Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 35
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Neurobiol Aging ; 141: 129-139, 2024 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38909430

RESUMEN

White matter hyperintensities (WMH) are associated with cortical thinning. Although they are primarily detected in older participants, these lesions can appear in younger and midlife individuals. Here, we tested whether WMH are associated with cortical thinning in relatively younger (26-50 years) and relatively older (58-84) participants who were free of dementia, and how these associations are moderated by WMH localization. WMH were automatically quantified and categorized according to the localization of three classes of white matter tracts: association, commissural and projection fibers. Mediation analyses were used to infer whether differences in cortical thickness between younger and older participants were explained by WMH. Our results revealed that total WMH explained between 20.6 % and 65.5 % of the effect of age on cortical thickness in AD-signature regions including the lateral temporal lobes and supramarginal gyrus, among others. This mediation was slightly stronger for projection WMH, although it was still significant for association and commissural WMH. These results suggest that there is an interplay between vascular and AD causes of cognitive impairment that starts at younger ages.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Corteza Cerebral , Sustancia Blanca , Humanos , Sustancia Blanca/patología , Sustancia Blanca/diagnóstico por imagen , Anciano , Masculino , Femenino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Adulto , Envejecimiento/patología , Corteza Cerebral/patología , Corteza Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagen , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/patología , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagen , Disfunción Cognitiva/patología , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico por imagen
2.
Front Aging Neurosci ; 15: 1152582, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37151844

RESUMEN

Introduction: Aging negatively impacts the ability to rapidly and successfully switch between two or more tasks that have different rules or objectives. However, previous work has shown that the context impacts the extent of this age-related impairment: while there is relative age-related invariance when participants must rapidly switch back and forth between two simple tasks (often called "switch costs"), age-related differences emerge when the contexts changes from one in which only one task must be performed to one in which multiple tasks must be performed, but a trial-level switch is not required (e.g., task repeat trials within dual task blocks, often called "mixing costs"). Here, we explored these two kinds of costs behaviorally, and also investigated the neural correlates of these effects. Methods: Seventy-one younger adults and 175 older adults completed a task-switching experiment while they underwent fMRI brain imaging. We investigated the impact of age on behavioral performance and neural activity considering two types of potential costs: switch costs (dual-task switch trials minus dual-task non-switch trials), and mixing costs (dual-task non-switch minus single-task trials). Results: We replicated previous behavioral findings, with greater age associated with mixing, but not switch costs. Neurally, we found age-related compensatory activations for switch costs in the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex, pars opercularis, superior temporal gyrus, and the posterior and anterior cingulate, but age-related under recruitment for mixing costs in fronto-parietal areas including the supramarginal gyrus and pre and supplemental motor areas. Discussion: These results suggest an age-based dissociation between executive components that contribute to task switching.

3.
Psychol Aging ; 38(1): 30-48, 2023 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36701535

RESUMEN

People are generally able to selectively attend and remember high-value over low-value information. Here, we investigated whether young and older adults would display typical value-based memory selectivity effects for to-be-learned item-value associations when goal-directed information about the meaning of associated values was presented before and after encoding. In two experiments, both young and older adults were presented with one (Experiment 1) or multiple (Experiment 2) lists of words that were arbitrarily paired with different numerical values (e.g., "door-8") or font colors (e.g., "door" presented in red), which indicated each word's value. In Experiment 1, participants were told that the numerical value indicated the relative importance of each item either before they studied the list (preencoding), after they studied it (postencoding), or not at all (no value control instructions). Older adults were significantly more selective in the preencoding condition relative to the other conditions, whereas younger adults were not selective in any condition on this single-list (numerical) value task of Experiment 1. In Experiment 2, young and older adults were tested on four additional lists of both pre- and postencoding trials each after studying and recalling four lists of words without any value instructions. Results from Experiment 2 revealed that both young and older adults selectively prioritized high-value words on the preencoding trials, but not on postencoding trials, on this color-based categorical (low-medium-high) value task. The present study highlights a critical role of goal-directed knowledge of value-based instructions prior to encoding to facilitate typically observed value-directed memory selectivity for important information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Objetivos , Humanos , Anciano , Recuerdo Mental , Aprendizaje , Motivación
4.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 152(2): 464-482, 2023 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36048057

RESUMEN

In 10 experiments, we investigated the relations among curiosity and people's confidence in their answers to general information questions after receiving different kinds of feedback: yes/no feedback, true or false informational feedback under uncertainty, or no feedback. The results showed that when people had given a correct answer, yes/no feedback resulted in a near complete loss of curiosity. Upon learning they had made an error via yes/no feedback, curiosity increased, especially for high-confidence errors. When people were given true feedback under uncertainty (they were given the correct answer but were not told that it was correct), curiosity increased for high-confidence errors but was unchanged for correct responses. In contrast, when people were given false feedback under uncertainty, curiosity increased for high-confidence correct responses but was unchanged for errors. These results, taken as a whole, are consistent with the region of proximal learning model which proposes that while curiosity is minimal when people are completely certain that they know the answer, it is maximal when people believe that they almost know. Manipulations that drew participants toward this region of "almost knowing" resulted in increased curiosity. A serendipitous result was the finding (replicated four times in this study) that when no feedback was given, people were more curious about high-confidence errors than they were about equally high-confidence correct answers. It was as if they had some knowledge, tapped selectively by their feelings of curiosity, that there was something special (and possibly amiss) about high-confidence errors. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Conducta Exploratoria , Retroalimentación Psicológica , Humanos , Conducta Exploratoria/fisiología , Retroalimentación Psicológica/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Incertidumbre , Emociones
5.
Front Psychol ; 14: 1296662, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38314250

RESUMEN

Objectives: The Directed Forgetting paradigm has proven to be a powerful tool to explore motivated forgetting in the lab. Past work has shown that older adults are less able to intentionally suppress information from memory relative to younger adults, which is often attributed to deficits in inhibitory abilities. Instructions in traditional Directed Forgetting tasks contain terms that may elicit stereotype threat in older adults, which may negatively impact memory. Here, we tested whether the instructions in a Directed Forgetting task affected older adults' ability to appropriately control the contents of memory. Methods: In two experiments that differed in the number of words presented (30 vs. 48 items), younger and older adults were randomized into one of four crossed Conditions of a Directed Forgetting task. At encoding, participants were either instructed to remember/ forget items, or to think about/not think about items. At test, they were either asked whether the memory probe was old or new, or whether they had seen it before (yes/no). Each experiment contained data from 100 younger (18- 40 years) and 98 older (60+ years) adults, with ~25 participants per Condition. All participants were recruited from Prolific and tested online. Results: In neither Experiment 1 nor Experiment 2 did we find evidence of a stereotype threat effect, or age-related effects of directed forgetting. We did find that performance for to-be-forgotten items was worse in conditions with encoding instructions that contained words that might trigger stereotype threat relative to conditions that did not contain such words: when explicitly told to forget items, both older and younger adults forgot more items than did participants who were cued to not think about the words and put them out of mind. However, we found no such difference across the two different remember instructions: regardless of whether participants were told to remember or to think about items, recognition memory for to be retained items was high. The pattern of results across the two experiments was similar, except, not surprisingly, participants performed worse in Experiment 2 than Experiment 1. Interestingly, we found that higher accuracy for to be remembered items was associated with a more positive outlook of one's own memory relative to others. Discussion: These results suggest that directed forgetting may not always be impaired in older adults.

6.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 1955, 2022 02 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35121804

RESUMEN

White matter hyperintensities (WMH) are a key hallmark of subclinical cerebrovascular disease and are known to impair cognition. Here, we parcellated WMH using a novel system that segments WMH based on both lobar regions and distance from the ventricles, dividing the brain into a coordinate system composed of 36 distinct parcels ('bullseye' parcellation), and then investigated the effect of distribution on cognition using two different analytic approaches. Data from a well characterized sample of healthy older adults (58 to 84 years) who were free of dementia were included. Cognition was evaluated using 12 computerized tasks, factored onto 4 indices representing episodic memory, speed of processing, fluid reasoning and vocabulary. We first assessed the distribution of WMH according to the bullseye parcellation and tested the relationship between WMH parcellations and performance across the four cognitive domains. Then, we used a data-driven approach to derive latent variables within the WMH distribution, and tested the relation between these latent components and cognitive function. We observed that different, well-defined cognitive constructs mapped to specific WMH distributions. Speed of processing was correlated with WMH in the frontal lobe, while in the case of episodic memory, the relationship was more ubiquitous, involving most of the parcellations. A principal components analysis revealed that the 36 bullseye regions factored onto 3 latent components representing the natural aggrupation of WMH: fronto-parietal periventricular (WMH principally in the frontal and parietal lobes and basal ganglia, especially in the periventricular region); occipital; and temporal and juxtacortical WMH (involving WMH in the temporal lobe, and at the juxtacortical region from frontal and parietal lobes). We found that fronto-parietal periventricular and temporal & juxtacortical WMH were independently associated with speed of processing and episodic memory, respectively. These results indicate that different cognitive impairment phenotypes might present with specific WMH distributions. Additionally, our study encourages future research to consider WMH classifications using parcellations systems other than periventricular and deep localizations.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Cognición , Disfunción Cognitiva/etiología , Leucoencefalopatías/complicaciones , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Sustancia Blanca/diagnóstico por imagen , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Análisis por Conglomerados , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Leucoencefalopatías/diagnóstico por imagen , Leucoencefalopatías/fisiopatología , Masculino , Memoria Episódica , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Análisis de Componente Principal , Vocabulario , Sustancia Blanca/fisiopatología
7.
Front Psychol ; 13: 1020915, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36825240

RESUMEN

Background: Cognitive inhibition is among the executive functions that decline early in the course of normal aging. Failures to be able to inhibit irrelevant information from memory may represent an essential factor of age-associated memory impairment. While a variety of elaborate behavioral tasks have been developed that presumably all index memory inhibition, the extent to which these different tasks measure the same underlying cognitive construct that declines with age has not been well explored. Methods: In the current study, 100 and 75 cognitively healthy younger (n = 71; age = 30.7 ± 5.4 years, 56.7% female) and older (n = 104, age = 69.3 ± 5.9 years, 66.2% female) adults with equivalent educational attainment performed three computer-based memory inhibition tasks: the Retrieval Induced Forgetting task, the Suppress task, and the Directed Forgetting task. We conducted a principal component analysis using scores derived from different components of these tasks to explore whether and how the tasks relate to one another. We further investigated how age, sex and education, along with, in a subsample of the participants, a neuropsychological measure of episodic memory, impacted both the task scores individually, and the principal components derived from the exploratory analysis. Results: We identified 3 distinct sources of variability which represent potentially independent cognitive processes: memory retrieval facilitation, and two memory inhibition processes that distinguished themselves by the degree of volitional initiation of memory suppression. Only the memory retrieval component correlated with a neuropsychologically-derived episodic memory score, and both memory inhibition principal components were age dependent. Conclusion: Our findings provide support for a distinction in memory suppression processes between those 'instructed' to be performed and those which happen without explicit instruction. This distinction adds nuance to the dichotomous classification of controlled vs. automatic inhibitory mechanisms, which have been shown in previous work to vary as a function of the degree of frontal involvement. Our findings further demonstrate that while both of these measures of inhibition were affected by age, the episodic memory component was not, suggesting that inhibitory impairments may precede memory deficits in healthy aging.

8.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 15: 695416, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34512283

RESUMEN

Background: The current pilot study was designed to examine the association between hippocampal γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) concentration and episodic memory in older individuals, as well as the impact of two major risk factors for Alzheimer's disease (AD)-female sex and Apolipoprotein ε4 (ApoE ε4) genotype-on this relationship. Methods: Twenty healthy, community-dwelling individuals aged 50-71 (11 women) took part in the study. Episodic memory was evaluated using a Directed Forgetting task, and GABA+ was measured in the right hippocampus using a Mescher-Garwood point-resolved magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) sequence. Multiple linear regression models were used to quantify the relationship between episodic memory, GABA+, ApoE ɛ4, and sex, controlling for age and education. Results: While GABA+ did not interact with ApoE ɛ4 carrier status to influence episodic memory (p = 0.757), the relationship between GABA+ and episodic memory was moderated by sex: lower GABA+ predicted worse memory in women such that, for each standard deviation decrease in GABA+ concentration, memory scores were reduced by 11% (p = 0.001). Conclusions: This pilot study suggests that sex, but not ApoE ɛ4 genotype, moderates the relationship between hippocampal GABA+ and episodic memory, such that women with lower GABA+ concentration show worse memory performance. These findings, which must be interpreted with caution given the small sample size, may serve as a starting point for larger studies using multimodal neuroimaging to understand the contributions of GABA metabolism to age-related memory decline.

9.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 76(10): 2013-2022, 2021 11 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34232279

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: Declines in the ability to inhibit information, and the consequences to memory of unsuccessful inhibition, have been frequently reported to increase with age. However, few studies have investigated whether sex moderates such effects. Here, we examined whether inhibitory ability may vary as a function of age and sex, and the interaction between these two factors. METHOD: 202 older (mean age = 69.40 years) and younger (mean age =30.59 years) participants who had equivalent educational attainment and self-reported health completed 2 tasks that varied only in the time point at which inhibition should occur: either prior to, or after, encoding. RESULTS: While we did not find evidence for age or sex differences in inhibitory processes when information needed to be inhibited prior to encoding, when encoded information being actively held in working memory needed to be suppressed, we found that older women were particularly impaired relative to both younger women and men of either age group. DISCUSSION: These results provide further support for the presence of memorial inhibitory deficits in older age, but add nuance by implicating biological sex as an important mediator in this relationship, with it more difficult for older women to inhibit what was once relevant in memory.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Inhibición Proactiva , Inhibición Reactiva , Autocontrol/psicología , Factores Sexuales , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Vida Independiente , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción
10.
Neurobiol Aging ; 106: 95-102, 2021 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34265506

RESUMEN

Evidence suggests that older adults have difficulty relative to younger adults in forgetting irrelevant information. Here we sought to understand the physical basis of this deficit by investigating the relationship between cortical thickness and intentional forgetting, using an item-method directed forgetting task. We tested younger (n = 44) and older (n = 54) adults' memories for words that they were instructed to either remember or to forget, and then extracted cortical thickness values from brain regions previously shown, using functional neuroimaging, to be associated with memory suppression, including the right inferior frontal gyrus, the right postcentral gyrus and the left superior/middle frontal gyrus. Results from a parallel mediation model indicated that variations in cortical thickness in the right inferior frontal gyrus, but not the right postcentral gyrus or left superior/middle frontal gyrus, partially explained age-related differences in directed forgetting: older adults with thinner cortices in this area showed worse forgetting ability. This is the first study to explore how neuromorphological differences affect the ability to intentionally suppress items in memory. The results suggest that age-related differences in directed forgetting may be partly driven by cortical thickness in a brain structure known to be functionally involved in directed forgetting, and inhibitory control more broadly, supporting a contribution of deficient inhibition to this phenomenon.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/patología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/patología , Memoria/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Corteza Prefrontal/patología , Anciano , Corteza Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Neuroimagen , Corteza Prefrontal/diagnóstico por imagen
11.
Semin Cell Dev Biol ; 116: 146-159, 2021 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33573856

RESUMEN

In this review, we focus on the potential role of the γ-aminobutyric acidergic (GABAergic) system in age-related episodic memory impairments in humans, with a particular focus on Alzheimer's disease (AD). Well-established animal models have shown that GABA plays a central role in regulating and synchronizing neuronal signaling in the hippocampus, a brain area critical for episodic memory that undergoes early and significant morphologic and functional changes in the course of AD. Neuroimaging research in humans has documented hyperactivity in the hippocampus and losses of resting state functional connectivity in the Default Mode Network, a network that itself prominently includes the hippocampus-presaging episodic memory decline in individuals at-risk for AD. Apolipoprotein ε4, the highest genetic risk factor for AD, is associated with GABAergic dysfunction in animal models, and episodic memory impairments in humans. In combination, these findings suggest that GABA may be the linchpin in a complex system of factors that eventually leads to the principal clinical hallmark of AD: episodic memory loss. Here, we will review the current state of literature supporting this hypothesis. First, we will focus on the molecular and cellular basis of the GABAergic system and its role in memory and cognition. Next, we report the evidence of GABA dysregulations in AD and normal aging, both in animal models and human studies. Finally, we outline a model of GABAergic dysfunction based on the results of functional neuroimaging studies in humans, which have shown hippocampal hyperactivity to episodic memory tasks concurrent with and even preceding AD diagnosis, along with factors that may modulate this association.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/genética , Neuronas GABAérgicas/patología , Trastornos de la Memoria/genética , Animales , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Humanos , Ratones , Redes Neurales de la Computación
12.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 14: 12, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32174818

RESUMEN

[This corrects the article DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00449.].

13.
PLoS One ; 15(2): e0228167, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32040518

RESUMEN

A key challenge in the field of cognitive neuroscience is to identify discriminable cognitive functions, and then map these functions to brain activity. In the current study, we set out to explore the relationships between performance arising from different cognitive tasks thought to tap different domains of cognition, and then to test whether these distinct latent cognitive abilities also are subserved by corresponding "latent" brain substrates. To this end, we tested a large sample of adults under the age of 40 on twelve cognitive tasks as they underwent fMRI scanning. Exploratory factor analysis revealed 4-factor model, dissociating tasks into processes corresponding to episodic memory retrieval, reasoning, speed of processing and vocabulary. An analysis of the topographic covariance patterns of the BOLD-response acquired during each task similarity also converged on four neural networks that corresponded to the 4 latent factors. These results suggest that distinct ontologies of cognition are subserved by corresponding distinct neural networks.


Asunto(s)
Ontologías Biológicas , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición , Modelos Neurológicos , Adulto , Conducta/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Análisis de Clases Latentes , Masculino , Adulto Joven
14.
Curr Opin Behav Sci ; 35: 40-47, 2020 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33709011

RESUMEN

We propose a framework for understanding epistemic curiosity as a metacognitive feeling state that is related to the individual's Region of Proximal Learning (RPL), an adaptive mental space where we feel we are on the verge of knowing or understanding. First, we review several historical views, contrasting the RPL perspective with alternative views of curiosity. Second, we detail the processes, conditions, and outcomes within the RPL framework which are proposed to be related to curiosity. Finally, we review several lines of evidence relevant to the relation between RPL and curiosity. These include (1) differences in the conditions under which experts and novices mind wander, (2) experiments investigating people's choices of whether to study materials for which they have high versus low feelings of knowing, (3) results related to people's engagement with corrections to errors made with high confidence, and (4) curiosity, attention, and learning data related to the tip-of-the-tongue state.

15.
Neuropsychologia ; 133: 107180, 2019 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31473197

RESUMEN

Episodic memory impairment is the hallmark symptom of Alzheimer's Disease (AD). However, episodic memory has also been shown to decline across the lifespan. Here, we investigated whether episodic memory is differentially affected relative to other cognitive abilities before old age, and whether being an Apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 carrier -a genetic risk factor for developing AD-exacerbates any such impairments. We used general linear models to test for performance differences within 4 composite measures of cognition - episodic memory, semantic memory, speed of processing, and fluid reasoning-- as a function of age group (young, Mage = 30.21 vs. middle-aged, Mage = 50.84) and APOE-ε4 genotype status (ε4+ vs. ε4-). We replicated findings of age-related reductions in episodic memory, speed of processing, and fluid reasoning, and age-related increases in semantic memory. However, we also found that APOE genotype status moderated the age-related declines in episodic memory: APOE-ε4+ middle-aged adults exhibited impairments relative to both APOE-ε4- middle-aged participants, and APOE-ε4+ younger adults. These results suggest specific and dynamic alterations to episodic memory as a function of APOE allelic variation and age.


Asunto(s)
Apolipoproteína E4/genética , Cognición/fisiología , Envejecimiento Cognitivo/fisiología , Memoria Episódica , Memoria/fisiología , Adulto , Alelos , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/genética , Femenino , Genotipo , Humanos , Modelos Lineales , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
16.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 4(1): 4, 2019 Feb 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30758685

RESUMEN

In five experiments, we examined the conditions under which participants remembered true and false information given as feedback. Participants answered general information questions, expressed their confidence in the correctness of their answers, and were given true or false feedback. In all five experiments, participants hypercorrected when they had made a mistake; that is, they remembered better the correct feedback to errors made with high compared to low confidence. However, in none of the experiments did participants hyper'correct' when false feedback followed an initially correct response. Telling people whether the feedback was right or wrong made little difference, suggesting that people already knew whether the feedback was true or false and differentially encoded the true feedback compared to the false feedback. An exception occurred when false feedback followed an error: participants hyper'corrected' to this false feedback, suggesting that when people are wrong initially, they are susceptible to further incorrect information. These results indicate that people have some kind of privileged access to whether their answers are right or wrong, above and beyond their confidence ratings, and that they behave differently when trying to remember new "corrective" information depending upon whether they, themselves, were right or wrong initially. The likely source of this additional information is knowledge about the truth of the feedback, which they rapidly process and use to modulate memory encoding.

17.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 13: 449, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31920603

RESUMEN

Occupational activity represents a large percentage of people's daily activity and thus likely is as impactful for people's general and cognitive health as other lifestyle components such as leisure activity, sleep, diet, and exercise. Different occupations, however, require different skills, abilities, activities, credentials, work styles, etc., constituting a rich multidimensional formative exposure with likely consequences for brain development over the lifespan. In the current study, we were interested in how different occupations with their different attributes relate to five variables: structural brain health, duration of early-life education, gender, IQ, and age, although the main focus was the relationship to brain health. To this end, we used the Occupation Information Network (O∗NET), which provides quantification of occupations along 246 items. Occupational patterns with different loadings for these 246 items were derived from 277 community-dwelling adults, ranging in age from 40 to 80, based upon the five subject measures. We found significant patterns underlying four of our variables of interest, with gender and education predictably showing the most numerous and strongest associations, while brain health and intelligence showed weaker associations, and age did not manifest any associations. For the occupational pattern associated with brain health, we found mainly positive associations on items pertaining to rigorous problem-solving, leadership, responsibility, and information processing. We emphasize that the findings are correlational and cannot establish causation. Future extensions of this work will assess the influence of occupation on future cognitive brain status and cognitive performance.

18.
Neuroimage ; 172: 51-63, 2018 05 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29355766

RESUMEN

To better understand the impact of aging, along with other demographic and brain health variables, on the neural networks that support different aspects of cognitive performance, we applied a brute-force search technique based on Principal Components Analysis to derive 4 corresponding spatial covariance patterns (termed Reference Ability Neural Networks -RANNs) from a large sample of participants across the age range. 255 clinically healthy, community-dwelling adults, aged 20-77, underwent fMRI while performing 12 tasks, 3 tasks for each of the following cognitive reference abilities: Episodic Memory, Reasoning, Perceptual Speed, and Vocabulary. The derived RANNs (1) showed selective activation to their specific cognitive domain and (2) correlated with behavioral performance. Quasi out-of-sample replication with Monte-Carlo 5-fold cross validation was built into our approach, and all patterns indicated their corresponding reference ability and predicted performance in held-out data to a degree significantly greater than chance level. RANN-pattern expression for Episodic Memory, Reasoning and Vocabulary were associated selectively with age, while the pattern for Perceptual Speed showed no such age-related influences. For each participant we also looked at residual activity unaccounted for by the RANN-pattern derived for the cognitive reference ability. Higher residual activity was associated with poorer brain-structural health and older age, but -apart from Vocabulary-not with cognitive performance, indicating that older participants with worse brain-structural health might recruit alternative neural resources to maintain performance levels.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Cognición/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiopatología , Adulto , Anciano , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Longevidad/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
19.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 73(6): 954-963, 2018 08 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27633612

RESUMEN

Objectives: Recent work has identified different aspects of executive function that may underlie cognitive changes associated with age. The current study used a multifactorial design to investigate age sensitivity in the ability to shift between different task sets and the interaction of this ability with several specific aspects of executive control. Method: A large, well-characterized sample of younger (n = 40) and clinically healthy older (n = 51) adults completed a task switching paradigm in which 3 aspects of executive control were manipulated between subjects: a) sensorimotor demand (the number of distinct stimulus-response options); b) stimulus-level interference (i.e., flanker effects); and c) updating/monitoring (the frequency of task switches). Results: Unique age-related deficits were observed for different aspects of local task switching performance costs and updating/monitoring, but not for interference. Sensorimotor demand was also an important additional factor that interacted with task switching performance. Discussion: Our findings suggest that task switching, coupled with infrequent and unexpected transitions from one task set to another, in the context of high motoric demands, is particularly difficult for older adults.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva , Desempeño Psicomotor , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
20.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 73(4): 612-621, 2018 04 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26976135

RESUMEN

Objective: Declines in working memory are a ubiquitous finding within the cognitive-aging literature. A unitary inhibitory selection mechanism that serves to guide attention toward task-relevant information and resolve interference from task-irrelevant information has been proposed to underlie such deficits. However, inhibition can occur at multiple time points in the memory-processing stream. Here, we tested whether the time point at which inhibition occurs in the memory-processing stream affects age-related memory decline. Method: Clinically healthy younger (n = 23) and older (n = 22) adults performed two similar item-recognition working memory tasks. In one task, participants received an instruction cue telling them which words to attend to followed by a memory set, promoting perceptual inhibition at the time of encoding. In the other task, participants received the instruction cue after they received the memory set, fostering inhibition of items already in memory. Results: We found that older and younger adults differed in their ability to inhibit items both during encoding and when items had to be inhibited in memory but that these age differences were exaggerated when irrelevant information had to be inhibited from memory. These results provide insights into the mechanisms that support cognitive changes to memory processes in healthy aging.


Asunto(s)
Inhibición Psicológica , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tiempo de Reacción , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA