Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 7 de 7
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 125: 380-391, 2021 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33652021

RESUMEN

Time is a fundamental dimension of our perception of the world and is therefore of critical importance to the organization of human behavior. A corpus of work - including recent optogenetic evidence - implicates striatal dopamine as a crucial factor influencing the perception of time. Another stream of literature implicates dopamine in reward and motivation processes. However, these two domains of research have remained largely separated, despite neurobiological overlap and the apothegmatic notion that "time flies when you're having fun". This article constitutes a review of the literature linking time perception and reward, including neurobiological and behavioral studies. Together, these provide compelling support for the idea that time perception and reward processing interact via a common dopaminergic mechanism.


Asunto(s)
Dopamina , Percepción del Tiempo , Cuerpo Estriado , Humanos , Motivación , Recompensa
2.
Front Psychol ; 12: 791835, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35250692

RESUMEN

Studies have shown that during social interaction a shared system underlies inferring one's own mental state, and the mental states of others - processes often referred to as mentalization. However, no validated assessment has been developed to measure second order mentalization (one's beliefs about how transparent one's thoughts are to others), or whether this capacity plays a significant role in social interaction. The current work presents a interactive mentalization theory, which divides these directional and second order aspects of mentalization, and investigates whether these constructs are measurable, stable, and meaningful in social interactions. We developed a 20-item, self-report interactive mentalization questionnaire (IMQ) in order to assess the different sub-components of mentalization: self-self, self-other, and other-self mentalization (Study 1). We then tested this scale on a large, online sample, and report convergent and discriminant validity in the form of correlations with other measures (Study 2), as well as correlations with social deception behaviors in real online interaction with Mturk studies (Study 3 and Study 4). These results validate the IMQ, and support the idea that these three factors can predict mentalization in social interaction.

3.
Nat Hum Behav ; 3(7): 702-708, 2019 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31110337

RESUMEN

Theoretical models distinguish between neural responses elicited by distal threats and those evoked by more immediate threats1-3. Specifically, slower 'cognitive' fear responses towards distal threats involve a network of brain regions including the ventral hippocampus (vHPC) and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), while immediate 'reactive' fear responses rely on regions such as the periaqueductal grey4,5. However, it is unclear how anxiety and its neural substrates relate to these distinct defensive survival circuits. We tested whether individual differences in trait anxiety would impact escape behaviour and neural responses to slow and fast attacking predators: conditions designed to evoke cognitive and reactive fear, respectively. Behaviourally, we found that trait anxiety was not related to escape decisions for fast threats, but individuals with higher trait anxiety escaped earlier during slow threats. Functional magnetic resonance imaging showed that when subjects faced slow threats, trait anxiety positively correlated with activity in the vHPC, mPFC, amygdala and insula. Furthermore, the strength of functional coupling between two components of the cognitive circuit-the vHPC and mPFC-was correlated with the degree of trait anxiety. This suggests that anxiety predominantly affects cognitive fear circuits that are involved in volitional strategic escape.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Reacción de Fuga/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Amígdala del Cerebelo/diagnóstico por imagen , Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiología , Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiopatología , Ansiedad/psicología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Corteza Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiopatología , Cognición/fisiología , Miedo , Femenino , Neuroimagen Funcional , Hipocampo/diagnóstico por imagen , Hipocampo/fisiología , Hipocampo/fisiopatología , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Modelos Teóricos , Sustancia Gris Periacueductal/diagnóstico por imagen , Sustancia Gris Periacueductal/fisiología , Sustancia Gris Periacueductal/fisiopatología , Personalidad , Corteza Prefrontal/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiopatología , Adulto Joven
4.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 4163, 2019 03 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30853713

RESUMEN

It is increasingly recognized that Alzheimer's disease (AD) exists before dementia is present and that shifts in amyloid beta occur long before clinical symptoms can be detected. Early detection of these molecular changes is a key aspect for the success of interventions aimed at slowing down rates of cognitive decline. Recent evidence indicates that of the two established methods for measuring amyloid, a decrease in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) amyloid ß1-42 (Aß1-42) may be an earlier indicator of Alzheimer's disease risk than measures of amyloid obtained from Positron Emission Tomography (PET). However, CSF collection is highly invasive and expensive. In contrast, blood collection is routinely performed, minimally invasive and cheap. In this work, we develop a blood-based signature that can provide a cheap and minimally invasive estimation of an individual's CSF amyloid status using a machine learning approach. We show that a Random Forest model derived from plasma analytes can accurately predict subjects as having abnormal (low) CSF Aß1-42 levels indicative of AD risk (0.84 AUC, 0.78 sensitivity, and 0.73 specificity). Refinement of the modeling indicates that only APOEε4 carrier status and four plasma analytes (CGA, Aß1-42, Eotaxin 3, APOE) are required to achieve a high level of accuracy. Furthermore, we show across an independent validation cohort that individuals with predicted abnormal CSF Aß1-42 levels transitioned to an AD diagnosis over 120 months significantly faster than those with predicted normal CSF Aß1-42 levels and that the resulting model also validates reasonably across PET Aß1-42 status (0.78 AUC). This is the first study to show that a machine learning approach, using plasma protein levels, age and APOEε4 carrier status, is able to predict CSF Aß1-42 status, the earliest risk indicator for AD, with high accuracy.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/sangre , Péptidos beta-Amiloides/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Apolipoproteínas E/sangre , Quimiocina CCL26/sangre , Cromogranina A/sangre , Fragmentos de Péptidos/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Péptidos beta-Amiloides/sangre , Biomarcadores/sangre , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Fragmentos de Péptidos/sangre , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas
5.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 43(11): 1925-1936, 2017 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28406689

RESUMEN

Human time perception can be influenced by contextual factors, such as the presence of reward. Yet, the exact nature of the relationship between time perception and reward has not been conclusively characterized. We implemented a novel experimental paradigm to measure estimations of time across a range of suprasecond intervals, during the anticipation and after the consumption of fruit juice, a physiologically relevant primary reward. We show that average time estimations were systematically affected by the consumption of reward, but not by the anticipation of reward. Compared with baseline estimations of time, reward consumption was associated with subsequent overproductions of time, and this effect increased for larger magnitudes of reward. Additional experiments demonstrated that the effect of consumption did not extend to a secondary reward (money), a tasteless, noncaloric primary reward (water), or a sweet, noncaloric reward (aspartame). However, a tasteless caloric reward (maltodexrin) did induce overproductions of time, although this effect did not scale with reward magnitude. These results suggest that the consumption of caloric primary rewards can alter time perception, which may be a psychophysiological mechanism by which organisms regulate homeostatic balance. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Ingestión de Energía , Recompensa , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Adulto , Aspartame , Agua Potable , Jugos de Frutas y Vegetales , Humanos , Polisacáridos , Adulto Joven
6.
Proc Biol Sci ; 284(1849)2017 02 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28228517

RESUMEN

Temporal persistence refers to an individual's capacity to wait for future rewards, while forgoing possible alternatives. This requires a trade-off between the potential value of delayed rewards and opportunity costs, and is relevant to many real-world decisions, such as dieting. Theoretical models have previously suggested that high monetary reward rates, or positive energy balance, may result in decreased temporal persistence. In our study, 50 fasted participants engaged in a temporal persistence task, incentivised with monetary rewards. In alternating blocks of this task, rewards were delivered at delays drawn randomly from distributions with either a lower or higher maximum reward rate. During some blocks participants received either a caloric drink or water. We used survival analysis to estimate participants' probability of quitting conditional on the delay distribution and the consumed liquid. Participants had a higher probability of quitting in blocks with the higher reward rate. Furthermore, participants who consumed the caloric drink had a higher probability of quitting than those who consumed water. Our results support the predictions from the theoretical models, and importantly, suggest that both higher monetary reward rates and physiologically relevant rewards can decrease temporal persistence, which is a crucial determinant for survival in many species.


Asunto(s)
Ingestión de Alimentos , Metabolismo Energético , Motivación/fisiología , Recompensa , Conducta de Elección , Alimentos , Humanos , Probabilidad
7.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 11: 1, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28174525

RESUMEN

Cardiac signals reflect the function of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) and have previously been associated with a range of self-regulatory behaviors such as emotion regulation and memory recall. It is unknown whether cardiac signals may also be associated with self-regulation in the temporal domain, in particular impulsivity. We assessed both decision impulsivity (temporal discounting, TD) and time perception impulsivity (duration reproduction, DR) in 120 participants while they underwent electrocardiography in order to test whether cardiac signals were related to these two aspects of impulsivity. We found that over the entire period of task performance, individuals with higher heart rates had a tendency toward lower discount rates, supporting previous research that has associated sympathetic responses with decreased impulsivity. We also found that low-frequency components of heart rate variability (HRV) were associated with a less accurate perception of time, suggesting that time perception may be modulated by ANS function. Overall, these findings constitute preliminary evidence that autonomic function plays an important role in both decision impulsivity and time perception.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...