Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 38
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 158: 105450, 2024 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37925091

RESUMEN

Over the last decades, theoretical perspectives in the interdisciplinary field of the affective sciences have proliferated rather than converged due to differing assumptions about what human affective phenomena are and how they work. These metaphysical and mechanistic assumptions, shaped by academic context and values, have dictated affective constructs and operationalizations. However, an assumption about the purpose of affective phenomena can guide us to a common set of metaphysical and mechanistic assumptions. In this capstone paper, we home in on a nested teleological principle for human affective phenomena in order to synthesize metaphysical and mechanistic assumptions. Under this framework, human affective phenomena can collectively be considered algorithms that either adjust based on the human comfort zone (affective concerns) or monitor those adaptive processes (affective features). This teleologically-grounded framework offers a principled agenda and launchpad for both organizing existing perspectives and generating new ones. Ultimately, we hope the Human Affectome brings us a step closer to not only an integrated understanding of human affective phenomena, but an integrated field for affective research.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta , Emociones , Humanos
2.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 7(1): 63, 2022 07 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35841438

RESUMEN

Surgical face masks reduce the spread of airborne pathogens but also disturb the flow of information between individuals. The risk of getting seriously ill after infection with SARS-COV-2 during the present COVID-19 pandemic amplifies with age, suggesting that face masks should be worn especially during face-to-face contact with and between older people. However, the ability to accurately perceive and understand communication signals decreases with age, and it is currently unknown whether face masks impair facial communication more severely in older people. We compared the impact of surgical face masks on dynamic facial emotion recognition in younger (18-30 years) and older (65-85 years) adults (N = 96) in an online study. Participants watched short video clips of young women who facially expressed anger, fear, contempt or sadness. Faces of half of the women were covered by a digitally added surgical face mask. As expected, emotion recognition accuracy declined with age, and face masks reduced emotion recognition accuracy in both younger and older participants. Unexpectedly, the effect of face masks did not differ between age groups. Further analyses showed that masks also reduced the participants' overall confidence in their emotion judgements, but not their performance awareness (the difference between their confidence ratings for correct and incorrect responses). Again, there were no mask-by-age interactions. Finally, data obtained with a newly developed questionnaire (attitudes towards face masks, atom) suggest that younger and older people do not differ in how much they feel impaired in their understanding of other people's emotions by face masks or how useful they find face masks in confining the COVID-19 pandemic. In sum, these findings do not provide evidence that the impact of face masks on the decoding of facial signals is disproportionally larger in older people.


Asunto(s)
COVID-19 , Expresión Facial , Adulto , Anciano , COVID-19/prevención & control , Femenino , Humanos , Máscaras , Pandemias , SARS-CoV-2
3.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 128: 592-620, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34089764

RESUMEN

Social feelings have conceptual and empirical connections with affect and emotion. In this review, we discuss how they relate to cognition, emotion, behavior and well-being. We examine the functional neuroanatomy and neurobiology of social feelings and their role in adaptive social functioning. Existing neuroscience literature is reviewed to identify concepts, methods and challenges that might be addressed by social feelings research. Specific topic areas highlight the influence and modulation of social feelings on interpersonal affiliation, parent-child attachments, moral sentiments, interpersonal stressors, and emotional communication. Brain regions involved in social feelings were confirmed by meta-analysis using the Neurosynth platform for large-scale, automated synthesis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Words that relate specifically to social feelings were identfied as potential research variables. Topical inquiries into social media behaviors, loneliness, trauma, and social sensitivity, especially with recent physical distancing for guarding public and personal health, underscored the increasing importance of social feelings for affective and second person neuroscience research with implications for brain development, physical and mental health, and lifelong adaptive functioning.


Asunto(s)
Neurociencias , Interacción Social , Cognición , Emociones , Humanos , Conducta Social
4.
Cortex ; 131: 210-220, 2020 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32916419

RESUMEN

Current theories in cognitive neuroscience assume that internal simulation, i.e., the reproduction of brain activity underlying another person's inner state and behaviour in the perceiver's brain, plays an important role in understanding others. Here we test the prediction that common neural activity during facial communication of affect leads to interpersonal understanding. Six female senders and 30 male observers (six of which were the senders romantic partners and 24 unknown others) underwent pseudo-hyperscanning fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging). Senders were asked to submerge themselves into emotional situations and to facially express their emotional feelings as they arose to the observer. Observers were uninformed about the sender's task and were asked to watch and feel with the sender. Using between-brain spatial correlation analysis we found that mere emotion recognition was not closely related to the degree to which an observer reproduced the sender's spatial pattern of neural activity in his own brain. However, in runs in which the observer had correctly identified the communicated emotion, between-brain similarity of spatial patterns of neural activity predicted the degree to which the observer experienced a similar emotional feeling as the sender. This effect remained significant when differences between romantic partners and unknown others and sender effects were removed. These findings are in line with previous studies that suggest that facial emotion recognition, at least at a coarse level, might be supported by neural processes that do not rely on internal simulation. Shared affective experiences, on the other hand, might arise from common neural activity between the sender's and the observer's brain, leading to a "shared space of affect" which might be critical for the flow of more subtle affective information between brains.


Asunto(s)
Expresión Facial , Reconocimiento Facial , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Comunicación , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino
5.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 1938, 2020 02 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32029756

RESUMEN

How empathically people respond to a stranger's pain or pleasure does not only depend on the situational context, individual traits and intentions, but also on interindividual factors. Here we ask whether empathic responses towards unknown others are modulated by behavioural similarity as a potential marker of genetic relatedness. Participants watched two supposed human players who were modelled as having a strong (player LP) or weak (player NLP) tendency to lead in social situations executing penalty shots in a virtual reality robot soccer game. As predicted, empathic response were modulated by shared behavioural traits: participants whose tendency to lead was more similar to player LP's tendency to lead experienced more reward, and showed stronger neural activity in reward-related brain regions, when they saw player LP score a goal, and participants whose tendency to lead was more similar to player NLP's tendency to lead showed stronger empathic responses when they saw player NLP score a goal. These findings highlight the potentially evolutionary grounded role of phenotypic similarity for neural processes underlying human social perception.


Asunto(s)
Conducta/fisiología , Empatía/fisiología , Adulto , Encéfalo/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Dolor/psicología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Recompensa , Percepción Social , Adulto Joven
6.
Neurosci Lett ; 694: 225-230, 2019 02 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30342078

RESUMEN

Observational and experimental studies have shown that sleep deprivation disinhibits emotional responses to disturbing and rewarding external events. On the other hand, most studies on sleep deprivation and interpersonal emotion recognition report that sensitivity to others' emotions is dampened during sleep deprivation. This is at odds with current neuroscientific theories of social cognition that assume that affective experiences and emotion recognition in others are closely tied at the neural and physiological level. In this observational study we show that sleep deprivation can actually increase emotion recognition accuracy from dynamically unfolding facial expressions if they are viewed sufficiently long. Participants viewed 2-4 s or 8-10s video clips of female senders who facially communicated anger, disgust, fear or sadness to their romantic partner and evaluated the sender's affective state in a forced-choice paradigm, either during sleep deprivation after a night shift (N = 40) or after normal night sleep (N = 50). All participants showed a significant increase in emotion recognition accuracy from 2-4 s to 8-10 s stimulus presentation times. Emotion recognition accuracy did not differ between sleep-deprived and control participants for 2-4 s videos, but sleep-deprived participants showed significantly higher emotion recognition accuracy than control participants for 8-10 s videos. We surmise that this effect might be due to the break-down of prefrontal activity associated with sleep deprivation, which might not only disinhibit affective responses to external events but might also release simulation-based neural processes that contribute to interpersonal emotion recognition from dynamic facial expressions at longer time scales than usually investigated in emotion recognition studies.


Asunto(s)
Emociones , Expresión Facial , Reconocimiento Facial , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Privación de Sueño/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Adulto Joven
7.
Front Neurol ; 8: 682, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29326646

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Affective dysfunctions are common in patients with Parkinson's disease, but the underlying neurobiological deviations have rarely been examined. Parkinson's disease is characterized by a loss of dopamine neurons in the substantia nigra resulting in impairment of motor and non-motor basal ganglia-cortical loops. Concerning emotional deficits, some studies provide evidence for altered brain processing in limbic- and lateral-orbitofrontal gating loops. In a second line of evidence, human premotor and inferior parietal homologs of mirror neuron areas were involved in processing and understanding of emotional facial expressions. We examined deviations in brain activation during processing of facial expressions in patients and related these to emotion recognition accuracy. METHODS: 13 patients and 13 healthy controls underwent an emotion recognition task and a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measurement. In the Emotion Hexagon test, participants were presented with blends of two emotions and had to indicate which emotion best described the presented picture. Blended pictures with three levels of difficulty were included. During fMRI scanning, participants observed video clips depicting emotional, non-emotional, and neutral facial expressions or were asked to produce these facial expressions themselves. RESULTS: Patients performed slightly worse in the emotion recognition task, but only when judging the most ambiguous facial expressions. Both groups activated inferior frontal and anterior inferior parietal homologs of mirror neuron areas during observation and execution of the emotional facial expressions. During observation, responses in the pars opercularis of the right inferior frontal gyrus, in the bilateral inferior parietal lobule and in the bilateral supplementary motor cortex were decreased in patients. Furthermore, in patients, activation of the right anterior inferior parietal lobule was positively related to accuracy in the emotion recognition task. CONCLUSION: Our data provide evidence for a contribution of human homologs of monkey mirror areas to the emotion recognition deficit in Parkinson's disease.

8.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 113(16): E2248-57, 2016 Apr 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27044071

RESUMEN

Being able to comprehend another person's intentions and emotions is essential for successful social interaction. However, it is currently unknown whether the human brain possesses a neural mechanism that attracts people to others whose mental states they can easily understand. Here we show that the degree to which a person feels attracted to another person can change while they observe the other's affective behavior, and that these changes depend on the observer's confidence in having correctly understood the other's affective state. At the neural level, changes in interpersonal attraction were predicted by activity in the reward system of the observer's brain. Importantly, these effects were specific to individual observer-target pairs and could not be explained by a target's general attractiveness or expressivity. Furthermore, using multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA), we found that neural activity in the reward system of the observer's brain varied as a function of how well the target's affective behavior matched the observer's neural representation of the underlying affective state: The greater the match, the larger the brain's intrinsic reward signal. Taken together, these findings provide evidence that reward-related neural activity during social encounters signals how well an individual's "neural vocabulary" is suited to infer another person's affective state, and that this intrinsic reward might be a source of changes in interpersonal attraction.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Intención , Relaciones Interpersonales , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
9.
Front Psychiatry ; 7: 51, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27092085

RESUMEN

Premonitory urges are a cardinal feature in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Severity of premonitory urges can be assessed with the "Premonitory Urge for Tic Disorders Scale" (PUTS). However, convergent validity of the measure has been difficult to assess due to the lack of other urge measures. We investigated the relationship between average real-time urge intensity assessed by an in-house developed real-time urge monitor (RUM), measuring urge intensity continuously for 5 min on a visual analog scale, and general urge intensity assessed by the PUTS in 22 adult Tourette patients (mean age 29.8 ± 10.3 SD, 19 males). Additionally, underlying factors of premonitory urges assessed by the PUTS were investigated in the adult sample using factor analysis and were replicated in 40 children and adolescents diagnosed with Tourette syndrome (mean age 12.05 ± 2.83 SD, 31 males). Cronbach's α for the PUTS 10 was acceptable (α = 0.79) in the adult sample. Convergent validity between average real-time urge intensity scores (as assessed with the RUM) and the 10-item version of the PUTS (r = 0.64) and the 9-item version of the PUTS (r = 0.66) was good. A factor analysis including the 10 items of the PUTS and average real-time urge intensity scores revealed three factors. One factor included the average real-time urge intensity score and appeared to measure urge intensity, whereas the other two factors can be assumed to reflect the (sensory) quality of urges and subjective control, respectively. The factor structure of the 10 PUTS items alone was replicated in a sample of children and adolescents. The results indicate that convergent validity between the PUTS and the real-time urge assessment monitor is good. Furthermore, the results suggest that the PUTS might assess more than one dimension of urges, and it may be worthwhile developing different subscales of the PUTS assessing premonitory urges in terms of intensity and quality, as well as subjectively experienced control over tics and premonitory urges.

10.
Cortex ; 77: 24-37, 2016 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26922503

RESUMEN

Premonitory urges are a cardinal feature in Tourette syndrome and are commonly viewed as the driving force of tics, building up before and subsiding after the execution of tics. Although the urge-tic interplay is one of the most preeminent features in Tourette syndrome, the temporal relationship between tics and urges has never been examined experimentally, mainly due to the lack of an appropriate assessment tool. We investigated the temporal relationship between urge intensity and tics in 17 Tourette patients and between urge intensity and eye blinks in 16 healthy controls in a free ticcing/blinking condition and a tic/blink suppression condition. For this purpose, an urge assessment tool was developed that allows real-time monitoring and quantification of urge intensity. Compared to free ticcing/blinking, urge intensity was higher during the suppression condition in both Tourette patients and healthy controls, while tics and blinks occurred less frequently. The data show that urge intensity increases prior to tics and decreases after tics in a time window of approximately ±10 sec. Tic suppression had a significant effect on the shape of the urge distribution around tics and led to a decrease in the size of the correlation between urge intensity and tics, indicating that tic suppression led to a de-coupling of tics and urges. In healthy controls, urges to blink were highly associated with eye blink execution, albeit in a narrower time frame (∼±5 sec). Blink suppression had a similar effect on the urge distribution associated with eye blinks as tic suppression had on the urge to tic in Tourette patients. These results corroborate the negative reinforcement model, which proposes that tics are associated with a relief in urges, thereby perpetuating ticcing behaviour. This study also documents similarities and differences between urges to act in healthy controls and urges to tic in Tourette syndrome.


Asunto(s)
Parpadeo/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Tics/fisiopatología , Síndrome de Tourette/fisiopatología , Adolescente , Adulto , Conducta/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Refuerzo en Psicología , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Tics/etiología , Síndrome de Tourette/complicaciones , Adulto Joven
11.
Neuroimage ; 104: 189-98, 2015 Jan 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25315788

RESUMEN

Social context plays an important role in human communication. Depending on the nature of the source, the same communication signal might be processed in fundamentally different ways. However, the selective modulation (or "gating") of the flow of neural information during communication is not fully understood. Here, we use multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA) and multivoxel connectivity analysis (MVCA), a novel technique that allows to analyse context-dependent changes of the strength interregional coupling between ensembles of voxels, to examine how the human brain differentially gates content-specific sensory information during ongoing perception of communication signals. In a simulated electronic communication experiment, participants received two alternative text messages during fMRI ("happy" or "sad") which they believed had been sent either by their real-life friend outside the scanner or by a computer. A region in the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) selectively increased its functional coupling with sensory-content encoding regions in the visual cortex when a text message was perceived as being sent by the participant's friend, and decreased its functional coupling with these regions when a text message was perceived as being sent by the computer. Furthermore, the strength of neural encoding of content-specific information of text messages in the dmPFC was modulated by the social tie between the participant and her friend: the more of her spare time a participant reported to spend with her friend the stronger was the neural encoding. This suggests that the human brain selectively gates sensory information into the relevant network for processing the mental states of others, depending on the source of the communication signal.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación , Filtrado Sensorial/fisiología , Conducta Social , Medio Social , Adulto , Emociones , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Amigos/psicología , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
12.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 8: 77, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24578686

RESUMEN

Research on the mechanisms underlying human facial emotion recognition has long focussed on genetically determined neural algorithms and often neglected the question of how these algorithms might be tuned by social learning. Here we show that facial emotion decoding skills can be significantly and sustainably improved by practice without an external teaching signal. Participants saw video clips of dynamic facial expressions of five different women and were asked to decide which of four possible emotions (anger, disgust, fear, and sadness) was shown in each clip. Although no external information about the correctness of the participant's response or the sender's true affective state was provided, participants showed a significant increase of facial emotion recognition accuracy both within and across two training sessions two days to several weeks apart. We discuss several similarities and differences between the unsupervised improvement of facial decoding skills observed in the current study, unsupervised perceptual learning of simple stimuli described in previous studies and practice effects often observed in cognitive tasks.

13.
PLoS One ; 8(8): e69886, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23990890

RESUMEN

Imitation of facial expressions engages the putative human mirror neuron system as well as the insula and the amygdala as part of the limbic system. The specific function of the latter two regions during emotional actions is still under debate. The current study investigated brain responses during imitation of positive in comparison to non-emotional facial expressions. Differences in brain activation of the amygdala and insula were additionally examined during observation and execution of facial expressions. Participants imitated, executed and observed happy and non-emotional facial expressions, as well as neutral faces. During imitation, higher right hemispheric activation emerged in the happy compared to the non-emotional condition in the right anterior insula and the right amygdala, in addition to the pre-supplementary motor area, middle temporal gyrus and the inferior frontal gyrus. Region-of-interest analyses revealed that the right insula was more strongly recruited by (i) imitation and execution than by observation of facial expressions, that (ii) the insula was significantly stronger activated by happy than by non-emotional facial expressions during observation and imitation and that (iii) the activation differences in the right amygdala between happy and non-emotional facial expressions were increased during imitation and execution, in comparison to sole observation. We suggest that the insula and the amygdala contribute specifically to the happy emotional connotation of the facial expressions depending on the task. The pattern of the insula activity might reflect increased bodily awareness during active execution compared to passive observation and during visual processing of the happy compared to non-emotional facial expressions. The activation specific for the happy facial expression of the amygdala during motor tasks, but not in the observation condition, might reflect increased autonomic activity or feedback from facial muscles to the amygdala.


Asunto(s)
Afecto/fisiología , Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Adulto , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Masculino , Neuronas Espejo/fisiología , Red Nerviosa , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
14.
Neural Comput ; 25(6): 1548-84, 2013 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23517095

RESUMEN

By minimizing the zero-norm of the separating hyperplane, the support feature machine (SFM) finds the smallest subspace (the least number of features) of a data set such that within this subspace, two classes are linearly separable without error. This way, the dimensionality of the data is more efficiently reduced than with support vector-based feature selection, which can be shown both theoretically and empirically. In this letter, we first provide a new formulation of the previously introduced concept of the SFM. With this new formulation, classification of unbalanced and nonseparable data is straightforward, which allows using the SFM for feature selection and classification in a large variety of different scenarios. To illustrate how the SFM can be used to identify both the smallest subset of discriminative features and the total number of informative features in biological data sets we apply repetitive feature selection based on the SFM to a functional magnetic resonance imaging data set. We suggest that these capabilities qualify the SFM as a universal method for feature selection, especially for high-dimensional small-sample-size data sets that often occur in biological and medical applications.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/irrigación sanguínea , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Máquina de Vectores de Soporte , Encéfalo/fisiología , Humanos , Oxígeno
15.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 8(4): 370-7, 2013 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22275167

RESUMEN

Previous studies have shown overlapping neural activations for observation and execution or imitation of emotional facial expressions. These shared representations have been assumed to provide indirect evidence for a human mirror neuron system, which is suggested to be a prerequisite of action comprehension. We aimed at clarifying whether shared representations in and beyond human mirror areas are specifically activated by affective facial expressions or whether they are activated by facial expressions independent of the emotional meaning. During neuroimaging, participants observed and executed happy and non-emotional facial expressions. Shared representations were revealed for happy facial expressions in the pars opercularis, the precentral gyrus, in the superior temporal gyrus/medial temporal gyrus (MTG), in the pre-supplementary motor area and in the right amygdala. All areas showed less pronounced activation in the non-emotional condition. When directly compared, significant stronger neural responses emerged for happy facial expressions in the pre-supplementary motor area and in the MTG than for non-emotional stimuli. We assume that activation of shared representations depends on the affect and (social) relevance of the facial expression. The pre-supplementary motor area is a core-shared representation-structure supporting observation and execution of affective contagious facial expressions and might have a modulatory role during the preparation of executing happy facial expressions.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Emociones/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Encéfalo/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Femenino , Felicidad , Humanos , Masculino , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Adulto Joven
16.
PLoS One ; 8(12): e82756, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24386113

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The goal of this study was to test whether central mechanisms of scratching-induced itch attenuation can be activated by scratching the limb contralateral to the itching limb when the participant is made to visually perceive the non-itching limb as the itching limb by means of mirror images. METHODS: Healthy participants were asked to assess the intensity of an experimentally induced itch at their right forearm while they observed externally guided scratch movements either at their right (itching) or left (non-itching) forearm which were either mirrored or not mirrored. In the first experiment, a mirror placed between the participant's forearms was used to create the visual illusion that the participant's itching (right) forearm was being scratched while in fact the non-itching (left) forearm was scratched. To control visibility of the left (non-mirrored) forearm, a second experiment was performed in which unflipped and flipped real-time video displays of the participant's forearms were used to create experimental conditions in which the participant visually perceived scratching either on one forearm only, on both forearms, or no scratching at all. RESULTS: In both experiments, scratching the non-itching limb attenuated perceived itch intensity significantly and selectively in the mirror condition, i.e., when the non-itching forearm was visually perceived as the itching limb. DISCUSSION: These data provide evidence that the visual illusion that an itching limb is being scratched while in fact the non-itching limb contralateral to the itching limb is scratched, can lead to significant itch relief. This effect might be due to a transient illusionary intersensory perceptual congruency of visual, tactile and pruriceptive signals. "Mirror scratching" might provide an alternative treatment to reduce itch perception in focal skin diseases with persistent pruritus without causing additional harm to the affected skin and might therefore have significant clinical impact.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Prurito/psicología , Percepción Visual , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino
17.
Brain ; 135(Pt 4): 1128-40, 2012 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22434215

RESUMEN

Patients with Parkinson's disease suffer from significant motor impairments and accompanying cognitive and affective dysfunction due to progressive disturbances of basal ganglia-cortical gating loops. Parkinson's disease has a long presymptomatic stage, which indicates a substantial capacity of the human brain to compensate for dopaminergic nerve degeneration before clinical manifestation of the disease. Neuroimaging studies provide evidence that increased motor-related cortical activity can compensate for progressive dopaminergic nerve degeneration in carriers of a single mutant Parkin or PINK1 gene, who show a mild but significant reduction of dopamine metabolism in the basal ganglia in the complete absence of clinical motor signs. However, it is currently unknown whether similar compensatory mechanisms are effective in non-motor basal ganglia-cortical gating loops. Here, we ask whether asymptomatic Parkin mutation carriers show altered patterns of brain activity during processing of facial gestures, and whether this might compensate for latent facial emotion recognition deficits. Current theories in social neuroscience assume that execution and perception of facial gestures are linked by a special class of visuomotor neurons ('mirror neurons') in the ventrolateral premotor cortex/pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann area 44/6). We hypothesized that asymptomatic Parkin mutation carriers would show increased activity in this area during processing of affective facial gestures, replicating the compensatory motor effects that have previously been observed in these individuals. Additionally, Parkin mutation carriers might show altered activity in other basal ganglia-cortical gating loops. Eight asymptomatic heterozygous Parkin mutation carriers and eight matched controls underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging and a subsequent facial emotion recognition task. As predicted, Parkin mutation carriers showed significantly stronger activity in the right ventrolateral premotor cortex during execution and perception of affective facial gestures than healthy controls. Furthermore, Parkin mutation carriers showed a slightly reduced ability to recognize facial emotions that was least severe in individuals who showed the strongest increase of ventrolateral premotor activity. In addition, Parkin mutation carriers showed a significantly weaker than normal increase of activity in the left lateral orbitofrontal cortex (inferior frontal gyrus pars orbitalis, Brodmann area 47), which was unrelated to facial emotion recognition ability. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that compensatory activity in the ventrolateral premotor cortex during processing of affective facial gestures can reduce impairments in facial emotion recognition in subclinical Parkin mutation carriers. A breakdown of this compensatory mechanism might lead to the impairment of facial expressivity and facial emotion recognition observed in manifest Parkinson's disease.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Expresión Facial , Mutación/genética , Enfermedad de Parkinson/genética , Enfermedad de Parkinson/fisiopatología , Ubiquitina-Proteína Ligasas/genética , Adulto , Alelos , Análisis de Varianza , Encéfalo/irrigación sanguínea , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Función Ejecutiva , Salud de la Familia , Femenino , Fluorodesoxiglucosa F18/farmacocinética , Lateralidad Funcional/genética , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Levodopa/farmacocinética , Modelos Lineales , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Oxígeno , Enfermedad de Parkinson/diagnóstico por imagen , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Estimulación Luminosa , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Autoinforme , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad
18.
Cogn Emot ; 26(2): 252-60, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21973031

RESUMEN

Theories of emotion propose that responses to emotional pictures can occur independently of whether or not people are aware of the picture content. Because evidence from dissociation paradigms is inconclusive, we manipulated picture awareness gradually and studied whether emotional responses varied with degree of awareness. Spider fearful and non-fearful participants viewed pictures of spiders and flowers at four levels of backward masking while electrodermal activity and heart rate were measured continuously. Recognition ratings confirmed that participants' picture awareness decreased with masking. Critically, effects of spider fear on emotion ratings and heart rate also decreased with masking. These findings suggest that effects of spider fear on emotion ratings and heart rate are closely related to picture awareness.


Asunto(s)
Concienciación/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Trastornos Fóbicos/fisiopatología , Trastornos Fóbicos/psicología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Respuesta Galvánica de la Piel/fisiología , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
19.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 33(10): 2464-76, 2012 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21998031

RESUMEN

We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate brain processes underlying control of emotional responses towards a person in distress by cognitive social distance modulation. fMRI and peripheral physiological responses (startle response and electrodermal activity) were recorded from 24 women while they watched victim-offender scenes and modulated their social distance to the victim by cognitive reappraisal. We found that emotional responses, including startle eyeblink and amygdala responses, can effectively be modulated by social distance modulation. Furthermore, our data provide evidence that activity in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) and the anterior paracingulate cortex (aPCC), two brain regions that have previously been associated with brain processes related to distant and close others, is differentially modulated by intentional social distance modulation: activity in the dmPFC increased with increasing disengagement from the victim and activity in the aPCC increased with increasing engagement with the victim. We suggest that these two regions play opposing roles in cognitive modulation of social distance and affective responses towards persons in distress that enable the adaptive and flexible social behavior observed in humans.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Empatía/fisiología , Distancia Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Víctimas de Crimen , Femenino , Humanos , Interpretación de Imagen Asistida por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Adulto Joven
20.
Neuroimage ; 54(1): 439-46, 2011 Jan 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20624471

RESUMEN

When people interact, affective information is transmitted between their brains. Modern imaging techniques permit to investigate the dynamics of this brain-to-brain transfer of information. Here, we used information-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate the flow of affective information between the brains of senders and perceivers engaged in ongoing facial communication of affect. We found that the level of neural activity within a distributed network of the perceiver's brain can be successfully predicted from the neural activity in the same network in the sender's brain, depending on the affect that is currently being communicated. Furthermore, there was a temporal succession in the flow of affective information from the sender's brain to the perceiver's brain, with information in the perceiver's brain being significantly delayed relative to information in the sender's brain. This delay decreased over time, possibly reflecting some 'tuning in' of the perceiver with the sender. Our data support current theories of intersubjectivity by providing direct evidence that during ongoing facial communication a 'shared space' of affect is successively built up between senders and perceivers of affective facial signals.


Asunto(s)
Afecto/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Anciano , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Conductividad Eléctrica , Emociones/fisiología , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Percepción/fisiología , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Fenómenos Fisiológicos de la Piel
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...