Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
1.
Cortex ; 145: 273-284, 2021 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34775264

RESUMEN

Difficulty in acquiring object clitics (OCs), and the omission of OCs more specifically, is used as a marker of developmental language disorder (DLD) in children learning French. Research also suggests that OCs are a complex morphosyntactic property of French and, as such, they could be particularly taxing for working memory (WM) resources. In light of previous research, it seems more likely that difficulties with OCs could be a marker for atypical early language acquisition. Children receiving a cochlear implant (CI) represent a case of delayed language exposure linked to a lack of early auditory exposure. Few studies have investigated the production of clitics in children with CIs. Studies suggest that children with CIs make significantly more clitic omissions on tasks of clitic production than typically-developing (TD) children. To our knowledge, no study has looked at clitic comprehension in children with CIs learning French or at the relationship between WM and performance on tasks of OCs in this population. The present study aimed at examining the production and comprehension of OCs, as well as the relationship between clitic omission and WM, in both typically-developing monolinguals children and CI recipients learning French. Children with CIs performed significantly lower than the control group on both comprehension and production tasks. Clitic omission was significantly related to WM, but only in the CI group suggesting a differential relationship between early auditory experience, WM, and language acquisition.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Niño , Comprensión , Humanos , Lenguaje , Pruebas del Lenguaje
2.
J Child Lang ; 48(2): 285-324, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32524936

RESUMEN

Pierce et al. (2017) have proposed that variations in the timing, quality and quantity of language input during the earliest stages of development are related to variations in the development of phonological working memory and, in turn, to later language learning outcomes. To examine this hypothesis, three groups of children who are at-risk for language learning were examined: children with cochlear implants (CI), children with developmental language disorder (DLD), and internationally-adopted (IA) children, Comparison groups of typically-developing monolingual (MON) children and second language (L2) learners were also included. All groups were acquiring French as a first or second language and were matched on age, gender, and socioeconomic status, as well as other group-specific factors; they were between 5;0-7;3 years of age at time of testing. The CI and DLD groups scored significantly more poorly on the memory measures than the other groups; while the IA and L2 groups did not differ from one another. While the IA group performed more poorly than the MON group, there was no difference between the L2 and MON groups. We also found differential developmental relationships between phonological memory and language among the groups of interest in comparison to the typically-developing MON and L2 groups supporting the hypothesis that language experiences early in life are consequential for language development because of their effects on the development of phonological memory.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Preescolar , Humanos , Lenguaje , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Lingüística , Memoria a Corto Plazo
3.
Dev Sci ; 20(1)2017 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26825571

RESUMEN

This study examined whether decoding and linguistic comprehension abilities, broadly defined by the Simple View of Reading, in grade 1 each uniquely predicted the grade 6 writing performance of English-speaking children (n = 76) who were educated bilingually in both English their first language and French, a second language. Prediction was made from (1) English to English; (2) French to French; and (3) English to French. Results showed that both decoding and linguistic comprehension scores predicted writing accuracy but rarely predicted persuasive writing. Within the linguistic comprehension cluster of tests, Formulating Sentences was a strong consistent within- and between-language predictor of writing accuracy. In practical terms, the present results indicate that early screening for later writing ability using measures of sentence formulation early in students' schooling, in their L1 or L2, can provide greatest predictive power and allow teachers to differentiate instruction in the primary grades. Theoretically, the present results argue that there are correlations between reading-related abilities and writing abilities not only within the same language but also across languages, adding to the growing body of evidence for facilitative cross-linguistic relationships between bilinguals' developing languages.


Asunto(s)
Multilingüismo , Escritura/normas , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Lingüística , Masculino
4.
J Commun Disord ; 63: 47-62, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27814797

RESUMEN

This study aimed to gather information from school- and clinic-based professionals about their practices and opinions pertaining to the provision of bilingual supports to students with developmental disabilities. Using an online survey, data were collected in six socio-culturally and linguistically diverse locations across four countries: the United States, Canada, the United Kingdom, and the Netherlands. In total, 361 surveys were included in the analysis from respondents who were primarily teachers and speech-language pathologists working in schools, daycares/preschools, or community-based clinics. The overall picture that emerged from the data reflected a disconnection between practice and opinion. In general, respondents believed that children with both mild and severe disabilities are capable of learning a second language, although their opinions were more neutral for the latter group. However, children with both mild and severe disabilities who spoke only a minority language at home had less access to services for second language learners than did their typically developing peers, although respondents agreed that such services should be more available. Regardless of clinical group, children who lived in homes where a minority language was spoken were often exposed to, assessed in, and treated in the majority language only; again, respondents generally disagreed with these practices. Finally, second language classes were less available to children in the two disability groups compared to typically developing bilingual children, with general agreement that the opportunity to acquire a second language should be more available, especially to those with mild disabilities. Although the results indicate that there is a considerable gap between current practices and professional opinions, professionals appear to be more supportive of bilingual educational opportunities for these populations than was suggested by previous research.


Asunto(s)
Discapacidades del Desarrollo/psicología , Multilingüismo , Práctica Profesional , Adulto , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Internacionalidad , Internet , Terapia del Lenguaje/métodos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Grupos Minoritarios/psicología , Patología del Habla y Lenguaje/educación , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
5.
J Commun Disord ; 63: 1-14, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27461977

RESUMEN

Children with developmental disabilities (DD) often need and sometimes opt to become bilingual. The context for bilingual acquisition varies considerably and can impact outcomes. In this first article of the special issue, we review research on the timing and amount of bilingual exposure and outcomes of either direct language intervention or educational placements in three groups of children with DD: Specific Language Impairment (SLI), Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD), and Down syndrome (DS). Children with SLI have been studied more than the other two groups. Findings showed that, on the one hand, the communication skills of simultaneous bilinguals and matched monolinguals with DD were similar for all groups when the stronger language or both languages of the bilingual children were considered. On the other hand, similar to typically developing children, sequential bilinguals and matched monolinguals with SLI (other groups not studied) differed on some but not all second language (L2) measures; even after an extended period of exposure, differences in L2 outcomes were not completely resolved. There is emerging evidence that the typological similarity of the languages being learned influences L2 development in sequential bilinguals, at least in children with SLI. Increasing the frequency of exposure seems to be more related to development of the weaker language in bilinguals with DD than their stronger language. Language intervention studies show the efficacy of interventions but provide little evidence for transfer across languages. In addition, only one (unpublished) study has compared the language and academic outcomes of children with DD in different language education programs. Research on bilingual children with DD in different educational settings/programs is limited, probably as a result of restricted inclusion of these children in some educational settings. We argue for the implementation of full inclusion policies that provide increased access to dual language programs for children with DD and access to a complete range of support services.


Asunto(s)
Discapacidades del Desarrollo/psicología , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Multilingüismo , Niño , Desarrollo Infantil , Humanos , Internacionalidad
6.
Nat Commun ; 6: 10073, 2015 Dec 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26624517

RESUMEN

Early experiences may establish a foundation for later learning, however, influences of early language experience on later neural processing are unknown. We investigated whether maintenance of neural templates from early language experience influences subsequent language processing. Using fMRI, we scanned the following three groups performing a French phonological working memory (PWM) task: (1) monolingual French children; (2) children adopted from China before age 3 who discontinued Chinese and spoke only French; (3) Chinese-speaking children who learned French as a second language but maintained Chinese. Although all groups perform this task equally well, brain activation differs. French monolinguals activate typical PWM brain regions, while both Chinese-exposed groups also activate regions implicated in cognitive control, even the adoptees who were monolingual French speakers at testing. Early exposure to a language, and/or delayed exposure to a subsequent language, continues to influence the neural processing of subsequently learned language sounds years later even in highly proficient, early-exposed users.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Neuronas/fisiología , Adolescente , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Niño , Cognición , Femenino , Francia , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Multilingüismo , Radiografía
8.
J Child Lang ; 42(1): 196-209, 2015 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24274007

RESUMEN

The present study compared the performance of twenty-seven French-speaking internationally adopted (IA) children from China to that of twenty-seven monolingual non-adopted French-speaking children (CTL) matched for age, gender, and socioeconomic status on a Clitic Elicitation task. The IA children omitted significantly more accusative object clitics and made significantly more agreement errors using clitics than the CTL children. No other significant differences were found between the groups. The findings suggest that the adoptees may experience difficulties in morphosyntactic development possibly as a result of their delayed exposure to the adopted language.


Asunto(s)
Adopción , Lenguaje Infantil , Trastornos del Lenguaje/clasificación , Trastornos del Lenguaje/etiología , Niño , China , Femenino , Francia , Humanos , Conducta Verbal
9.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(48): 17314-9, 2014 Dec 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25404336

RESUMEN

Optimal periods during early development facilitate the formation of perceptual representations, laying the framework for future learning. A crucial question is whether such early representations are maintained in the brain over time without continued input. Using functional MRI, we show that internationally adopted (IA) children from China, exposed exclusively to French since adoption (mean age of adoption, 12.8 mo), maintained neural representations of their birth language despite functionally losing that language and having no conscious recollection of it. Their neural patterns during a Chinese lexical tone discrimination task matched those observed in Chinese/French bilinguals who have had continual exposure to Chinese since birth and differed from monolingual French speakers who had never been exposed to Chinese. They processed lexical tone as linguistically relevant, despite having no Chinese exposure for 12.6 y, on average, and no conscious recollection of that language. More specifically, IA participants recruited left superior temporal gyrus/planum temporale, matching the pattern observed in Chinese/French bilinguals. In contrast, French speakers who had never been exposed to Chinese did not recruit this region and instead activated right superior temporal gyrus. We show that neural representations are not overwritten and suggest a special status for language input obtained during the first year of development.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Lenguaje , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adolescente , Adopción , Niño , China , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Femenino , Francia , Humanos , Lactante , Multilingüismo , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Inconsciencia
10.
J Child Lang ; 41(6): 1195-223, 2014 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24168794

RESUMEN

The goal of the present study was to examine if internationally adopted (IA) children from China (M = 10;8) adopted by French-speaking families exhibit lags in verbal memory in addition to lags in verbal abilities documented in previous studies (Gauthier & Genesee, 2011). Tests assessing verbal and non-verbal memory, language, non-verbal cognitive ability, and socio-emotional development were administered to thirty adoptees. Their results were compared to those of thirty non-adopted monolingual French-speaking children matched on age, gender, and socioeconomic status. The IA children scored significantly lower than the controls on language, verbal short-term memory, verbal working memory, and verbal long-term memory. No group differences were found on non-verbal memory, non-verbal cognitive ability, and socio-emotional development, suggesting language-specific difficulties. Despite extended exposure to French, adoptees may experience language difficulties due to limitations in verbal memory, possibly as a result of their delayed exposure to that language and/or attrition of the birth language.


Asunto(s)
Adopción/psicología , Lenguaje Infantil , Memoria , Factores de Edad , Canadá , Preescolar , China/etnología , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Factores Sexuales , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
11.
J Child Lang ; 40(5): 1076-90, 2013 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23174272

RESUMEN

Acquisition of English grammatical morphology was examined in five internationally adopted (IA) children from China (aged 0;10-1;1 at adoption) during the first three years' exposure to English to determine whether acquisition patterns were characteristic of child second language (L2) learners or monolingual first language (L1) learners. Results from spontaneous and elicited speech showed that IA children acquired grammatical morphemes similarly to L1 learners; namely, (1) non-tense-marking morphemes were acquired earlier than tense-marking morphemes; (2) BE was acquired in synchrony with other tense-marking morphemes; and (3) a high percentage of omission errors and a low percentage of commission errors were observed.


Asunto(s)
Adopción , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Multilingüismo , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Estudios Longitudinales
12.
PLoS One ; 7(12): e52318, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23300641

RESUMEN

This longitudinal study tracked the neuro-cognitive changes associated with second language (L2) grammar learning in adults in order to investigate how L2 processing is shaped by a learner's first language (L1) background and L2 proficiency. Previous studies using event-related potentials (ERPs) have argued that late L2 learners cannot elicit a P600 in response to L2 grammatical structures that do not exist in the L1 or that are different in the L1 and L2. We tested whether the neuro-cognitive processes underlying this component become available after intensive L2 instruction. Korean- and Chinese late-L2-learners of English were tested at the beginning and end of a 9-week intensive English-L2 course. ERPs were recorded while participants read English sentences containing violations of regular past tense (a grammatical structure that operates differently in Korean and does not exist in Chinese). Whereas no P600 effects were present at the start of instruction, by the end of instruction, significant P600s were observed for both L1 groups. Latency differences in the P600 exhibited by Chinese and Korean speakers may be attributed to differences in L1-L2 reading strategies. Across all participants, larger P600 effects at session 2 were associated with: 1) higher levels of behavioural performance on an online grammaticality judgment task; and 2) with correct, rather than incorrect, behavioural responses. These findings suggest that the neuro-cognitive processes underlying the P600 (e.g., "grammaticalization") are modulated by individual levels of L2 behavioural performance and learning.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Lenguaje , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Animales , Conducta/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
13.
Child Dev ; 82(3): 887-901, 2011.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21413938

RESUMEN

The French language development of children adopted (n=24) from China was compared with that of control children matched for socioeconomic status, sex, and age. The children were assessed at 50 months of age, on average, and 16 months later. The initial assessment revealed that the 2 groups did not differ with respect to socioemotional adjustment or intellectual abilities. However, the adopted children's expressive language skills were significantly lower than those of the nonadopted children at both assessments. The receptive language skills were also significantly weaker for the adopted children at the second assessment. The results are discussed in terms of possible early age-of-acquisition effects that might affect adopted children's ability to acquire a second first language.


Asunto(s)
Adopción/psicología , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Multilingüismo , Preescolar , China/etnología , Comprensión , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Inteligencia , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Quebec , Valores de Referencia , Ajuste Social , Percepción del Habla , Conducta Verbal , Aprendizaje Verbal , Vocabulario
14.
J Child Lang ; 38(3): 554-78, 2011 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20738891

RESUMEN

Bilingual and monolingual children's (mean age=4;10) elicited production of the past tense in both English and French was examined in order to test predictions from Usage-Based theory regarding the sensitivity of children's acquisition rates to input factors such as variation in exposure time and the type/token frequency of morphosyntactic structures. Both bilingual and monolingual children were less accurate with irregular than regular past tense forms in both languages. Bilingual children, as a group, were less accurate than monolinguals with the English regular and irregular past tense, and with the French irregular past tense, but not with the French regular past tense. However, bilingual children were as accurate as monolinguals with the past tense in their language of greater exposure, except for English irregular verbs. It is argued that these results support the view that children's acquisition rates are sensitive to input factors, but with some qualifications.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Multilingüismo , Psicolingüística , Semántica , Percepción del Habla , Medición de la Producción del Habla , Aprendizaje Verbal , Canadá , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
16.
J Child Lang ; 34(1): 159-74, 2007 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17340942

RESUMEN

This study examined two- (n = 10) and three-year-old (n = 16) French-English bilingual children's repairs of breakdowns in communication that occurred when they did not use the same language as their interlocutor (Language breakdowns) and for other reasons (e.g. inaudible utterance). The children played with an experimenter who used only one language (English or French) during the play session. Each time a child used the other language, the experimenter made up to five requests for clarification, from non-specific (What?) to specific (Can you say that in French/English?). The experimenter also made requests for clarification when breakdowns occurred for other reasons, e.g. the child spoke too softly, produced an ambiguous utterance, etc. Both the two- and three-year-olds were capable of repairing Language breakdowns by switching languages to match that of their experimenter and they avoided this repair strategy when attempting to repair Other breakdowns. Moreover, they switched languages in response to non-specific requests. The results indicate that even two-and-a-half-year-old bilingual children are capable of identifying their language choice as a cause of communication breakdowns and that they can differentiate Language from Other kinds of communication breakdowns.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación , Multilingüismo , Conducta Verbal , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Medición de la Producción del Habla
17.
Cognition ; 100(2): 369-88, 2006 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16115614

RESUMEN

To trace how age and language experience shape the discrimination of native and non-native phonetic contrasts, we compared 4-year-olds learning either English or French or both and simultaneous bilingual adults on their ability to discriminate the English /d-th/ contrast. Findings show that the ability to discriminate the native English contrast improved with age. However, in the absence of experience with this contrast, discrimination of French children and adults remained unchanged during development. Furthermore, although simultaneous bilingual and monolingual English adults were comparable, children exposed to both English and French were poorer at discriminating this contrast when compared to monolingual English-learning 4-year-olds. Thus, language experience facilitates perception of the English /d-th/ contrast and this facilitation occurs later in development when English and French are acquired simultaneously. The difference between bilingual and monolingual acquisition has implications for language organization in children with simultaneous exposure.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Multilingüismo , Pruebas de Discriminación del Habla , Percepción del Habla , Aprendizaje Verbal , Canadá , Preescolar , Comparación Transcultural , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Fonética
18.
J Child Lang ; 32(4): 893-909, 2005 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16429716

RESUMEN

This study investigates subject omission in six English-Inuktitut simultaneous bilingual children, aged 1;8-3;9, to examine whether there are cross-language influences in their language development. Previous research with other language pairs has shown that the morphosyntax of one language can influence the development of morphosyntax in the other language. Most of this research has focused on Romance-Germanic language combinations using case studies. In this study, we examined a language pair (English-Inuktitut) with radically different morphosyntactic structures. Analysis of the English-only and Inuktitut-only utterances of the children revealed monolingual-like acquisition patterns and subject omission rates. The data indicate that these bilingual children possessed knowledge of the target languages that was language-specific and that previously identified triggers for crosslinguistic influence do not operate universally.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje Infantil , Lingüística , Multilingüismo , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Medición de la Producción del Habla , Grabación en Cinta , Aprendizaje Verbal
19.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 46(1): 113-27, 2003 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12647892

RESUMEN

The goal of this study was to determine whether bilingual children with specific language impairment (SLI) are similar to monolingual age mates with SLI, in each language. Eight French-English bilingual children with SLI were compared to age-matched monolingual children with SLI, both English and French speaking, with respect to their use of morphosyntax in language production. Specifically, using the extended optional infinitive (EOI) framework, the authors examined the children's use of tense-bearing and non-tense-bearing morphemes in obligatory context in spontaneous speech. Analyses revealed that the patterns predicted by the EOI framework were borne out for both the monolingual and bilingual children with SLI: The bilingual and monolingual children with SLI showed greater accuracy with non-tense than with tense morphemes. Furthermore, the bilingual and monolingual children with SLI had similar mean accuracy scores for tense morphemes, indicating that the bilingual children did not exhibit more profound deficits in the use of these grammatical morphemes than their monolingual peers. In sum, the bilingual children with SLI in this study appeared similar to their monolingual peers for the aspects of grammatical morphology examined in each language. These bilingual-monolingual similarities point to the possibility that SLI may not be an impediment to learning two languages, at least in the domain of grammatical morphology.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Lenguaje/complicaciones , Lenguaje , Multilingüismo , Trastornos del Habla/complicaciones , Niño , Estudios de Cohortes , Femenino , Humanos , Trastornos del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Grupo Paritario , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Trastornos del Habla/diagnóstico , Medición de la Producción del Habla
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA