Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 10 de 10
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
2.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33499424

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: A large majority of primary school pupils fail to achieve 30-min of daily, in-school moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA). The aim of this study was to investigate MVPA accumulation and subject frequency during academic lesson segments and the broader segmented school day. METHODS: 122 children (42.6% boys; 9.9 ± 0.3 years) from six primary schools in North East England, wore uniaxial accelerometers for eight consecutive days. Subject frequency was assessed by teacher diaries. Multilevel models (children nested within schools) examined significant predictors of MVPA across each school-day segment (lesson one, break, lesson two, lunch, lesson three). RESULTS: Pupils averaged 18.33 ± 8.34 min of in-school MVPA, and 90.2% failed to achieve the in-school 30-min MVPA threshold. Across all school-day segments, MVPA accumulation was typically influenced at the individual level. Lessons one and two-dominated by maths and English-were less active than lesson three. Break and lunch were the most active segments. CONCLUSION: This study breaks new ground, revealing that MVPA accumulation and subject frequency varies greatly during different academic lessons. Morning lessons were dominated by the inactive delivery of maths and English, whereas afternoon lessons involved a greater array of subject delivery that resulted in marginally higher levels of MVPA.


Asunto(s)
Ejercicio Físico , Educación y Entrenamiento Físico , Niño , Inglaterra , Humanos , Masculino , Instituciones Académicas , Conducta Sedentaria
3.
Pediatr Exerc Sci ; 31(3): 356-362, 2019 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30612529

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To assess physical activity outcomes of a pedometer-based physically active learning (PAL) intervention in primary school children. METHODS: Six paired schools were randomly allocated to either a 6-week teacher-led pedometer-based physically active learning intervention or a control (n = 154, female = 60%, age = 9.9 [0.3] y). Accelerometers assessed total daily sedentary time, light physical activity (LPA), and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA). Preintervention mean daily MVPA minutes grouped participants as Low Active (<45 min/d) and High Active (≥45 min/d). RESULTS: From the final sample size, the intervention (n = 52) significantly improved LPA versus control (n = 31, P = .04), by reducing sedentary time. More intervention (+10%) than control (+3%) pupils met the 60 minutes per day guidelines. In both intervention subgroups, pupils spent less time in LPA (P < .05) versus control. The greatest nonsignificant increase was found in the Low Active pupils MVPA levels. CONCLUSIONS: Improvements in LPA were statistically significant in the intervention versus control group. In subgroup analysis, Low Active pupils in the intervention showed the greatest beneficial effects and the Most Active pupils may have replaced MVPA and sedentary time with LPA. The intervention group housed clusters of pupils showing variable responsiveness, justifying routine examination of subgroup variability in future studies.


Asunto(s)
Acelerometría , Ejercicio Físico , Monitores de Ejercicio , Aprendizaje Basado en Problemas , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Educación y Entrenamiento Físico , Instituciones Académicas , Conducta Sedentaria
4.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 371(1690)2016 Mar 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26926281

RESUMEN

Research indicates that in experimental settings, young children of 3-7 years old are unlikely to devise a simple tool to solve a problem. This series of exploratory studies done in museums in the US and UK explores how environment and ownership of materials may improve children's ability and inclination for (i) tool material selection and (ii) innovation. The first study takes place in a children's museum, an environment where children can use tools and materials freely. We replicated a tool innovation task in this environment and found that while 3-4 year olds showed the predicted low levels of innovation rates, 4-7 year olds showed higher rates of innovation than the younger children and than reported in prior studies. The second study explores the effect of whether the experimental materials are owned by the experimenter or the child on tool selection and innovation. Results showed that 5-6 year olds and 6-7 year olds were more likely to select tool material they owned compared to tool material owned by the experimenter, although ownership had no effect on tool innovation. We argue that learning environments supporting tool exploration and invention and conveying ownership over materials may encourage successful tool innovation at earlier ages.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil/psicología , Ambiente , Propiedad , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 7: 789, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24312043

RESUMEN

The aim of the current study was to investigate the effect of breakfast consumption on cognitive performance and mood in adolescents, and any interaction that breakfast consumption might have with cognitive load. The rationale for this approach was that the beneficial effects of any intervention with regard to cognitive function may be more readily apparent when more demands are placed on the system. Furthermore, as skipping breakfast is particularly prevalent within this age group, thus, we focused on adolescents who habitually skip breakfast. Cognitive load was modulated by varying the level of difficulty of a series of cognitive tasks tapping memory, attention, and executive functions. Mood measured with Bond-Lader scales (1974) as well as measures of thirst, hunger, and satiety were recorded at each test session both at baseline and after the completion of each test battery. Forty adolescents (mean age = 14:2) participated in this within-subjects design study. According to treatment, all participants were tested before and after the intake of a low Glycaemic index breakfast (i.e., a 35 g portion of AllBran and 125 ml semi-skimmed milk) and before and after no breakfast consumption. Assessment time had two levels: 8.00 am (baseline) and 10.45 am. The orders of cognitive load tasks were counterbalanced. Overall it appeared that following breakfast participants felt more alert, satiated, and content. Following breakfast consumption, there was evidence for improved cognitive performance across the school morning compared to breakfast omission in some tasks (e.g., Hard Word Recall, Serial 3's and Serial 7's). However, whilst participants performance on the hard version of each cognitive task was significantly poorer compared to the corresponding easy version, there was limited evidence to support the hypothesis that the effect of breakfast was greater in the more demanding versions of the tasks.

6.
Child Dev ; 84(5): 1519-25, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23480031

RESUMEN

It is impossible to perceive who owns an object; this must be inferred. One way that children make such inferences is through a first possession bias--when two agents each use an object, children judge the object belongs to the one who used it first. Two experiments show that this bias does not result from children directly inferring ownership from first possession; the experiments instead support an alternative account according to which the first possession bias reflects children's historical reasoning. In Experiment 1, eighty-five 3- to 5-year-olds only based inferences on first possession when it was informative about the past. In Experiment 2, thirty-two 5-year-olds based ownership judgments on testimony about past contact, while disregarding testimony about future contact.


Asunto(s)
Formación de Concepto/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Propiedad , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepción/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología
7.
New Dir Child Adolesc Dev ; 2011(132): 79-89, 2011.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21671343

RESUMEN

Appropriate behavior in relation to an object often requires judging whether it is owned and, if so, by whom. The authors propose accounts of how people make these judgments. Our central claim is that both judgments often involve making inferences about object history. In judging whether objects are owned, people may assume that artifacts (e.g., chairs) are owned and that natural objects (e.g., pinecones) are not. However, people may override these assumptions by inferring the history of intentional acts made in relation to objects. In judging who owns an object, people may often consider which person likely possessed the object in the past--such reasoning may be responsible for people's bias to assume that the first person known to possess an object is its owner.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Juicio , Propiedad , Niño , Conducta Infantil , Preescolar , Cognición , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental , Apego a Objetos , Solución de Problemas , Psicología Infantil
8.
Cognition ; 110(2): 260-4, 2009 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19101667

RESUMEN

Two studies investigated the relative importance of information about intended design and current use on judgments about the function (Experiment 1) or category (Experiment 2) of novel artifacts in preschool children and adults. Adults assigned function and name on the basis of information about design across all conditions, while children's decisions about function dissociated from decisions about category. Function judgments (in both 4 and 6-year-olds) were neutral between design and current use, both when the current use was idiosyncratic (e.g. performed by just one agent) and conventional (performed by many people; Experiment 1). By contrast, where category judgments were required for the very same objects (Experiment 2), children named according to design intentions - but only if the alternate function was idiosyncratic. Judging function and assigning category are thus cognitive tasks that draw on different information across development, a fact that should be captured by theories of developing artifact concept structure.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Preescolar , Clasificación , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
9.
Br J Nutr ; 100(5): 1086-96, 2008 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18507881

RESUMEN

Adequate levels of vitamins and minerals are essential for optimal neural functioning. A high proportion of individuals, including children, suffer from deficiencies in one or more vitamins or minerals. This study investigated whether daily supplementation with vitamins/minerals could modulate cognitive performance and mood in healthy children. In this randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel groups investigation, eighty-one healthy children aged from 8 to 14 years underwent laboratory assessments of their cognitive performance and mood pre-dose and at 1 and 3 h post-dose on the first and last days of 12 weeks' supplementation with a commercially available vitamins/mineral product (Pharmaton Kiddi). Interim assessments were also completed at home after 4 and 8 weeks at 3 h post-dose. Each assessment comprised completion of a cognitive battery, delivered over the Internet, which included tasks assessing mood and the speed and accuracy of attention and aspects of memory (secondary, semantic and spatial working memory). The vitamin/mineral group performed more accurately on two attention tasks: 'Arrows' choice reaction time task at 4 and 8 weeks; 'Arrow Flankers' choice reaction time task at 4, 8 and 12 weeks. A single task outcome (Picture Recognition errors) evinced significant decrements at 12 weeks. Mood was not modulated in any interpretable manner. Whilst it is possible that the significant improvements following treatment were due to non-significant numerical differences in performance at baseline, these results would seem to suggest that vitamin/mineral supplementation has the potential to improve brain function in healthy children. This proposition requires further investigation.


Asunto(s)
Afecto/efectos de los fármacos , Cognición/efectos de los fármacos , Minerales/administración & dosificación , Vitaminas/administración & dosificación , Adolescente , Análisis de Varianza , Atención/efectos de los fármacos , Niño , Dieta , Suplementos Dietéticos , Método Doble Ciego , Combinación de Medicamentos , Femenino , Humanos , Internet , Masculino , Memoria/efectos de los fármacos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
10.
Bioelectromagnetics ; 27(3): 215-20, 2006 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16304701

RESUMEN

Recent studies have indicated that acute exposure to low level radiofrequency (RF) electromagnetic fields generated by mobile phones affects human cognition. However, the relatively small samples used, in addition to methodological problems, make the outcomes of these studies difficult to interpret. In our study we tested a large sample of volunteers (168) using a series of cognitive tasks apparently sensitive to RF exposure (a simple reaction task, a vigilance task, and a subtraction task). Participants performed those tasks twice, in two different sessions. In one session they were exposed to RFs, with half of subjects exposed to GSM signals and the other half exposed to CW signals, while in the other session they were exposed to sham signals. No significant effects of RF exposure on performance for either GSM or CW were found, independent of whether the phone was positioned on the left or on the right side.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Atención/efectos de la radiación , Teléfono Celular , Exposición a Riesgos Ambientales , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/efectos de la radiación , Ondas de Radio , Adolescente , Adulto , Cognición/fisiología , Cognición/efectos de la radiación , Relación Dosis-Respuesta en la Radiación , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Dosis de Radiación , Factores de Tiempo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...