Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 10 de 10
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Cogn Process ; 14(1): 89-98, 2013 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23011642

RESUMEN

Recently, studies have demonstrated that negative valence reduces the magnitude of the belief-bias effect in syllogistic reasoning. This effect has been localized in the reasoning stage, in the form of increased deliberation on trials where validity and conclusion believability are incongruent. Here, using signal detection theory, we show that the attenuation of belief bias observed when valence was negative can also be evoked by a liberal response bias at the decision stage. Indeed, when valence was negative participants adopted a more liberal criterion for judging syllogisms as "valid," and were overconfident in their judgments. They also displayed less sensitivity in distinguishing between valid and invalid syllogisms. Our findings dovetail with recent evidence from memory research suggesting that negative valence can evoke a liberal response bias without improving performance. Our novel contribution is the demonstration that the attenuating effect of negative valence on belief bias can take multiples routes--by influencing the decision stage as was the case here, the reasoning stage as has been demonstrated elsewhere, and potentially both stages.


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Emociones/fisiología , Lógica , Pensamiento/fisiología , Adulto , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio/fisiología , Masculino , Solución de Problemas/fisiología , Pruebas Psicológicas , Detección de Señal Psicológica
2.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 130(2): 103-14, 2009 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19081556

RESUMEN

In both a perceptual and a general knowledge comparison task, participants categorized the time they took to decide, selecting one of six categories ordered from "Slow" to Fast". Subsequently, they rated confidence on a six-category scale ranging from "50%" to "100%". Participants were able to accurately scale their response times thus enabling the treatment of the response time (RT) categories as potential confidence categories. Probability assessment analyses of RTs revealed indices of over/underconfidence, calibration, and resolution, each subject to the "hard-easy" effect, comparable to those obtained with the actual confidence ratings. However, in both the perceptual and knowledge domains, resolution (i.e., the ability to use the confidence categories to distinguish correct from incorrect decisions) was significantly better with confidence ratings than with RT categorization. Generally, comparable results were obtained with scaling of the objective RTs, although subjective categorization of RTs provided probability assessment indices superior to those obtained from objective RTs. Taken together, the findings do not support the view that confidence arises from a scaling of decision time.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Autoevaluación (Psicología) , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Cognición/fisiología , Intervalos de Confianza , Discriminación en Psicología , Humanos , Probabilidad , Estudiantes , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
3.
Hum Factors ; 49(4): 646-60, 2007 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17702216

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To examine the effects of 30 hr of sleep loss and continuous cognitive work on performance in a distributed team decision-making environment. BACKGROUND: To date, only a few studies have examined the effect of sleep loss on distributed team performance, and only one other to our knowledge has examined the relationship between sleep loss and social-motivational aspects of teams (Hoeksema-van Orden, Gaillard, & Buunk, 1998). METHOD: Sixteen teams participated; each comprised 4 members. Three team members made threat assessments on a military surveillance task and then forwarded their judgments electronically to a team leader, who made a final assessment on behalf of the team. RESULTS: Sleep loss had an antagonistic effect on team decision-making accuracy and decision time. However, the performance loss associated with fatigue attributable to sleep loss was mediated by being part of a team, as compared with performing the same task individually - that is, we found evidence of a "motivational gain" effect in these sleepy teams. We compare these results with those of Hoeksema-van Orden et al. (1998), who found clear evidence of a "social loafing" effect in sleepy teams. CONCLUSION: The divergent results are discussed in the context of the collective effort model (Karau & Williams, 1993) and are attributable in part to a difference between independent and interdependent team tasks. APPLICATION: The issues and findings have implications for a wide range of distributed, collaborative work environments, such as military network-enabled operations.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Procesos de Grupo , Motivación , Privación de Sueño/psicología , Adulto , Canadá , Humanos , Personal Militar , Privación de Sueño/fisiopatología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
4.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 13(4): 182-96, 2007 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18194046

RESUMEN

Sixty-four adults participated in a study examining the accuracy of metacognitive judgments during 28 hr of sleep deprivation (SD) and continuous cognitive work. Three tasks were studied (perceptual comparison, general knowledge, and mental addition), collectively spanning a range of cognitive abilities and levels of susceptibility to SD. Subjective and objective measures of sleepiness confirmed the expected patterns of increasing fatigue with SD. Participants displayed differing levels of metacognitive abilities across tasks, but traditional indices of the confidence-accuracy relation (i.e., calibration, resolution, over- and underconfidence), as well as the accuracy of pre- and posttask estimates of performance, remained stable over the SD period. The findings suggest that people can accurately assess their own cognitive performance when deprived of 1 night of sleep and that this ability need not be based on subjective estimates of sleepiness. The implications and limitations of the study are discussed and directions for future research are proposed.


Asunto(s)
Cultura , Fatiga/epidemiología , Juicio , Modelos Psicológicos , Trastornos del Inicio y del Mantenimiento del Sueño/epidemiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
5.
Aviat Space Environ Med ; 75(9): 764-70, 2004 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15460627

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Elevated hearing thresholds have been documented in some astronauts after long-term spaceflights although noise levels were lower than those normally associated with noise-induced hearing loss in ground-based operations. The present study was conducted to determine whether prolonged exposure (70 h) to levels (72 dBA) recorded on the International Space Station (ISS) service module would impact diverse measures of auditory function, as well as cognition and memory, motivation, and cardiovascular function. METHOD: Five mixed gender subgroups of five normal-hearing subjects, aged 20-50 yr, were sequestered for 70 h in an environment that modeled conditions on the ISS. They were assigned to one of three background conditions: quiet (n = 5), continuous noise from the ISS service module (n = 10), or continuous noise during the day only (n = 10). Subjects were tested repeatedly within and across days as individuals or pair mates. RESULTS: There were no negative effects of the noise on any of the outcome measures. Introduction of a delay or noise in a communication channel used in the assessment of speech communicability significantly affected the time taken for joint problem solving by partners. DISCUSSION: The results of this study were not consistent with the observation of hearing loss measured after spaceflights. Nor were changes evident in cognition, motivation, or cardiovascular function. Factors which might account for the discrepancy are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Umbral Auditivo/fisiología , Audición/fisiología , Ruido , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Vuelo Espacial , Adulto , Cámaras de Exposición Atmosférica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicoacústica , Simulación del Espacio , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Factores de Tiempo
6.
Percept Psychophys ; 66(3): 430-45, 2004 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15283068

RESUMEN

In the first phase of each of two experiments, participants learned to associate a set of labels (i.e., consonant-vowel-consonant [CVC]) with a set of line lengths by using a paired-associate learning procedure. In the second phase of each experiment, these learned labels were used as memorial standards in the method of constant stimuli. Psychometric functions and the associated indices of discriminative performance (i.e., Weber fractions [WFs], just noticeable difference, and point of subjective equality) were then obtained for the remembered standards. In Experiment 1, WFs (i.e., the indices of memory precision) obtained with remembered standards were found to be higher (i.e., had poorer discriminability) than were WFs obtained with perceptual standards. In addition, WFs obtained with the remembered standards exhibited serial position effects (i.e., poorer discriminability for central items in the memory ensemble) and systematically varied with set size (i.e., the number of standards in the memory set), but WFs obtained with perceptual standards did not depend on serial position or set size. In Experiment 2, increasing the number of acquisition trials reduced WFs and diminished serial position effects. In addition, WFs did not vary systematically with the "physical" spacing between the standards in memory, but they did with the ordinal spacing. The results are consistent with a noisy analogue representation of remembered magnitudes, whereby central items in a memory ensemble are subject to lateral inhibition and thus reduced discriminability. Finally, presentation order effects, as defined by the classic time-order error, were observed with purely perceptual comparisons but not with comparisons involving a remembered standard. This latter finding is inconsistent with a strong form of the functional equivalence view of perception and memory.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Memoria , Psicofísica/métodos , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Distribución Aleatoria
7.
Hum Psychopharmacol ; 19(5): 323-32, 2004 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15252824

RESUMEN

The stimulant modafinil has proved to be an effective treatment modality for narcolepsy and related sleep disorders and is also being studied for use during sustained military operations to ameliorate the effects of fatigue due to sleep loss. However, a previous study reported that a relatively large, single dose of modafinil (300 mg), administered to already sleep-deprived individuals, caused participants to overestimate their cognitive abilities (i.e. 'overconfidence'). Because the predominant application of modafinil is in otherwise healthy, non-sleep-deprived individuals, the present study investigated the generality of modafinil-induced overconfidence in a group of 18 healthy, non sleep-deprived adults. The design involved a double-blind, placebo controlled, fully within-subjects manipulation of placebo and modafinil (4 mg/kg: approximately 300 mg, on average) over three 50-min cognitive testing sessions (i.e. before drug ingestion, and at 90 and 180 min after drug ingestion). The cognitive task battery included subjective assessments of mood, fatigue, affect, vigor and motivation, and cognitive assessments of serial reaction time, logical reasoning, visual comparison, mental addition and vigilance. In addition, trial-by-trial confidence judgements were obtained for two of the cognitive tasks and more global, task level assessments of performance were obtained for four of the cognitive tasks. Relative to placebo, modafinil improved fatigue levels, motivation, reaction time and vigilance. In terms of self-assessments of cognitive performance, both the placebo and modafinil conditions were 'well calibrated' on trial-by-trial confidence judgements, showing neither marked over- nor under-confidence. Of note, the modafinil condition displayed a non-significant tendency towards 'overconfidence' for task-level assessments of performance. The present findings highlight the need for continued research on the many complex interactions involving fatigue states, occasional versus long-term stimulant use, and subjective assessments of fatigue and cognitive performance.


Asunto(s)
Compuestos de Bencidrilo/farmacología , Estimulantes del Sistema Nervioso Central/farmacología , Cognición/efectos de los fármacos , Adolescente , Adulto , Método Doble Ciego , Fatiga/psicología , Humanos , Masculino , Modafinilo , Tiempo de Reacción/efectos de los fármacos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Percepción Visual/efectos de los fármacos
8.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 29(3): 658-74, 2003 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12848332

RESUMEN

Adaptive decision processes were investigated in experiments involving an unexpected change in the global ease or difficulty of the task. Under accuracy stress, a shift from an easy to a difficult context induced a marked increase in decision time, but a shift from a difficult to an easy context did not. Under speed stress, a shift to a more difficult context induced lower accuracy and rated confidence, depending on the difficulty of the decisions. A view of caution developed in D. Vickers's (1979) accumulator theory--whereby one seeks to base decisions on more information--is compared with a view based on slow and fast guessing theory (W. M. Petrusic, 1992; W. M. Petrusic & J. V. Baranski, 1989a)--whereby one seeks to base decisions on more diagnostic information. On balance, the findings support the latter view.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Atención , Toma de Decisiones , Percepción Visual , Distribución Aleatoria
9.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 10(1): 177-83, 2003 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12747505

RESUMEN

Current theories of confidence in human judgment assume that confidence and the decision it is based on are inextricably tied to the same process (decisional locus theories) or that confidence processing begins only once the primary decision has been completed (postdecisional locus theories). In the absence of auxiliary assumptions, however, neither class of theory permits the judgment of confidence to affect primary decision processing. In the present study, we examined the effect of rendering confidence judgments on the properties of the decision process in a sensory discrimination task. An examination of the properties of the time taken to determine confidence (i.e., the time taken to render the judgment of confidence) revealed clear evidence of postdecisional confidence processing. Concomitantly, the requirement of confidence judgments was found to substantially increase decisional response times, suggesting that some confidence processing occurs during the primary decision process. We discuss the implications of these findings for contemporary models of confidence in human judgment.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Técnicas de Apoyo para la Decisión , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Juicio , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Aprendizaje por Probabilidad , Percepción del Tamaño , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Intervalos de Confianza , Umbral Diferencial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Disposición en Psicología , Detección de Señal Psicológica
10.
Aviat Space Environ Med ; 73(11): 1079-88, 2002 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12433231

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Previous studies have revealed that modafinil elevates resting core temperature during periods of sustained wakefulness. The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of modafinil on core temperature during rest and exercise throughout 40 h of sustained wakefulness in a warm environment. METHODS: Ten males performed a drug session (three 100 mg doses per day) and a placebo session that involved a control day, 40 h of sustained wakefulness, and a recovery sleep. For 38 h of the sustained wakefulness, subjects were exposed to 30 degrees C with 50% relative humidity. During the afternoon of both days of wakefulness and during the early morning of the second day, subjects performed 2 h of exercise at 60% VO2max while exposed to the warm environment. RESULTS: The data revealed that rectal temperature (Tre) was elevated at rest 0.15-0.2 degrees C following modafinil ingestion throughout the period of sustained wakefulness. This increase in body temperature at rest was due to an increase in heat production during the first day of wakefulness followed by a lower evaporative heat loss during the second day. During exercise, an inconsistent effect of the drug on Tre was observed throughout the 38-h period. On the first afternoon, the impact of modafinil on Tre was no longer evident after 20 min of exercise. In contrast, during the early morning and afternoon of the second day, the effects of the drug on Tre at rest remained during exercise. For seven subjects who had Tre data for 80 min during all exercise periods, Tre during the placebo session was 38.9, 38.4, and 38.7 degrees C after 80 min of exercise for periods one, two and three, respectively, whereas the corresponding values during the modafinil session were 38.8, 38.7, and 38.9 degrees C. CONCLUSION: With a greater cumulative dose of the drug, Tre remained elevated throughout the exercise period to an extent similar to the increase observed under resting conditions when compared with the placebo condition.


Asunto(s)
Compuestos de Bencidrilo/farmacología , Temperatura Corporal/efectos de los fármacos , Estimulantes del Sistema Nervioso Central/farmacología , Ejercicio Físico/fisiología , Calor , Vigilia/fisiología , Adulto , Compuestos de Bencidrilo/sangre , Estimulantes del Sistema Nervioso Central/sangre , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Esquema de Medicación , Ambiente , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Humanos , Humedad , Masculino , Modafinilo , Recto , Descanso/fisiología , Privación de Sueño/fisiopatología , Factores de Tiempo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...