Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 112
Filtrar
1.
Cogn Process ; 2024 May 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38748044

RESUMEN

Research has shown that differences between males and females are not in general intelligence, but only in some specific factors and tasks. We used the Italian standardization of the Leiter-3, which is a completely nonverbal cognitive battery, to investigate the nature of sex/gender (we used sex/gender to reflect the awareness that the effects of biological 'sex' and socially constructed 'gender' cannot be easily separated and that most individuals' identities are informed by both sex and gender) differences. In doing so we used a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis approach. Results confirmed that males and females perform similarly in general intelligence, but present with some specificities. Males perform better on some, but not all, tasks requiring the spatial manipulation of the stimuli, but females consistently outperform males in tasks such as the nonverbal Stroop, requiring inhibition and attention control to a larger extent. The clinical and practical implications of our findings are considerable. The identification of specific cognitive strengths and weaknesses in males and females underscores the importance of tailored approaches in clinical assessments and interventions.

2.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; : 1-8, 2024 Mar 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38461450

RESUMEN

INSTRUCTION: Children with a Specific Learning Disorder (SLD) write linguistic material more slowly than children with typically developing (TD). However, it is not known whether the same difficulties are present when they write numbers. The goal of the present study was to fill this gap and to compare TD's and SLD's speed in writing numbers both in words and in digits. METHODS: Therefore, we examined the ability to write numbers in words and digits (numerals) in a sample of sixth- to eighth-grade children diagnosed with SLD. We assessed 32 children with SLD (17 males and 15 females) and a control group of students with TD matched for sex, age, and grade with two writing speed tasks: writing numbers in words and in digits. The two tasks were administered both in normal condition (N) and in articulatory suppression condition (AS). RESULTS: We found that 6th to 8th graders with a SLD were slower than TD children when writing numbers, both in words and in digits, and their slowness was similar in the two cases. However, when the tasks were carried out under a condition of articulatory suppression, the SLD group exhibited a conspicuous impairment, only when writing numbers in words. A similar pattern of performance was observed also in the case of writing errors. CONCLUSION: We concluded that children with SLD have a general speed problem that may affect writing of different materials but also a specific problem related to the processing of phonological information during writing.

3.
J Intell ; 11(12)2023 Dec 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38132841

RESUMEN

Despite growing research on adults with specific learning disabilities (SLDs), evidence concerning their intellectual profile remains scarce. The present study examined the results of the administration of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition to 301 adults diagnosed with SLDs and compared them to the results obtained from previous studies with a large sample of children with SLDs. The results showed that: (1) as observed among children, adults with SLDs also presented higher scores in the subtests implying reasoning (associated with the General Ability Index, GAI) and lower scores in the subtests involving working memory and processing speed; (2) the discrepancy between full-scale IQ and the GAI had a good predictive value in discriminating adults with and without SLDs; (3) the four-factor hierarchical structure of intelligence proposed for the general adult population held for adults with SLDs as well, even though there were substantial differences in the loadings and a five-factor structure could be more appropriate; (4) similarities as well as strong differences were present between adults and children with SLDs. In adults, scores on subtests were generally lower, particularly in working memory and processing speed. However, in some cases, scores were equal or even higher (as in the "Similarity" subtest) among adults, meaning that the discrepancy between the full scale and the GAI was accentuated.

4.
J Intell ; 11(9)2023 Sep 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37754907

RESUMEN

Despite being repeatedly investigated in children with typical development, research on gender differences in intellectual abilities in specific groups of children, including children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), has been scarce. In this paper, we evaluated the performance of a large group of Italian children with ADHD using the WISC-IV. We aimed at investigating the presence of gender differences using a multi-group confirmatory factor analysis approach. Results showed that the WISC is largely gender-invariant. However, some tasks present non-invariant patterns (block design and coding). Differences at the latent level also showed some differences (favoring boys) in the verbal comprehension index. Conversely, differences at the latent level were not found in the full-scale IQ or in the other main indices. These results have theoretical and practical implications.

5.
J Learn Disabil ; 56(5): 410-420, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36905142

RESUMEN

Copying a text quickly and accurately is important both in school and in daily life. However, this skill has never been systematically studied, either in children with typical development (TD) or in children with specific learning disabilities (SLD). The aim of this research was to study the features of a copy task and its relationship with other writing tasks. For this purpose, 674 children with TD and 65 children with SLD from Grades 6 through 8 in Italy were tested with a copy task and other writing assessment tasks, measuring three aspects of writing: handwriting speed, spelling, and expressive writing. Children with SLD performed worse on the copy task, both in terms of speed and accuracy, than children with TD. Copy speed was predicted by grade level and by all three major writing skills for children with TD but only by handwriting speed and spelling for children with SLD. Copy accuracy was predicted by gender and the three major writing skills for children with TD but only by spelling for children with SLD. These results suggest that children with SLD also have difficulty copying a text and benefit less than children with TD from their other writing skills.


Asunto(s)
Escritura Manual , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje , Humanos , Niño , Escritura , Lenguaje , Instituciones Académicas , Italia
6.
Res Dev Disabil ; 129: 104306, 2022 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35863174

RESUMEN

This study examines the structure, profile, and diagnostic significance of intelligence in a group of 948 children diagnosed with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) assessed with the WISC-IV and compared with children with specific learning disorders (SLDs) and with typically developing children. Based on four indexes, the WISC-IV configuration found in TD resulted applicable to ADHD, but with generally lower loadings on g. The Perceptual Reasoning and Verbal Comprehension indexes not only had higher loadings compared to the other two indexes but also represented the relative strengths of children with ADHD, as previously observed for children with SLD. In fact, the WISC pattern could be successfully used for discriminating between ADHD and TD, but not between ADHD and SLD. The latter result was not due to a co-occurrence of a learning disorder because the presence or absence of an associated diagnosis of SLD negligibly affected the pattern observed in ADHD. We concluded that the characteristics of intelligence in children with ADHD can be relevant for assessing this disorder, and that ADHD and SLDs share largely similar underlying cognitive features.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje , Trastorno Específico de Aprendizaje , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Niño , Humanos , Inteligencia , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje/diagnóstico , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje/psicología , Trastorno Específico de Aprendizaje/diagnóstico , Escalas de Wechsler
7.
Dev Psychol ; 58(9): 1687-1701, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35666924

RESUMEN

Several models of working memory (WM) have been proposed in the literature. Most of the research on the architecture of WM is based on adults or older children, but less is known about younger children. In this study, we tested various models of WM on a sample of 739 Italian children, ranging in age from 3 to 8 years, primarily of European heritage and from medium to medium-high socioeconomic background. Participants were assessed with 12 WM tasks, systematically varying the modality and level of executive control required (based on the number of activities to be performed at once: retention alone, ignoring distractors, and dealing with dual tasks). We examined younger children (n = 501, Mage = 56.8 months, SD = 6.4, 48% boys) and older children (n = 238, Mage = 80.0 months, SD = 9.0, 58% boys) separately using multigroup confirmatory factor analyses. A Bayesian analytical approach was adopted. Our results suggested that a four-factor model distinguishing between verbal, visual, spatial-simultaneous, and spatial-sequential components of WM achieved the best fit. Overall, the WM structure was very similar in the two groups. We further explored this result with an additional model with a central executive factor loaded on high-control tasks only and found evidence for the presence of an executive control component. The contribution of this factor in terms of explained variance was only modest, however. Our findings demonstrate that it is important to distinguish between WM components in young children. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Adolescente , Adulto , Teorema de Bayes , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
J Learn Disabil ; 55(4): 338-348, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34416830

RESUMEN

Letters and numbers are different domains, and their differentiation increases with schooling. It has nonetheless been argued that reading alphabetic and numerical materials partly involves the same processes, even in adults. Whether individuals with dyslexia have difficulty reading and writing numbers remains to be established. This study examined this issue in a group of 30 young adults with a diagnosis of dyslexia, without any concurrent specific difficulty in processing quantities, compared with a typically developing group matched for gender, age, university attended and course of studies, and approximate calculation ability. The results showed that adults with dyslexia also have severe difficulty in reading and writing numbers. It emerged that their number reading speed correlated moderately with word reading speed. We concluded that dyslexia is specifically related with difficulties in reading and writing not only alphabetic material but also numerical material. Our findings suggest that these abilities should be considered more carefully when assessing and supporting individuals with dyslexia.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia , Lectura , Cognición , Dislexia/diagnóstico , Humanos , Universidades , Escritura , Adulto Joven
9.
Br J Educ Psychol ; 91(4): 1537-1554, 2021 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34148228

RESUMEN

It has been suggested that not only domain-specific factors but also working memory (WM) may play a crucial role in mathematical learning included Geometry, but the issue has not been deeply explored. In the present study, we examined the role of domain-specific factors and of verbal versus visuospatial WM on geometric learning of a new geometrical figure (trapezoid), never presented previously by the teachers participating to the study, after a lecture also involving manipulatives. Results on 105 children in their Year 4 indicated that not only some domain-specific components (geometric declarative knowledge and calculation) but also visuospatial working memory had a significant specific impact on the ability of solving geometric problems requiring to calculate the perimeter and the area of the new figure. On the contrary, verbal WM and geometrical mental imagery did not offer a specific contribution. These findings could have important educational implications, stressing the importance of taking into account the main different aspects supporting the acquisition of geometry.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Niño , Humanos , Matemática , Solución de Problemas
10.
Handb Clin Neurol ; 174: 83-91, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32977898

RESUMEN

Nonverbal learning disability is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a core deficit in visuospatial processing with possibly associated problems in attention, motor, academic, and social skills, but without associated neurologic or genetic syndromes. The present chapter, after a brief historic overview of this disorder, will present fresh evidence that clearly shows neuropsychologic and neuroanatomical distinctions between children with nonverbal learning disability and those with other neurodevelopmental disorders. It ends with an attempt to find shared and valid diagnostic criteria. Acknowledging this disorder as a distinct diagnostic category will open up new research avenues with important scientific and clinical implications.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje , Niño , Humanos , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje/etiología
11.
Front Psychol ; 11: 751, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32390914

RESUMEN

Since the introduction of writing systems, reading comprehension has always been a foundation for achievement in several areas within the educational system, as well as a prerequisite for successful participation in most areas of adult life. The increased availability of technologies and web-based resources can be a really valid support, both in the educational and clinical field, to devise training activities that can also be carried out remotely. There are studies in current literature that has examined the efficacy of internet-based programs for reading comprehension for children with reading comprehension difficulties but almost none considered distance rehabilitation programs. The present paper reports data concerning a distance program Cloze, developed in Italy, for improving language and reading comprehension. Twenty-eight children from 3rd to 6th grade with comprehension difficulties were involved. These children completed the distance program for 15-20 min for at least three times a week for about 4 months. The program was presented separately to each child, with a degree of difficulty adapted to his/her characteristics. Text reading comprehension (assessed distinguishing between narrative and informative texts) increased after intervention. These findings have clinical and educational implications as they suggest that it is possible to promote reading comprehension with a distance individualized program, avoiding the need for the child displacements, necessary for reaching a rehabilitation center.

12.
Psychol Res ; 84(6): 1622-1634, 2020 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30949787

RESUMEN

The present study took advantage of data collected on more than 2100 Italian adult and elderly individuals during the standardization of the WAIS-IV to examine the relationship between working memory (WM) components and intelligence, and how age affects this relationship. Administering the WAIS-IV enabled us to obtain five different measures for assessing different aspects of WM, and a measure of General Ability (GA) strongly loading on the g-factor. The main results were as follows: (1) age-related impairments in WM are substantial, and they are partly similar and partly different for the various WM measures; (2) the relationship between the WM measures and the General Ability Index (GAI) varies, becoming stronger when the active control required by the WM task is higher; (3) comparing the WM-GAI relationships between different age groups reveals some similar patterns, as well as some specific effects that depend on the WM task considered.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Inteligencia , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Escalas de Wechsler/estadística & datos numéricos , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
13.
Br J Educ Psychol ; 90 Suppl 1: 91-104, 2020 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31369147

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Reading can be assessed using different materials, including non-words and texts. Unlike reading words or non-words, reading a text may be supported by reading comprehension, and the extent of this support could change with the amount of schooling. AIM: The present study aimed to examine how reading decoding in a shallow orthography like Italian changed with years of schooling, depending on the type of material and the contribution of non-word reading and reading comprehension to text reading speed. METHODS: Six hundred and forty two typically developing Italian students from 8 to 16 years old were involved. They were administered grade-appropriate tasks assessing text reading speed, non-word reading speed, and reading comprehension. RESULTS: The results showed that, although the two reading speed measures correlated closely, non-word reading speed improved only slightly with age, while the increase in text reading speed was steeper. Reading comprehension was a significant direct predictor of text reading speed after controlling for non-word reading speed. Importantly, however, while the difference in reading speed between non-words and text widened with schooling, the role of reading comprehension declined significantly, the ΔR2 dropping from .10 in primary school to just .01 in high school. CONCLUSIONS: These findings and their implications are discussed in the light of the relationship between reading comprehension and reading speed in a language with a shallow orthography across school grades.


Asunto(s)
Comprensión , Escolaridad , Psicolingüística , Lectura , Adolescente , Niño , Comprensión/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Factores de Tiempo
14.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 182: 86-101, 2019 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30807907

RESUMEN

It has been hypothesized that children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) present difficulties in processing time durations. However, so far evidence on this difficulty and its related mechanisms has been unclear and collected only with rating scales or laboratory experimental tasks. The current study examined whether this difficulty can be seen in children carrying out everyday actions (e.g., telephone calls, cooking activities) and to what extent it is influenced by working memory (WM) abilities. In total, 182 children aged 7 to 10 years were included in the study: 91 children with ADHD symptoms and 91 typically developing (TD) children matched for gender and other characteristics. We administered sequence reordering, time reproduction, and duration comparison tasks, and as stimuli we used six movies lasting 10 to 60 s showing three women completing six different actions. We also collected measures of verbal and visuospatial WM tests (digit span and Corsi task). Children with ADHD symptoms tended to underestimate the long durations and were less accurate than TD children in remembering the exact order of events and in comparing the duration of two different events. These difficulties appeared to be related to WM abilities.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Recuerdo Mental , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
15.
Res Dev Disabil ; 87: 64-72, 2019 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30776739

RESUMEN

Children with a specific learning disorder (SLD) are often characterized by marked intellectual strengths and weaknesses. In the last few years, research has focused on a common discrepancy between low working memory and processing speed on the one hand, and high verbal and visuoperceptual intelligence on the other. SLD profiles featuring a specific discrepancy between verbal and visuoperceptual abilities have been only marginally considered, however, and their systematic comparison vis-à-vis typically-developing (TD) populations has yet to be conducted. The present study examined a dataset of 1624 WISC-IV profiles of children with a diagnosis of SLD. It emerged that the proportion of children with a Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI) at least 1.5 SD (22 standardized points) lower than their scores on the Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI) was larger than the proportion of SLD children with the opposite discrepant profile; it was also larger than the same proportion found among TD children. Comparing the two discrepant profiles revealed that the children also differed by type of learning difficulty, gender, and performance in the WISC-IV Symbol search task. Further examination suggested that children who were discrepant and also distinctly poor in visuoperceptual intelligence were particularly slow in general processing.


Asunto(s)
Pruebas de Inteligencia , Inteligencia , Trastorno Específico de Aprendizaje , Conducta Verbal , Percepción Visual , Adolescente , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Cognición , Comprensión , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Escalas de Wechsler
16.
Child Neuropsychol ; 25(8): 1063-1083, 2019 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30706757

RESUMEN

Reading decoding requires the association of a visual input (the printed word) with a verbal output (the spoken word), and the learning of visual-phonological associations via repeated exposure occurs both explicitly (by instruction and training) and implicitly (by exposure to written material). However, a general ability to implicitly learn visual-phonological associations (or cross-modal bindings) in children with reading difficulties (RD) has not been deeply explored. The present paper examined this issue in two studies comparing groups of children with RD with matched groups of control children in a working memory binding task involving sequences of variable combinations of nonsense shapes and nonwords (odd trials), as well as fixed combinations (even trials). Stimuli presentation was followed by a recognition test in which the nonwords were given one at a time and the children were required to mouse click on their respective shapes. In Study 1, fixed bindings were presented in random sequences across even trials, and the recognition test presented the nonwords at random. In Study 2, fixed bindings were presented in the same order across even trials, and the recognition test presented the nonwords following the same sequence order. In both studies, a consistent and strong learning effect of fixed bindings was observed for the control groups, with significantly better performance than the groups with RD. Such results suggest that poor cross-modal binding learning is associated with reading difficulties. Implications for the study and treatment of dyslexia are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia/complicaciones , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Fonética , Lectura , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Proyectos de Investigación
17.
Child Neuropsychol ; 25(1): 103-121, 2019 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29460683

RESUMEN

The writing abilities of children with ADHD symptoms were examined in a simple dictation task, and then in two conditions with concurrent verbal or visuospatial working memory (WM) loads. The children with ADHD symptoms generally made more spelling mistakes than controls, and the concurrent loads impaired their performance, but with partly different effects. The concurrent verbal WM task prompted an increase in the phonological errors, while the concurrent visuospatial WM task prompted more non-phonological errors, matching the Italian phonology, but not the Italian orthography. In the ADHD group, the children proving better able to cope with a concurrent verbal WM load had a better spelling performance too. The ADHD and control groups had a similar handwriting speed, but the former group's writing quality was poorer. Our results suggest that WM supports writing skills, and that children with ADHD symptoms have general writing difficulties, but strength in coping with concurrent verbal information may support their spelling performance.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Escritura Manual , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Destreza Motora , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor
18.
J Atten Disord ; 23(3): 282-292, 2019 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27581244

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The hypothesis behind this study was that trained teachers using cooperative learning procedures with children in their classroom (aged from 6 to 10 years) can influence the social skills of children with ADHD symptoms and their acceptance by their peers. METHOD: The study involved 30 children with ADHD symptoms attending 12 different classes, where cooperative learning was adopted in some, and standard practices in others. ADHD children's symptoms, social skills, and cooperative behavior were assessed by means of a teacher's questionnaire, and the social preferences of the children in their class were collected. RESULTS: Changes emerged in teachers' assessments of the children's cooperative behavior in the experimental classes. Improvements in the sociometric status of children with ADHD symptoms were only seen in the cooperative learning classes. CONCLUSION: These results show the importance of well-structured intervention in classes that include children with ADHD symptoms. Implications of these findings for future intervention are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Aprendizaje , Grupo Paritario , Conducta Social , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Niño , Conducta Cooperativa , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Técnicas Sociométricas , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
19.
J Learn Disabil ; 52(2): 99-108, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29985098

RESUMEN

Successful reading demands the ability to combine visual-phonological information into a single representation and is associated with an efficient short-term memory. Reading disability may consequently involve an impaired working memory binding of visual and phonological information. The present study proposes two span tasks for assessing visual-phonological working memory binding. The tasks involved memorizing cross-modal associations between nonsense figures and nonwords, and they were administered, with other working memory measures, to children with and without a reading disability. The tasks required recognizing which figure was associated with a given nonword and recalling which nonword was associated with a given figure. Children with a reading disability had a similar significant deficit in both cross-modal binding tasks when compared with the control children, and the difference remained significant even after controlling for other verbal and nonverbal working memory measures. The cross-modal binding tasks described here seem to capture a core aspect of working memory associated with reading and may be a useful procedure for assessing reading disabilities.


Asunto(s)
Asociación , Dislexia/fisiopatología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicolingüística
20.
Dyslexia ; 25(1): 57-68, 2019 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30411447

RESUMEN

Research has shown that individuals with dyslexia have difficulty in reading not only in their native language but also in a second language (L2). The considered L2, however, has always been a language acquired through exposure to both written and oral forms. The present study examines the case of Italian adolescents reading in Latin as an L2, which is the special case of a dead language with very limited use of orality. As the learning of Latin is mainly based on the acquisition of grammar, this study also examined the relationship between grammatical proficiency and reading ability in Latin. Results suggested that, compared with control peers, students with dyslexia had difficulty in reading words and non-words in Latin. Interestingly, in spite of Latin being learnt mainly through written language, the extent of their difficulty was no larger than they encountered when reading in their native language. Also, despite the fact that students with dyslexia showed relatively less severe difficulties with Latin grammar (as compared to reading), this did not support them when reading Latin words, unlike typical readers. The theoretical and educational implications of these results are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia/psicología , Lenguaje , Aprendizaje , Lectura , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Lingüística , Masculino , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...