Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 20
Filtrar
1.
J Mot Behav ; 50(1): 80-95, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28350231

RESUMEN

Tool use is typically explored via actor-tool interactions. However, the target-object (that which is being acted on) may influence perceived action possibilities and thereby guide action. Three different tool-target-object pairings were tested (Experiment 1). The hammering action demonstrated the greatest sensitivity and therefore subsequently used to further investigate target-object pairings. The hammer was removed as an option and instructions were provided using pictorial (Experiment 2), written (Experiment 3), and both pictorial and written formats (Experiment 4). The designed tool is chosen when available (Experiment 1) and when removed as a choice (i.e., the hammer), participants perform the same action associated with the designed tool (i.e., hammering) regardless of instruction method (Experiments 2, 3, and 4).


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Intención , Masculino , Adulto Joven
2.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 164: 1-9, 2016 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26705898

RESUMEN

People typically move in an anticipatory manner, planning the intended action in advance to minimize the energy costs associated with producing the action (e.g., Rosenbaum et al., 2009). This is exemplified behaviorally in the end-state comfort effect, which is characterized by the selection of an uncomfortable initial posture to enable a comfortable posture upon completion of the movement (Rosenbaum et al., 1990). The main objective of this study was to further investigate the end-state comfort effect in left- and right-handers (N=20). More specifically, to: (a) understand the influence of mode of action execution; and (b) delineate the role of handedness. The overturned glass task (Fischman, 1997) was used as means of assessment, where participants were asked to demonstrate picking up a glass to pour water in four modes of execution: (1) pantomime without a stimulus; (2) pantomime with image of the glass as a guide; (3) pantomime with glass as a guide; and (4) grasping the glass. End-state comfort was displayed regardless of mode of execution, hand used to complete the task or handedness group. However, kinematic analysis revealed distinct differences, highlighting how movement parameters are altered as a result the mode of action execution.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Postura/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Femenino , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
3.
Res Dev Disabil ; 34(11): 4142-53, 2013 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24060728

RESUMEN

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most commonly diagnosed neurobehavioural disorder. Characterized by recurring problems with impulsiveness and inattention in combination with hyperactivity, motor impairments have also been well documented in the literature. The aim of this study was to compare the fine and gross motor skills of male and female children with ADHD and their neurotypical counterparts within seven skill assessments. This included three fine motor tasks: (1) spiral tracing, (2) dot filling, (3) tweezers and beads; and four gross motor tasks: (1) twistbox, (2) foot tapping, (3) small plate finger tapping, and (4) large plate finger tapping. It was hypothesized that children with ADHD would display poorer motor skills in comparison to neurotypical controls in both fine and gross motor assessments. However, statistically significant differences between the groups only emerged in four of the seven tasks (spiral tracing, dot filling, tweezers and beads and foot tapping). In line with previous findings, the complexity underlying upper limb tasks solidified the divide in performance between children with ADHD and their neurotypical counterparts. In light of similar research, impairments in lower limb motor skill were also observed. Future research is required to further delineate trends in motor difficulties in ADHD, while further investigating the underlying mechanisms of impairment.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Trastornos de la Destreza Motora/fisiopatología , Desempeño Psicomotor , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/complicaciones , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Destreza Motora , Trastornos de la Destreza Motora/complicaciones
4.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 42(10): 2192-201, 2012 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22350451

RESUMEN

Motor control deficits have been documented in children with high functioning autism and Asperger syndrome (HFA/AS), but the extent to which these disorders affect the children's footedness must be delineated. Twelve typically developing (TD) children and 12 children with HFA/AS, ages 6-9 years, were recruited. Motor control skills were assessed through a variety of footedness tasks to determine location and nature of impairment, regarding motor dominance. Overall, greater inconsistencies in dominance arose in children with HFA/AS, through disparities in measures of preference. Results will have broader implications for understanding motor impairments in children with HFA/AS as determined by comparing performance on footedness tasks, as well as for the design of interventions to account for these deficits.


Asunto(s)
Síndrome de Asperger/fisiopatología , Trastorno Autístico/fisiopatología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Niño , Pie , Mano , Humanos
5.
Laterality ; 16(6): 707-21, 2011 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21391106

RESUMEN

The current study investigated (a) whether or not the WatHand Cabinet Test (WHCT, Bryden, Roy, & Spence, 2007) could be used as accurately as the Waterloo Handedness Questionnaire (WHQ) to classify individuals into language lateralisation groups based on their hand preference, and (b) the relationship between direction and degree of hand preference and language lateralisation. A total of 142 participants (82 right-handers and 60 left-handers) completed the WHQ and the WHCT, and performed a fused-words dichotic listening test. Findings indicated that the WHCT was robust alternative to the WHQ in providing a measure of hand preference as there was a high correlation between the WHCT and the WHQ, and individuals were divided into similar language lateralisation groups when using either the WHCT or the WHQ as the classifying variable. More specifically, there existed a predictable pattern of language lateralisation into which members of different handedness groups fell. The same pattern exists whether handedness is defined using subjective questionnaires or more objective observational measures of hand preference.


Asunto(s)
Pruebas de Audición Dicótica/psicología , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Lenguaje , Encuestas y Cuestionarios/normas , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Sesgo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
6.
Dev Neuropsychol ; 32(3): 825-46, 2007.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17956184

RESUMEN

The current study was performed to assess the WatHand Cabinet Test (WHCT), a newly developed multidimensional observational test of handedness. Because the test is observational, it is ideal for assessing children, as it does not require a high degree of verbal comprehension on the part of the participants. 548 individuals participated in the present study on a voluntary basis. Individuals of varying ages were examined (including 3 to 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, and 11-year-olds, and 19-to 24-year-olds). Each participant was asked to complete the WHCT, the Annett Pegboard (Annett, 1985), and the Waterloo Handedness Questionnaire (WHQ) (M.P. Bryden, 1977). Overall, the total score on the WHCT was significantly correlated with both the WHQ, r = .795, p < .01, and the Annett Pegboard, r = .542, p < .01. Sub-scores measuring skilled performance, bimanual performance, and internal consistency were also examined, as well as performance on the three tests as a function of age. Overall, findings indicate that that the WHCT is a valid test of hand preference in both children and adults. Its ease of use, quick administration, and built-in quantitative sub-scores offer a robust alternative method for measuring hand preference.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Observación/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Análisis de Regresión , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
7.
Exp Brain Res ; 183(4): 541-56, 2007 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17668195

RESUMEN

In the present series of experiments we investigated how object transport and rotate movements are performed when they are directed away from (Experiment 1) and toward (Experiment 2) the body under both unimanual and bimanual conditions. Our results indicated that unimanual conditions are faster and more efficiently produced than bimanual movements in far peripersonal space, suggesting that there is a cost to performing bimanual movements. However, in near peripersonal space, bimanual same movements were performed in a manner similar to unimanual movements, indicating that there is no significant cost associated with similar bimanual movements that are performed using the lower visual field and in near peripersonal space. Both experiments also indicate that the two hands are tightly synchronized when the two movements being performed require the same rotation. However, when performing bimanual movements where the rotation being performed by the two hands is different, this synchronization is weaker. Finally, the combined results from the two experiments indicated that movements made toward the body are not performed in a similar manner to movements that are made away from the body. Specifically, it is clear from the current studies that movements toward the body are performed faster and possibly that the hands are less synchronized for bimanual movements requiring different rotations by the two hands.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Mano/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Rotación , Soporte de Peso/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Movimiento , Postura
8.
Laterality ; 12(4): 364-77, 2007 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17558818

RESUMEN

Previous work by Bryden and Roy (1999) showed a larger performance difference between the hands for placing pegs into holes on the Grooved Pegboard test than for removing pegs from the holes. The authors argued that these data provided evidence of the importance of task demands in manual asymmetries. However the study failed to control for the differing starting positions of the pegs. To clarify this possible confound, the purpose of the current investigation was to determine the influence of starting position on the between-hand performance differences on the Grooved Pegboard Test. To do so, both the start and end positions were manipulated, such that participants moved the pegs from the receptacle to another receptacle or to a set of holes, or participants moved the pegs from a set of holes to a receptacle or another set of holes. A total of 30 right-handed individuals (as classified using the Waterloo Handedness Questionnaire) participated in the experiment, completing five trials with each hand for each of the four conditions. While no significant effects of start position were found, a significant interaction between hand and end position, F(1.29) = 30.85, P<.001, was found for the time to complete the task, where larger differences between the hands, fovouring the right hand, were seen for placing pegs into the holes as opposed to the receptacles. This effect was also found when the data were expressed using a laterality quotient. The results are discussed in terms of the influence of task complexity on manual asymmetries.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Lateralidad Funcional , Destreza Motora , Orientación , Desempeño Psicomotor , Adolescente , Adulto , Conducta de Elección , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción
9.
Laterality ; 10(5): 429-40, 2005 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16191813

RESUMEN

The current study investigated the incidence of various health disorders in a sample population of undergraduate students, using a 25-item hand preference questionnaire. Analysis revealed significant differences existed in the incidence of several health disorders as a function of handedness: however, these differences depended on how hand preference was defined. Overall, the results showed that non-right-handed participants (i.e., those individuals with left-handed, ambidextrous, or inconsistent hand preferences) were significantly more likely to report specific health disorders (such as high blood pressure and epilepsy) than right-handed participants. The findings provide support for the argument that left-handedness, or non-right-handedness, is associated with various health disorders. The results are discussed in light of the Geschwind-Behan-Galaburda model of cerebral lateralisation.


Asunto(s)
Epilepsia/epidemiología , Lateralidad Funcional , Estado de Salud , Hipertensión/epidemiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Incidencia , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Factores de Riesgo
10.
Brain Cogn ; 58(3): 258-68, 2005 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15963376

RESUMEN

The purpose of the present study was twofold: first to examine the influences of sex and handedness on manual performance on the Grooved Pegboard Test; and secondly to provide normative data for two versions (Place and Remove tasks) of the Grooved Pegboard Test, as previous work (Bryden & Roy, 1999) had suggested that the Remove task of the Grooved pegboard may provide a purer measure of motor speed of the two hands than the standard administration of the Grooved Pegboard Test. One hundred and fifty-three (47 males and 106 females) participants completed the Grooved Pegboard Test. Individuals performed the standard version of the Grooved Pegboard Test (Place task) and a novel version of the test (Remove task). In the standard version, participants were timed on their speed for placing the pegs, while in the novel version they were timed on their speed for removing the pegs. Results confirmed previously noted hand and sex differences in the Place task of the Grooved Pegboard Test, as well as the lack of effect of handedness on performance (Bornstein, 1995; Ruff & Parker, 1993). Significant performance differences between the hands were also noted for the Remove task. Findings also indicated that the Remove task was sensitive to sex and handedness effects.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Mano/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Valores de Referencia , Factores Sexuales
11.
Brain Cogn ; 57(1): 26-9, 2005 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15629210

RESUMEN

The purpose of the current investigation was to examine the age-related changes in the performance of the two hands on the Annett pegboard (Annett, 1970). The current study was part of a large-scale study investigating the development of unimanual and bimanual performance. Three hundred and two right-handed individuals participated in the present study, comprising five different age groups (3-5 year olds, 6 and 7 year olds, 8 and 9 year olds, 10-12 year olds, and 19-24 year olds). All participants completed the Waterloo Handedness Questionnaire and the standard Annett pegboard. Analyses revealed significant overall performance differences between the hands, as expected, where the right hand was significantly faster than the left hand. Additionally, significant performance differences between the hands were noted as a function of age. More specifically, the adults showed a much smaller performance difference between the hands than the other age groups. The results are discussed in light of current theories of the development of hand preference and manual asymmetry.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Valores de Referencia
12.
Contraception ; 63(4): 223-7, 2001 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11376650

RESUMEN

As research has shown that knowledge concerning the health risks and benefits of oral contraceptive (OC) use is a contributor to OC compliance, the following study examined an educated sample of young women to determine the level of knowledge concerning the risks and benefits of OC use. A 28-item questionnaire was developed by the research team and distributed to female undergraduate students. Five questions were designed to assess the respondent's knowledge of OC. Overall, it found that OC users answered 71.9% of the questions correctly, whereas non-users answered significantly fewer questions correctly. As well, the number of correct responses increased with academic year, indicating that younger women were less knowledgeable about OC. The results of this study indicate that despite increased efforts to educate women, knowledge of OC remains a major problem, even in a sample of women with relatively high socio-economic status.


Asunto(s)
Anticonceptivos Orales/efectos adversos , Conocimientos, Actitudes y Práctica en Salud , Promoción de la Salud , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Cooperación del Paciente , Factores de Riesgo , Fumar/efectos adversos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Universidades
13.
Contraception ; 63(4): 229-33, 2001 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11376651

RESUMEN

The etymology and use of oral contraceptives (OCs) has greatly influenced the sexual practices and social lives (e.g., decisions concerning childbearing, careers) of adolescent and adult women. In addition, OCs have the potential to benefit or harm its users depending upon the utilization practices and characteristics/lifestyle habits of the women consuming them. A 28-item questionnaire was distributed to female students at a university in Central Ontario and examined: (1) socio-demographic information; (2) background information concerning oral contraceptive use (e.g., length of time taking the pill, concerns about pill use, side effects while on the pill); and (3) procedure for taking the pill (e.g. maximizing effectiveness and safety). Preliminary analysis indicated that females may not be equipped with adequate knowledge or receiving the necessary support to practice the most efficacious behaviours surrounding pill use.


Asunto(s)
Anticonceptivos Orales/administración & dosificación , Adolescente , Adulto , Anticonceptivos Orales/efectos adversos , Demografía , Femenino , Conocimientos, Actitudes y Práctica en Salud , Humanos , Cooperación del Paciente , Educación del Paciente como Asunto , Conducta Sexual , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Universidades
14.
Laterality ; 6(2): 141-7, 2001 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15513166

RESUMEN

Controversy exists concerning whether handedness is a unidimensional trait or a multidimensional trait. However, researchers espousing the view that handedness is multidimensional are in disagreement regarding what the dimensions are that underlie handedness. Healey, Liederman, and Geschwind (1986) have provided some evidence that the type of musculature utilised (i.e., proximal versus distal) may be an underlying factor in determining handedness. The present research attempted to test this notion by examining the effects of target orientation on the relative performance of the two hands, using a peg-moving task. Target orientation was examined by utilising key-shaped pegs that fit securely into key-shaped holes. The results showed that the presence of target orientation increased the preferred-hand advantage. It is considered that this increased advantage for the right hand may be due to the hemispheric control of proximal versus distal musculature suggested by Geschwind and Galaburda (1985).

15.
Brain Cogn ; 44(3): 402-14, 2000 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11104533

RESUMEN

The present paper describes a performance method for determining hand preference. The task requires participants to reach into different regions of hemispace to perform various actions (point, pick up, toss, sweep, and position) with a dowel located at each position. In accordance with the participants' hand preference as measured by the Waterloo Handedness Questionnaire, the preferred hand was used more frequently on the various performance tasks. The distribution of hand use in working space indicates that preferred hand use was almost exclusive for actions carried out in ipsilateral hemispace, while it is used only moderately for actions in contralateral hemispace, revealing that this hand is used throughout a wider range of extrapersonal space than the nonpreferred hand. These trends were observed across all of the performance tasks, suggesting that task complexity did not affect the frequency of preferred hand use either overall or, more specifically, in right hemispace, as was predicted. This finding is inconsistent with empirical work on questionnaires indicating that verbal reports of preferred hand use increase for more complex tasks (e.g., Steenhuis & Bryden, 1988). As well, performance on the preferential reaching task correlated significantly with hand preference as measured on the Waterloo Handedness Questionnaire (Bryden, 1977), unlike the other performance measure examined, indicating that the preferential reaching task is sensitive to differences in the degree of hand preference.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Adulto , Mano/fisiología , Humanos , Movimiento/fisiología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
16.
Brain Cogn ; 43(1-3): 60-4, 2000.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10857664

RESUMEN

The present study describes a performance-based method of measuring hand preference in children. Three aspects of handedness were considered to be important in developing the paradigm (a) overall hand preference across a number of tasks, (b) consistency of hand use and, (c) the use of the preferred hand in a bimanual task. The new paradigm, termed the WatHand Box Test (WBT), requires participants to perform a variety of unimanual tasks such as, using a hammer, tossing a ball, and opening a lock with a key. To determine the validity of the WBT and examine the developmental trends in hand preference, eighty right-handed children and adults (ages 3-4, 6-7, 9-10, and 18-24 years) performed the WBT. First, the WBT was found to correlate significantly with scores on a standard hand preference questionnaire for the adults. As well, significant developmental trends were noted in hand preference as measured by the WBT. Most specifically, three- and four-year-olds had significantly lower scores on the WBT indicating a less stable pattern of hand preference than in the other three age groups.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
17.
Brain Cogn ; 43(1-3): 370-4, 2000.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10857728

RESUMEN

The present study describes a developmental performance measure of hand preference that considers task complexity and position in hemispace. Eighty right-handed children and adults (ages 3-4, 6-7, 9-10, 18-24) were observed for hand selection responses to 2 unimanual tasks (simple vs complex) across positions in hemispace. Results revealed an age-related trend in the tendency to use the preferred hand in right and left hemispace. While the adult's and 3- to 4-year-old's preferred hand use decreased as they moved into left hemispace, children between the ages of 6 and 10 years tended to use their preferred hands consistently throughout both regions of hemispace. The relationship between hand preference and skilled, cost-efficient performance throughout development are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
18.
Brain Cogn ; 43(1-3): 421-5, 2000.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10857739

RESUMEN

Twenty-nine right-handed participants made unimanual movements to 5 target locations while standing and while balancing on a rocker platform. Manual preference was affected by target location (p < .001; manual preference tended to be concordant with target hemispace) and posture (p < .05; responses from the rocker platform were associated with a lower level of right-hand responding). A location by posture interaction (p < .05) indicated that the effect of posture was present only for targets in left hemispace; the right hand was used less frequently in the rocker condition than simple standing. Manual preference is viewed as a process of response selection motivated by an effort to minimize neural processing and mechanical costs.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Postura , Encéfalo/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
19.
Laterality ; 4(1): 27-37, 1999 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15513102

RESUMEN

The degree of hand preference and the magnitude of hand differences in performance are thought to relate to skill level. The purpose of the experiments discussed here was to examine the effect of skill as reflected in the spatial precision demands of a movement, on the extent of manual asymmetries. Experiment 1 utilised the Annett pegboard, and manipulated spatial precision by varying peg diameter. The second experiment utilised the Grooved pegboard, and manipulated spatial precision by altering the size of the target hole. Analyses revealed that movement time increased as a function of the increased spatial precision demands. Although the preferred hand was always faster, the magnitude of this advantage was only affected in Experiment 2, in which the manipulation of spatial precision was maximal. The implications of these findings for the skill interpretation of handedness are discussed.

20.
Spat Vis ; 8(1): 77-94, 1994.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8049171

RESUMEN

The McCollough effect (ME) is an example of a pattern-contingent colour aftereffect. This study describes some characteristics of another visual aftereffect linking pattern and colour here called a colour-contingent pattern aftereffect (CCPA). After inducing with, for example, a magenta and black radial pattern and a green and black pattern of concentric circles, presentation of a green homogeneous field evoked a faint image of a radial pattern superimposed on the field, whereas presentation of a magenta homogeneous field produced a faint image of concentric circles. The pattern was blurred and fleeting, occurring with the onset of the homogeneous field, but nevertheless was evoked reliably. Various properties of these colour-contingent pattern aftereffects are reported. Although the aftereffects have some of the characteristics of the ME, the CCPA is not as long lasting as the ME, and, unlike the usual ME, it is abolished if eye-movements are made during induction.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Humanos , Visión Ocular/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...