Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 20
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Front Psychol ; 13: 951057, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36160576

RESUMEN

Evidence from adult studies of deaf signers supports the dissociation between neural systems involved in processing visual linguistic and non-linguistic body actions. The question of how and when this specialization arises is poorly understood. Visual attention to these forms is likely to change with age and be affected by prior language experience. The present study used eye-tracking methodology with infants and children as they freely viewed alternating video sequences of lexical American sign language (ASL) signs and non-linguistic body actions (self-directed grooming action and object-directed pantomime). In Experiment 1, we quantified fixation patterns using an area of interest (AOI) approach and calculated face preference index (FPI) values to assess the developmental differences between 6 and 11-month-old hearing infants. Both groups were from monolingual English-speaking homes with no prior exposure to sign language. Six-month-olds attended the signer's face for grooming; but for mimes and signs, they were drawn to attend to the "articulatory space" where the hands and arms primarily fall. Eleven-month-olds, on the other hand, showed a similar attention to the face for all body action types. We interpret this to reflect an early visual language sensitivity that diminishes with age, just before the child's first birthday. In Experiment 2, we contrasted 18 hearing monolingual English-speaking children (mean age of 4.8 years) vs. 13 hearing children of deaf adults (CODAs; mean age of 5.7 years) whose primary language at home was ASL. Native signing children had a significantly greater face attentional bias than non-signing children for ASL signs, but not for grooming and mimes. The differences in the visual attention patterns that are contingent on age (in infants) and language experience (in children) may be related to both linguistic specialization over time and the emerging awareness of communicative gestural acts.

2.
Cognition ; 212: 104659, 2021 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33798950

RESUMEN

The well-known Stroop interference effect has been instrumental in revealing the highly automated nature of lexical processing as well as providing new insights to the underlying lexical organization of first and second languages within proficient bilinguals. The present cross-linguistic study had two goals: 1) to examine Stroop interference for dynamic signs and printed words in deaf ASL-English bilinguals who report no reliance on speech or audiological aids; 2) to compare Stroop interference effects in several groups of bilinguals whose two languages range from very distinct to very similar in their shared orthographic patterns: ASL-English bilinguals (very distinct), Chinese-English bilinguals (low similarity), Korean-English bilinguals (moderate similarity), and Spanish-English bilinguals (high similarity). Reaction time and accuracy were measured for the Stroop color naming and word reading tasks, for congruent and incongruent color font conditions. Results confirmed strong Stroop interference for both dynamic ASL stimuli and English printed words in deaf bilinguals, with stronger Stroop interference effects in ASL for deaf bilinguals who scored higher in a direct assessment of ASL proficiency. Comparison of the four groups of bilinguals revealed that the same-script bilinguals (Spanish-English bilinguals) exhibited significantly greater Stroop interference effects for color naming than the other three bilingual groups. The results support three conclusions. First, Stroop interference effects are found for both signed and spoken languages. Second, contrary to some claims in the literature about deaf signers who do not use speech being poor readers, deaf bilinguals' lexical processing of both signs and written words is highly automated. Third, cross-language similarity is a critical factor shaping bilinguals' experience of Stroop interference in their two languages. This study represents the first comparison of both deaf and hearing bilinguals on the Stroop task, offering a critical test of theories about bilingual lexical access and cognitive control.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Multilingüismo , Audición , Humanos , Lingüística , Test de Stroop
3.
Dev Sci ; 24(4): e13086, 2021 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33484575

RESUMEN

Children's gaze behavior reflects emergent linguistic knowledge and real-time language processing of speech, but little is known about naturalistic gaze behaviors while watching signed narratives. Measuring gaze patterns in signing children could uncover how they master perceptual gaze control during a time of active language learning. Gaze patterns were recorded using a Tobii X120 eye tracker, in 31 non-signing and 30 signing hearing infants (5-14 months) and children (2-8 years) as they watched signed narratives on video. Intelligibility of the signed narratives was manipulated by presenting them naturally and in video-reversed ("low intelligibility") conditions. This video manipulation was used because it distorts semantic content, while preserving most surface phonological features. We examined where participants looked, using linear mixed models with Language Group (non-signing vs. signing) and Video Condition (Forward vs. Reversed), controlling for trial order. Non-signing infants and children showed a preference to look at the face as well as areas below the face, possibly because their gaze was drawn to the moving articulators in signing space. Native signing infants and children demonstrated resilient, face-focused gaze behavior. Moreover, their gaze behavior was unchanged for video-reversed signed narratives, similar to what was seen for adult native signers, possibly because they already have efficient highly focused gaze behavior. The present study demonstrates that human perceptual gaze control is sensitive to visual language experience over the first year of life and emerges early, by 6 months of age. Results have implications for the critical importance of early visual language exposure for deaf infants. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=2ahWUluFAAg.


Asunto(s)
Sordera , Lengua de Signos , Niño , Preescolar , Audición , Humanos , Lactante , Lenguaje , Semántica , Habla
4.
Vision Res ; 164: 34-43, 2019 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31557606

RESUMEN

Careful measurements of the temporal dynamics of speech have provided important insights into phonetic properties of spoken languages, which are important for understanding auditory perception. By contrast, analytic quantification of the visual properties of signed languages is still largely uncharted. Exposure to sign language is a unique experience that could shape and modify low-level visual processing for those who use it regularly (i.e., what we refer to as the Enhanced Exposure Hypothesis). The purpose of the current study was to characterize the visual spatiotemporal properties of American Sign Language (ASL) so that future studies can test the enhanced exposure hypothesis in signers, with the prediction that altered vision should be observed within, more so than outside, the range of properties found in ASL. Using an ultrasonic motion tracking system, we recorded the hand position in 3-dimensional space over time during sign language production of signs, sentences, and narratives. From these data, we calculated several metrics: hand position and eccentricity in space and hand motion speed. For individual signs, we also measured total distance travelled by the dominant hand and total duration of each sign. These metrics were found to fall within a selective range, suggesting that exposure to signs is a specific and unique visual experience, which might alter visual perceptual abilities in signers for visual information within the experienced range, even for non-language stimuli.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Lenguaje , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Fonética , Lengua de Signos , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Humanos , Adulto Joven
5.
J Vis Exp ; (147)2019 05 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31157775

RESUMEN

We discuss the use of the preferential looking paradigm in eye tracking studies in order to study how infants develop, understand, and attend to the world around them. Eye tracking is a safe and non-invasive way to collect gaze data from infants, and the preferential looking paradigm is simple to design and only requires the infant to be attending to the screen. By simultaneously showing two visual stimuli that differ in one dimension, we can assess whether infants show different looking behavior for either stimulus, thus demonstrating sensitivity to that difference. The challenges in such experimental approaches are that experiments must be kept brief (no more than 10 min) and be carefully controlled such that the two stimuli differ in only one way. The interpretation of null results must also be carefully considered. In this paper, we illustrate a successful example of an infant eye tracking study with a preferential looking paradigm to discover that 6-month-olds are sensitive to linguistic cues in a signed language despite having no prior exposure to signed language, suggesting that infants possess intrinsic or innate sensitivities to these cues.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Lenguaje , Visión Ocular , Calibración , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa
6.
J Vis ; 13(13): 8, 2013 Nov 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24198399

RESUMEN

Although global motion processing is thought to emerge early in infancy, there is debate regarding the age at which it matures to an adult-like level. In the current study, we address the possibility that the apparent age-related improvement in global motion processing might be secondary to age-related increases in the sensitivity of mechanisms (i.e., local motion detectors) that provide input to global motion mechanisms. To address this, we measured global motion processing by obtaining motion coherence thresholds using stimuli that were equally detectable in terms of contrast across all individuals and ages (3-, 4-, 5-, 6-, and 7-month-olds and adults). For infants, we employed a directional eye movement (DEM) technique. For adults, we employed both DEM and a self-report method. First, contrast sensitivity was obtained for a local task, using a stochastic motion display in which all the dots moved coherently. Contrast sensitivity increased significantly between 3 and 7 months, and between infancy and adulthood. Each subject was then tested on the global motion task with the contrast of the dots set to 2.5 × each individual's contrast threshold. Coherence thresholds were obtained by varying the percentage of coherently moving "signal" versus "noise" dots in the stochastic motion display. Results revealed remarkably stable global motion sensitivity between 3 and 7 months of age, as well as between infancy and adulthood. These results suggest that the mechanisms underlying global motion processing develop to an adult-like state very quickly.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Umbral Sensorial , Adulto Joven
7.
Doc Ophthalmol ; 127(1): 57-68, 2013 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23744448

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The current study assessed whether contrast sensitivity is affected in preterm infants with a history of spontaneously regressed retinopathy of prematurity (ROP, Stages 1-3). Specifically, we employed luminance (light/dark) and chromatic (red/green) stimuli, which are mediated by the magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) subcortical pathways, respectively. METHODS: Contrast sensitivity (CS) was measured using forced-choice preferential looking testing in 21 infants with a history of ROP and 41 control preterm infants who were born prematurely but did not develop ROP, tested between 8 and 47 weeks (2-11 months) postterm age. Infants were presented with chromatic and luminance drifting sinusoidal gratings, which appeared randomly on the left or right side of the monitor in each trial. The contrast of the stimuli varied across trials and was defined in terms of root mean squared cone contrast for long- and medium-wavelength cones. RESULTS: Between 8 and 25 weeks postterm, ROP infants had significantly worse CS, and there was a trend for greater impairment for luminance than chromatic CS. This delay was not seen at older ages between 26 and 47 weeks postterm. CONCLUSIONS: These findings are consistent with the concept that early maturation of the M pathway is vulnerable to biological insult, as in the case of ROP, to a greater extent than in the P pathway.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Retinopatía de la Prematuridad/fisiopatología , Adaptación a la Oscuridad/fisiología , Edad Gestacional , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Recien Nacido Prematuro , Estimulación Luminosa , Remisión Espontánea
8.
Brain Cogn ; 82(1): 117-26, 2013 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23455437

RESUMEN

Previous studies have asked whether visual sensitivity and attentional processing in deaf signers are enhanced or altered as a result of their different sensory experiences during development, i.e., auditory deprivation and exposure to a visual language. In particular, deaf and hearing signers have been shown to exhibit a right visual field/left hemisphere advantage for motion processing, while hearing nonsigners do not. To examine whether this finding extends to other aspects of visual processing, we compared deaf signers and hearing nonsigners on motion, form, and brightness discrimination tasks. Secondly, to examine whether hemispheric lateralities are affected by attention, we employed a dual-task paradigm to measure form and motion thresholds under "full" vs. "poor" attention conditions. Deaf signers, but not hearing nonsigners, exhibited a right visual field advantage for motion processing. This effect was also seen for form processing and not for the brightness task. Moreover, no group differences were observed in attentional effects, and the motion and form visual field asymmetries were not modulated by attention, suggesting they occur at early levels of sensory processing. In sum, the results show that processing of motion and form, believed to be mediated by dorsal and ventral visual pathways, respectively, are left-hemisphere dominant in deaf signers.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Sordera/fisiopatología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Personas con Deficiencia Auditiva/psicología , Estimulación Luminosa , Lengua de Signos
9.
Vision Res ; 82: 31-41, 2013 Apr 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23485427

RESUMEN

In order to investigate the effects of visual experience on early visual development, the current study compared contrast sensitivity across infants born with different degrees of moderate-to-late prematurity. Here the logic is that at any given postterm age, the most premature infants will have the oldest postnatal age. Given that postnatal age is a proxy for visual experience, the visual experience hypothesis predicts that infants who are more premature, yet healthy, should have higher sensitivity. Luminance (light/dark) and chromatic (red/green) contrast sensitivities (CS) were measured in 236 healthy infants (born -10 to +2 weeks relative to due date) between 5 and 32 weeks postterm age from due date and 8-38 weeks postnatal from birth date. For chromatic CS, we found clear evidence that infants who were most premature within our sample had the highest sensitivity. Specifically, 4-10 additional weeks of visual experience, by virtue of being born early, enhanced chromatic CS. For luminance CS, similar but weaker results were seen. Here, only infants with an additional 6-10 weeks of visual experience, and only at later age points in development, showed enhanced sensitivity. However, CS in preterm infants was still below that of fullterm infants with equivalent postnatal age. In sum, these results suggest that chromatic CS is influenced more by prematurity (and possibly visual experience) than luminance CS, which has implications for differential development of parvocellular and magnocellular pathways.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Recien Nacido Prematuro/fisiología , Nacimiento Prematuro/fisiopatología , Preescolar , Femenino , Edad Gestacional , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Análisis de Regresión
10.
Vision Res ; 52(1): 11-9, 2012 Jan 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22051893

RESUMEN

In order to investigate differences in the effects of spatial attention between the left visual field (LVF) and the right visual field (RVF), we employed a full/poor attention paradigm using stimuli presented in the LVF vs. RVF. In addition, to investigate differences in the effects of spatial attention between the dorsal and ventral processing streams, we obtained motion thresholds (motion coherence thresholds and fine direction discrimination thresholds) and orientation thresholds, respectively. The results of this study showed negligible effects of attention on the orientation task, in either the LVF or RVF. In contrast, for both motion tasks, there was a significant effect of attention in the LVF, but not in the RVF. These data provide psychophysical evidence for greater effects of spatial attention in the LVF/right hemisphere, specifically, for motion processing in the dorsal stream.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Predominio Ocular/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación/fisiología , Psicometría , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Adulto Joven
11.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 36(6): 1573-81, 2010 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20919784

RESUMEN

Iconicity is a property that pervades the lexicon of many sign languages, including American Sign Language (ASL). Iconic signs exhibit a motivated, nonarbitrary mapping between the form of the sign and its meaning. We investigated whether iconicity enhances semantic priming effects for ASL and whether iconic signs are recognized more quickly than noniconic signs are (controlling for strength of iconicity, semantic relatedness, familiarity, and imageability). Twenty deaf signers made lexical decisions to the 2nd item of a prime-target pair. Iconic target signs were preceded by prime signs that were (a) iconic and semantically related, (b) noniconic and semantically related, or (c) semantically unrelated. In addition, a set of noniconic target signs was preceded by semantically unrelated primes. Significant facilitation was observed for target signs when they were preceded by semantically related primes. However, iconicity did not increase the priming effect (e.g., the target sign PIANO was primed equally by the iconic sign GUITAR and the noniconic sign MUSIC). In addition, iconic signs were not recognized faster or more accurately than were noniconic signs. These results confirm the existence of semantic priming for sign language and suggest that iconicity does not play a robust role in online lexical processing.


Asunto(s)
Formación de Concepto/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Semántica , Lengua de Signos , Adulto , Comprensión , Sordera/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Conducta Imitativa , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Estados Unidos , Adulto Joven
12.
J Vis ; 9(10): 19.1-21, 2009 Sep 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19810800

RESUMEN

To investigate effects of visual experience versus preprogrammed mechanisms on visual development, we used multiple regression analysis to determine the extent to which a variety of variables (that differ in the extent to which they are tied to visual experience) predict luminance and chromatic (red/green) contrast sensitivity (CS), which are mediated by the magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) subcortical pathways, respectively. Our variables included gestational length (GL), birth weight (BW), gender, postnatal age (PNA), and birth order (BO). Two-month-olds (n = 60) and 6-month-olds (n = 122) were tested. Results revealed that (1) at 2 months, infants with longer GL have higher luminance CS; (2) at both ages, CS significantly increases over a approximately 21-day range of PNA, but this effect is stronger in 2- than 6-month-olds and stronger for chromatic than luminance CS; (3) at 2 months, boys have higher luminance CS than girls; and (4) at 2 months, firstborn infants have higher CS, while at 6 months, non-firstborn infants have higher CS. The results for PNA/GL are consistent with the possibility that P pathway development is more influenced by variables tied to visual experience (PNA), while M pathway development is more influenced by variables unrelated to visual experience (GL). Other variables, including prenatal environment, are also discussed.


Asunto(s)
Factores de Edad , Orden de Nacimiento , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Identidad de Género , Edad Gestacional , Análisis de Varianza , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Luz , Masculino , Análisis de Regresión
13.
J Vis ; 9(13): 15.1-16, 2009 Dec 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20055548

RESUMEN

In order to investigate the contributions of visual experience vs. preprogrammed mechanisms on visual development, the current study compared contrast sensitivity in preterm vs. fullterm infants. If development is tied to time since conception, preterm infants should match the developmental trajectories of fullterm infants when plotted in postterm age. By contrast, if development is influenced by visual experience, preterm and fullterm infants should match when plotted in postnatal age. Luminance (light/dark) and chromatic (red/green) contrast sensitivities (CS) were measured in 25 preterm (born, on average, 6.6 weeks early) and 77 fullterm infants, between 1 and 6 months postterm. In the first few months, luminance CS was found to be predicted by postterm age, suggesting that preprogrammed development is sufficient to account for luminance CS. By contrast, chromatic CS exceeded that predicted by postterm age, which suggests that time since birth confers a benefit on chromatic CS. The preterms' 6.6 weeks of additional time since birth is roughly equivalent to 3.7 weeks of development in chromatic CS. In sum, these results suggest that chromatic CS is more influenced by early postnatal visual experience than luminance CS, which may have implications for development of parvocellular and magnocellular pathways.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Visión de Colores/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Recien Nacido Prematuro/fisiología , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Edad Gestacional , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa
14.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 48(12): 5523-31, 2007 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18055801

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To investigate nasal-temporal asymmetries in the detection of horizontal motion and in cortical motion visual evoked potential (mVEP) responses in normal infants and children and in patients with infantile esotropia. METHODS: Monocular motion-detection thresholds were obtained separately for nasalward- and temporalward-moving random-dot patterns in a forced-choice, preferential-looking paradigm. Monocular mVEP responses were obtained while subjects viewed a 6-Hz oscillating, 1 cyc/deg vertical sine-wave grating. Nasal-temporal mVEP asymmetry was investigated with two measures from each subject: asymmetric indices (AIs) and interocular phase differences. Performance was compared in 33 visits of 28 normal subjects and 73 visits of 54 patients with infantile esotropia, ranging in age from 2 months to 5 years. RESULTS: At 3 to 5 months of age, both normal infants and patients with infantile esotropia had robust nasal-temporal asymmetries in motion-detection and mVEP measures. By 2 years of age, measures in all normal subjects were symmetric, as they were in patients successfully treated with glasses or alignment surgery, whereas patients who had not yet undergone alignment surgery, regardless of surgery status, had highly asymmetric mVEP responses and motion-detection thresholds. CONCLUSIONS: Young normal and esotropic infants exhibited nasal-temporal asymmetries in both motion detection and mVEP. These asymmetries similarly disappeared over time in normal infants and in patients with esotropia who had received successful, timely correction of misalignment. Although the initial capacity for motion processing is normal in the youngest patients with untreated esotropia at 5 months, cumulative abnormal binocular experience in these patients may disrupt motion mechanisms.


Asunto(s)
Esotropía/fisiopatología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Preescolar , Esotropía/cirugía , Humanos , Lactante , Músculos Oculomotores/cirugía , Umbral Sensorial
15.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 23(9): 2085-96, 2006 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16912735

RESUMEN

Several lines of evidence suggest that the image statistics of the environment shape visual abilities. To date, the image statistics of natural scenes and faces have been well characterized using Fourier analysis. We employed Fourier analysis to characterize images of signs in American Sign Language (ASL). These images are highly relevant to signers who rely on ASL for communication, and thus the image statistics of ASL might influence signers' visual abilities. Fourier analysis was conducted on 105 static images of signs, and these images were compared with analyses of 100 natural scene images and 100 face images. We obtained two metrics from our Fourier analysis: mean amplitude and entropy of the amplitude across the image set (which is a measure from information theory) as a function of spatial frequency and orientation. The results of our analyses revealed interesting differences in image statistics across the three different image sets, setting up the possibility that ASL experience may alter visual perception in predictable ways. In addition, for all image sets, the mean amplitude results were markedly different from the entropy results, which raises the interesting question of which aspect of an image set (mean amplitude or entropy of the amplitude) is better able to account for known visual abilities.


Asunto(s)
Cara/anatomía & histología , Interpretación de Imagen Asistida por Computador/métodos , Lengua de Signos , Percepción Visual , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Humanos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Sensibilidad y Especificidad
16.
Vision Res ; 45(12): 1557-67, 2005 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15781073

RESUMEN

The purpose of this study was to investigate asymmetries in detection of horizontal motion in normal infants and children and in patients with infantile esotropia. Motion detection thresholds (% motion signal) were measured in 75 normal infants and in 36 eyes of 27 infants with infantile esotropia (ET), using a forced-choice preferential looking paradigm with random-dot patterns. Absolute motion detection sensitivity and asymmetries in sensitivity for nasalward (N) vs. temporalward (T) directions of motion were compared in normal and patient populations, ranging in age from 1 month to 5 years. In normal infants, N and T thresholds were equivalent under 2.5 months of age, whereas a superiority for monocular detection of N motion was observed between 3.5 and 6.5 months of age. The nasalward advantage gradually diminished to symmetrical T:N performance by 8 months of age, matching that of adults. No asymmetry was observed in 15 normal infants who performed the task binocularly, hence, the asymmetry was not a leftward/rightward bias. In the youngest infantile ET patients tested, at 5 months of age, a nasalward superiority in motion detection was observed and was equivalent to that of same-age normal infants. However, unlike normals, this asymmetry persists in older patients. This greater asymmetry in infantile ET represents worse detection of T than N motion. This is the first report of an asymmetry in motion detection in normal infants across a wide age range. Initially, motion detection is normal in infants with infantile esotropia. Cumulative abnormal binocular experience in these patients may disrupt motion mechanisms.


Asunto(s)
Esotropía/fisiopatología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Preescolar , Cabeza , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Nariz , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica , Tiempo de Reacción , Umbral Sensorial , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Visión Monocular/fisiología
17.
J Nutr ; 134(9): 2307-13, 2004 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15333721

RESUMEN

Between 6 and 12 mo of age, blood levels of the (n-3) long-chain PUFA, docosahexaenoic acid (DHA), in breast-fed infants typically decrease due to diminished maternal DHA stores and the introduction of DHA-poor solid foods displacing human milk as the primary source of nutrition. Thus, we utilized a randomized, clinical trial format to evaluate the effect of supplemental DHA in solid foods on visual development of breast-fed infants with the primary outcome, sweep visual-evoked potential (VEP) acuity, as an index for maturation of the retina and visual cortex. At 6 mo of age, breast-fed infants were randomly assigned to receive 1 jar (113 g)/d of baby food containing egg yolk enriched with DHA (115 mg DHA/100 g food; n = 25) or control baby food (0 mg DHA; n = 26). Gravimetric measures were used to estimate the supplemental DHA intake which was 83 mg DHA/d in the supplemented group and 0 mg/d in controls. Although many infants in both groups continued to breast-feed for a mean of 9 mo, RBC DHA levels decreased significantly between 6 and 12 mo (from 3.8 to 3.0 g/100 g total fatty acids) in control infants, whereas RBC DHA levels increased by 34% from 4.1 to 5.5 g/100 g by 12 mo in supplemented infants. VEP acuity at 6 mo was 0.49 logMAR (minimum angle of resolution) and improved to 0.29 logMAR by 12 mo in controls. In DHA-supplemented infants, VEP acuity was 0.48 logMAR at 6 mo and matured to 0.14 logMAR at 12 mo (1.5 lines on the eye chart better than controls). At 12 mo, the difference corresponded to 1.5 lines on the eye chart. RBC DHA levels and VEP acuity at 12 mo were correlated (r = -0.50; P = 0.0002), supporting the need of an adequate dietary supply of DHA throughout 1 y of life for neural development.


Asunto(s)
Lactancia Materna , Desarrollo Infantil , Ácidos Docosahexaenoicos/administración & dosificación , Yema de Huevo , Alimentos Fortificados , Alimentos Infantiles , Agudeza Visual , Ácidos Docosahexaenoicos/sangre , Eritrocitos/metabolismo , Potenciales Evocados Visuales , Femenino , Edad Gestacional , Humanos , Lactante , Retina/crecimiento & desarrollo , Factores de Tiempo , Corteza Visual/crecimiento & desarrollo
18.
Am J Ophthalmol ; 138(6): 1003-9, 2004 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15629292

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To define the prevalence and time course of significant changes in angle of deviation during the first months after the diagnosis of infantile esotropia and to determine whether long-term alignment and sensory outcomes differ when surgical alignment is performed on infants with stable vs unstable angles of deviation. DESIGN: Prospective cohort study. METHODS: setting: Institutional and clinical practice. patient population: Newly diagnosed patients with infantile esotropia (N = 208). observation procedure: Preoperative measurements of the angle of deviation on the initial visit and at approximate six-week intervals until surgery was performed. main outcome measures: Ocular alignment at six weeks, one year, and four years postoperative and stereoacuity at age five to nine years. RESULTS: Overall, 57% of infants had an esodeviation on the second visit that was within 10 prism diopters (p.d.) of the deviation measured on the initial visit (stable group), 33% had an increase of 10 p.d. or more (unstable group), and 11% had a decrease of 10 p.d. or more. Among the 127 patients with additional preoperative visits, many switched between the stable and unstable categories during follow-up. Long-term, stable and unstable preoperative alignment groups had similar postoperative motor alignment, re-operation rates, rates of prescription of hyperopic, or bifocal spectacle correction and stereoacuity. CONCLUSIONS: It may not be necessary to wait for a "stable" angle of esodeviation before surgery since both alignment and sensory outcomes were similar for stable and unstable groups.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Esotropía/fisiopatología , Músculos Oculomotores/fisiopatología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Edad de Inicio , Esotropía/cirugía , Humanos , Lactante , Músculos Oculomotores/cirugía , Periodo Posoperatorio , Cuidados Preoperatorios , Estudios Prospectivos , Reoperación , Resultado del Tratamiento
19.
Brain Cogn ; 49(1): 152-69, 2002 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12027400

RESUMEN

Visual abilities in deaf individuals may be altered as a result of auditory deprivation and/or because the deaf rely heavily on a sign language (American Sign Language, or ASL). In this study, we asked whether attentional abilities of deaf subjects are altered. Using a direction of motion discrimination task in the periphery, we investigated three aspects of spatial attention: orienting of attention, divided attention, and selective attention. To separate influences of auditory deprivation and sign language experience, we compared three subject groups: deaf and hearing native signers of ASL and hearing nonsigners. To investigate the ability to orient attention, we compared motion thresholds obtained with and without a valid spatial precue, with the notion that subjects orient to the stimulus prior to its appearance when a precue is presented. Results suggest a slight advantage for deaf subjects in the ability to orient spatial attention. To investigate divided attention, we compared motion thresholds obtained when a single motion target was presented to thresholds obtained when the motion target was presented among confusable distractors. The effect of adding distractors was found to be identical across subject groups, suggesting that attentional capacity is not altered in deaf subjects. Finally, to investigate selective attention, we compared performance for a single, cued motion target with that of a cued motion target presented among distractors. Here, deaf, but not hearing, subjects performed better when the motion target was presented among distractors than when it was presented alone, suggesting that deaf subjects are more affected by the presence of distractors. In sum, our results suggest that attentional orienting and selective attention are altered in the deaf and that these effects are most likely due to auditory deprivation as opposed to sign language experience.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Sordera/psicología , Audición/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento , Lengua de Signos , Percepción Espacial , Conducta Verbal , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Distribución Aleatoria
20.
Brain Cogn ; 49(1): 170-81, 2002 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12027401

RESUMEN

Recently, we reported a strong right visual field/left hemisphere advantage for motion processing in deaf signers and a slight reverse asymmetry in hearing nonsigners (Bosworth & Dobkins, 1999). This visual field asymmetry in deaf signers may be due to auditory deprivation or to experience with a visual-manual language, American Sign Language (ASL). In order to separate these two possible sources, in this study we added a third group, hearing native signers, who have normal hearing and have learned ASL from their deaf parents. As in our previous study, subjects performed a direction-of-motion discrimination task at different locations across the visual field. In addition to investigating differences in left vs right visual field asymmetries across subject groups, we also asked whether performance differences exist for superior vs inferior visual fields and peripheral vs central visual fields. Replicating our previous study, a robust right visual field advantage was observed in deaf signers, but not in hearing nonsigners. Like deaf signers, hearing signers also exhibited a strong right visual field advantage, suggesting that this effect is related to experience with sign language. These results suggest that perceptual processes required for the acquisition and comprehension of language (motion processing in the case of ASL) are recruited by the left, language-dominant, hemisphere. Deaf subjects also exhibited an inferior visual field advantage that was significantly larger than that observed in either hearing group. In addition, there was a trend for deaf subjects to perform relatively better on peripheral than on central stimuli, while both hearing groups showed the reverse pattern. Because deaf signers differed from hearing signers and nonsigners along these domains, the inferior and peripheral visual field advantages observed in deaf subjects is presumably related to auditory deprivation. Finally, these visual field asymmetries were not modulated by attention for any subject group, suggesting they are a result of sensory, and not attentional, factors.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Sordera , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Audición/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Lengua de Signos , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Distribución Aleatoria , Percepción Visual/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...