integrative framework for working with this distinctly physical and psychological disorder. This framework could be used within any treatment model. We suggest that treatments which help address the profound lack of trust, emotional security and self-acceptance in this patient group will in turn address unmet needs and improve well-being. Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. KEY PRACTITIONER MESSAGE: The concept of unmet physical and psychological needs can be used as an underlying integrative framework for understanding and working with this patient group, alongside any treatment model. A functional understanding of the neuro-biological, physiological and psychological mechanisms involved in anorexia nervosa can help patients reduce self-criticism and shame. Fears about being or becoming fat, greedy, needy, selfish and unacceptable ('Feared Self') drive over-compensatory self-depriving behaviour ('Anorexic Self'). Psychological treatment for anorexia nervosa should emphasize a focus on feelings and fostering experiences of acceptance and trust. Treatment for patients with anorexia nervosa needs to be longer than current clinical practice.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/psychology , Biobehavioral Sciences , Psychological Theory , Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Anorexia Nervosa/physiopathology , Anorexia Nervosa/therapy , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Culture , Emotions/physiology , Gyrus Cinguli/physiopathology , Humans , Neurotransmitter Agents/physiology , Psychological Distance , Risk Factors , Self-Assessment , Social Behavior , Starvation/physiopathology , Starvation/psychology , Starvation/therapy , Treatment Failure , Trust/psychology
9.
J Hum Hypertens ; 30(12): 755-760, 2016 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27334523

ABSTRACT

Substantial evidence links exaggerated mental stress induced blood pressure reactivity to future hypertension, but the results for heart rate reactivity are less clear. For this reason multivariate cluster analysis was carried out to examine the relationship between heart rate and blood pressure reactivity patterns and hypertension in a large prospective cohort (age range 55-60 years). Four clusters emerged with statistically different systolic and diastolic blood pressure and heart rate reactivity patterns. Cluster 1 was characterised by a relatively exaggerated blood pressure and heart rate response while the blood pressure and heart rate responses of cluster 2 were relatively modest and in line with the sample mean. Cluster 3 was characterised by blunted cardiovascular stress reactivity across all variables and cluster 4, by an exaggerated blood pressure response and modest heart rate response. Membership to cluster 4 conferred an increased risk of hypertension at 5-year follow-up (hazard ratio=2.98 (95% CI: 1.50-5.90), P<0.01) that survived adjustment for a host of potential confounding variables. These results suggest that the cardiac reactivity plays a potentially important role in the link between blood pressure reactivity and hypertension and support the use of multivariate approaches to stress psychophysiology.


Subject(s)
Blood Pressure , Cardiovascular System/physiopathology , Heart Rate , Hypertension/etiology , Starvation/complications , Stress, Psychological/etiology , Chi-Square Distribution , Cluster Analysis , Female , Humans , Hypertension/diagnosis , Hypertension/physiopathology , Hypertension/psychology , Logistic Models , Male , Middle Aged , Multivariate Analysis , Pregnancy , Prenatal Exposure Delayed Effects , Prognosis , Risk Assessment , Risk Factors , Starvation/diagnosis , Starvation/physiopathology , Starvation/psychology , Stress, Psychological/diagnosis , Stress, Psychological/physiopathology , Stress, Psychological/psychology , Time Factors
10.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 60: 26-30, 2016 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26608248

ABSTRACT

We have suggested that reduced food intake increases the risk for anorexia nervosa by engaging mesolimbic dopamine neurons, thereby initially rewarding dieting. Recent fMRI studies have confirmed that dopamine neurons are activated in anorexia nervosa, but it is not clear whether this response is due to the disorder or to its resulting nutritional deficit. When the body senses the shortage of nutrients, it rapidly shifts behavior toward foraging for food as a normal physiological response and the mesolimbic dopamine neurons may be involved in that process. On the other hand, the altered dopamine status of anorexics has been suggested to result from a brain abnormality that underlies their complex emotional disorder. We suggest that the outcomes of the treatments that emerge from that perspective remain poor because they target the mental symptoms that are actually the consequences of the food deprivation that accompanies anorexia. On the other hand, a method that normalizes the disordered eating behavior of anorexics results in much better physiological, behavioral, and emotional outcomes.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/metabolism , Dopamine/metabolism , Animals , Anorexia Nervosa/complications , Anorexia Nervosa/psychology , Anorexia Nervosa/therapy , Comorbidity , Diet, Reducing , Humans , Starvation/metabolism , Starvation/psychology
12.
Chronobiol Int ; 32(9): 1264-77, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26513010

ABSTRACT

Influences of starvation, re-feeding and time of food supply on daily rhythm features of melatonin (5-methoxy-N-acetyltryptamine) and its key regulator AANAT (arylalkylamine N-acetyltransferase) protein in the gut tissues were separately evaluated in carp Catla catla. The first experiment was aimed at demonstration of duration dependent effects of starvation and re-feeding after starvation on the daily profiles and rhythm features of gut melatonin and AANAT. Accordingly, juvenile carp were randomly distributed in three groups, which were (a) provided with balanced diet daily at a fixed time, that is, 10:00 clock hour or zeitgeber time (ZT) 4 (control), or (b) starved (for 2-, 4-, 6- or 8 days), or (c) initially starved for 8 days and then re-fed (for 2-, 4-, 6-, 8-, 12- or 16 days) daily with the same food and at the time (ZT4) used for control fish. The carp in each group were sampled for collection of gut tissues at six different time points at a regular interval of 4 h in a daily cycle. In another experiment, the influences of timing of food supply were separately examined in four fish groups, which were provided with a fixed amount of food once daily either at 06:00 or 12:00 or 18:00 or 24:00 clock hour corresponding to ZT0 or ZT6 or ZT12 or ZT18, respectively, for 7 days before sampling at 12 different time points with a regular interval of 2 h in a 24-h cycle. The study revealed a gradual increase in the mesor and amplitude values of melatonin and AANAT in gut with the progress of starvation till their values reached maximum at day-6 and remained steady thereafter. In contrast, re-feeding of 8-day starved fish resulted in a sharp decrease in their mesor and amplitude values after 2 days and then followed by a steady-state increase till re-attainment of their values close to control fish at the end of 16 days. The acrophase of these gut variables in each control, starved and re-fed fish was noted mostly at midday or ZT6. However, the results of another experiment demonstrated that a shift of food supply time led to a shift in their acrophase. The amount of residual food in the gut lumen in each, but not starved, fish by showing a significant positive correlation independently with the gut levels of melatonin and AANAT also indicated possible role of food as the synchronizer for their daily rhythms. Collectively, it appears reasonable to argue that daily profiles of gut melatonin and AANAT are strongly influenced by the availability of food, while their daily rhythm features seem to be dependent mostly on the time of food supply in carp.


Subject(s)
Carps/metabolism , Circadian Rhythm , Eating , Feeding Behavior , Fish Proteins/metabolism , Food Deprivation , Gastrointestinal Tract/metabolism , Melatonin/metabolism , Starvation/metabolism , Animal Feed , Animals , Arylalkylamine N-Acetyltransferase/metabolism , Gastrointestinal Tract/physiopathology , Starvation/physiopathology , Starvation/psychology , Time Factors
13.
Nat Neurosci ; 18(11): 1571-3, 2015 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26457555

ABSTRACT

People routinely make poor choices, despite knowledge of negative consequences. The authors found that individuals with anorexia nervosa, who make maladaptive food choices to the point of starvation, engaged the dorsal striatum more than healthy controls when making choices about what to eat, and that activity in fronto-striatal circuits was correlated with their actual food consumption in a meal the next day.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/physiopathology , Corpus Striatum/physiopathology , Food Preferences , Nerve Net/physiopathology , Starvation/psychology , Adaptation, Physiological , Decision Making/physiology , Humans
14.
Physiol Behav ; 149: 331-9, 2015 Oct 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26143188

ABSTRACT

Body weight and the levels of stored body fat have fitness consequences. Greater levels of fat may provide protection against catastrophic failures in the food supply, but they may also increase the risk of predation. Animals may therefore regulate their fatness according to their perceived risks of predation and starvation: the starvation-predation trade-off model. We tested the predictions of this model in wood mice (Apodemus sylvaticus) by experimentally manipulating predation risk and starvation risk. We predicted that under increased predation risk individuals would lose weight and under increased starvation risk they would gain it. We simulated increased predation risk by playing the calls made by predatory birds (owls: Tyto alba and Bubo bubo) to the mice. Control groups included exposure to calls of a non-predatory bird (blackbird: Turdus merula) or silence. Mice exposed to owl calls at night lost weight relative to the silence group, mediated via reduced food intake, but exposure to owl calls in the day had no significant effect. Exposure to blackbird calls at night also resulted in weight loss, but blackbird calls in the day had no effect. Mice seemed to have a generalised response to bird calls at night irrespective of their actual source. This could be because in the wild any bird calling at night will be a predation risk, and any bird calling in the day would not be, because at that time the mice would normally be resting, and hence not exposed to avian predators. Consequently, mice have not evolved to distinguish different types of call but only to respond to the time of day that they occur. Mice exposed to stochastic 24h starvation events altered their behaviour (reduced activity) during the refeeding days that followed the deprivation periods to regain the lost mass. However, they only marginally elevated their food intake and consequently had reduced body weight/fat storage compared to that of the control unstarved group. This response may have been constrained by physiological factors (alimentary tract absorption capacity) or behavioural factors (perceived risk of predation). Overall the responses of the mice appeared to provide limited support for the starvation-predation trade-off model, and suggest that wood mice are much more sensitive to predation risk than they are to starvation risk.


Subject(s)
Fasting/physiology , Murinae/physiology , Predatory Behavior/physiology , Starvation/physiopathology , Acoustic Stimulation/adverse effects , Analysis of Variance , Animals , Body Mass Index , Body Weight/physiology , Corticosterone/metabolism , Eating/physiology , Hyperphagia/etiology , Leptin/blood , Models, Animal , Oxygen Consumption , Risk Factors , Starvation/blood , Starvation/psychology , Time Factors
15.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 112(9): 2688-92, 2015 Mar 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25730858

ABSTRACT

Hunger motivates people to consume food, for which finding and acquiring food is a prerequisite. We test whether the acquisition component spills over to nonfood objects: Are hungry people more likely to acquire objects that cannot satisfy their hunger? Five laboratory and field studies show that hunger increases the accessibility of acquisition-related concepts and the intention to acquire not only food but also nonfood objects. Moreover, people act on this intention and acquire more nonfood objects (e.g., binder clips) when they are hungry, both when these items are freely available and when they must be paid for. However, hunger does not influence how much they like nonfood objects. We conclude that a basic biologically based motivation can affect substantively unrelated behaviors that cannot satisfy the motivation. This presumably occurs because hunger renders acquisition-related concepts and behaviors more accessible, which influences decisions in situations to which they can be applied.


Subject(s)
Behavior , Decision Making , Hunger , Starvation/psychology , Adult , Humans , Male
16.
Exp Gerontol ; 65: 46-52, 2015 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25769732

ABSTRACT

Conventional invertebrate models of aging have provided striking examples for the influence of food- and nutrient-sensing on lifespan and stress resilience. On the other hand, studies in highly social insects, such as honey bees, have revealed how social context can shape very plastic life-history traits, for example flexible aging dynamics in the helper caste (workers). It is, however, not understood how food perception and stress resilience are connected in honey bee workers with different social task behaviors and aging dynamics. To explore this linkage, we tested if starvation resilience, which normally declines with age, depends on food responsiveness in honey bees. We studied two typically non-senesced groups of worker bees with different social task behaviors: mature nurses (caregivers) and mature foragers (food collectors). In addition, we included a group of old foragers for which functional senescence is well-established. Bees were individually scored for their food perception by measuring the gustatory response to different sucrose concentrations. Subsequently, individuals were tested for survival under starvation stress. We found that starvation stress resilience, but not gustatory responsiveness differed between workers with different social task behaviors (mature nurses vs. mature foragers). In addition starvation stress resilience differed between foragers with different aging progressions (mature foragers vs. old foragers). Control experiments confirmed that differences in starvation resilience between mature nurses and mature foragers were robust against changing experimental conditions, such as water provision and activity. For all worker groups we established that individuals with low gustatory responsiveness were more resilient to starvation stress. Finally, for the group of rapidly aging foragers we found that low food responsiveness was linked to a delayed age-related decline in starvation resilience. Our study highlights associations between reduced food perception, increased survival capacity and delayed aging in highly social honey bees. We discuss that these associations may involve canonical internal nutrient sensing pathways, which are shared between honey bees and animal models with less plastic aging dynamics.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Aging , Food , Stress, Physiological , Stress, Psychological , Aging/physiology , Aging/psychology , Animals , Bees , Models, Animal , Social Behavior , Starvation/psychology , Taste
17.
Obes Rev ; 16 Suppl 1: 25-35, 2015 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25614201

ABSTRACT

Whether dieting makes people fatter has been a subject of considerable controversy over the past 30 years. More recent analysis of several prospective studies suggest, however, that it is dieting to lose weight in people who are in the healthy normal range of body weight, rather than in those who are overweight or obese, that most strongly and consistently predict future weight gain. This paper analyses the ongoing arguments in the debate about whether repeated dieting to lose weight in normal-weight people represents unsuccessful attempts to counter genetic and familial predispositions to obesity, a psychosocial reaction to the fear of fatness or that dieting per se confers risks for fatness and hence a contributing factor to the obesity epidemic. In addressing the biological plausibility that dieting predisposes the lean (rather than the overweight or obese) to regaining more body fat than what had been lost (i.e. fat overshooting), it integrates the results derived from the re-analysis of body composition data on fat mass and fat-free mass (FFM) losses and recoveries from human studies of experimental energy restriction and refeeding. These suggest that feedback signals from the depletion of both fat mass (i.e. adipostats) and FFM (i.e. proteinstats) contribute to weight regain through the modulation of energy intake and adaptive thermogenesis, and that a faster rate of fat recovery relative to FFM recovery (i.e. preferential catch-up fat) is a central outcome of body composition autoregulation in lean individuals. Such a temporal desynchronization in the restoration of the body's fat vs. FFM results in a state of hyperphagia that persists beyond complete recovery of fat mass and interestingly until FFM is fully recovered. However, as this completion of FFM recovery is also accompanied by fat deposition, excess fat accumulates. In other words, fat overshooting is a prerequisite to allow complete recovery of FFM. This confers biological plausibility for post-dieting fat overshooting - which through repeated dieting and weight cycling would increase the risks for trajectories from leanness to fatness. Given the increasing prevalence of dieting in normal-weight female and male among young adults, adolescents and even children who perceive themselves as too fat (due to media, family and societal pressures), together with the high prevalence of dieting for optimizing performance among athletes in weight-sensitive sports, the notion that dieting and weight cycling may be predisposing a substantial proportion of the population to weight gain and obesity deserves greater scientific scrutiny.


Subject(s)
Adipokines/metabolism , Appetite Regulation , Body Composition , Diet, Reducing , Homeostasis , Obesity/physiopathology , Starvation/physiopathology , Thinness/physiopathology , Weight Gain , Diet, Reducing/adverse effects , Diet, Reducing/psychology , Genetic Predisposition to Disease , Humans , Obesity/metabolism , Obesity/psychology , Starvation/complications , Starvation/metabolism , Starvation/psychology , Thermogenesis , Thinness/metabolism , Thinness/psychology
18.
Eat Behav ; 17: 10-3, 2015 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25528717

ABSTRACT

The characteristic relentless self-starvation behaviour seen in Anorexia Nervosa (AN) has been described as evidence of compulsivity, with increasing suggestion of transdiagnostic parallels with addictive behaviour. There is a paucity of standardised self-report measures of compulsive behaviour in eating disorders (EDs). Measures that index the concept of compulsive self-starvation in AN are needed to explore the suggested parallels with addictions. With this aim a novel measure of self-starvation was developed (the Self-Starvation Scale, SS). 126 healthy participants, and 78 individuals with experience of AN, completed the new measure along with existing measures of eating disorder symptoms, anxiety and depression. Initial validation in the healthy sample indicated good reliability and construct validity, and incremental validity in predicting eating disorder symptoms. The psychometric properties of the SS scale were replicated in the AN sample. The ability of this scale to predict ED symptoms was particularly strong in individuals currently suffering from AN. These results suggest the SS may be a useful index of compulsive food restriction in AN. The concept of 'starvation dependence' in those with eating disorders, as a parallel with addiction, may be of clinical and theoretical importance.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/diagnosis , Anorexia Nervosa/psychology , Compulsive Behavior , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Starvation/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Anxiety/diagnosis , Anxiety/psychology , Case-Control Studies , Depression/diagnosis , Depression/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychometrics , Reproducibility of Results , Young Adult
19.
PLoS One ; 9(10): e110743, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25338075

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Recent research suggests certain neuropsychological deficits occur in anorexia nervosa (AN). The role of starvation in these deficits remains unclear. Studies of individuals without AN can elucidate our understanding of the effect of short-term starvation on neuropsychological performance. METHODS: Using a within-subjects repeated measures design, 60 healthy female participants were tested once after fasting for 18 hours, and once when satiated. Measures included two tasks to measure central coherence and a set-shifting task. RESULTS: Fasting exacerbated set-shifting difficulties on a rule-change task. Fasting was associated with stronger local and impaired global processing, indicating weaker central coherence. CONCLUSIONS: Models of AN that propose a central role for set-shifting difficulties or weak central coherence should also consider the impact of short-term fasting on these processes.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/psychology , Set, Psychology , Starvation/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Anxiety/psychology , Depression/psychology , Fasting/psychology , Female , Humans , Neuropsychological Tests , Sense of Coherence , Young Adult
20.
Appetite ; 77: 52-61, 2014 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24583414

ABSTRACT

Numerous investigations have been conducted on the impact of short-term fasting on cognition in healthy individuals. Some studies have suggested that fasting is associated with executive function deficits; however, findings have been inconsistent. The lack of consensus regarding the impact of short-term fasting in healthy controls has impeded investigation of the impact of starvation or malnutrition in clinical groups, such as anorexia nervosa (AN). One method of disentangling these effects is to examine acute episodes of starvation experimentally. The present review systematically investigated the impact of short-term fasting on cognition. Studies investigating attentional bias to food-related stimuli were excluded so as to focus on general cognition. Ten articles were included in the review. The combined results are equivocal: several studies report no observable differences as a result of fasting and others show specific deficits on tasks designed to test psychomotor speed, executive function, and mental rotation. This inconsistent profile of fasting in healthy individuals demonstrates the complexity of the role of short-term fasting in cognition; the variety of tasks used, composition of the sample, and type and duration of fasting across studies may also have contributed to the inconsistent profile. Additional focused studies on neuropsychological profiles of healthy individuals are warranted in order to better develop an understanding of the role of hunger in cognition.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Executive Function , Fasting/psychology , Feeding Behavior/psychology , Hunger , Nutritional Status , Starvation/psychology , Anorexia Nervosa/psychology , Humans , Malnutrition
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...