Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 29
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Sci Rep ; 8(1): 6660, 2018 04 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29703989

RESUMEN

Certain personality traits (e.g. anxiousness, fearfulness), are known to affect the cognitive processing of environmental stimuli, such as the judgement of ambiguous stimuli (judgement bias). Our aim was to assess if personality traits are predictive of a more or less 'pessimistic' or 'optimistic' judgement bias in the domestic dog. We assessed dog personality (N = 31) using two validated protocols: the Dog Mentality Assessment (standardised battery test) and the CBARQ (owner-based survey). We used a common task based on the animals' latency to approach a bowl placed in one of three ambiguous positions (Near Positive, Middle, Near Negative) between a baited (Positive) and a non-baited food bowl (Negative) to assess judgement bias. Linear Mixed Model analyses revealed that dogs scoring higher on sociability, excitability and non-social-fear had shorter response latencies to bowls in an ambiguous location, indicating a more 'optimistic' bias. In contrast, dogs scoring higher on separation-related-behaviour and dog-directed-fear/aggression traits were more likely to judge an ambiguous stimulus as leading to a negative outcome, indicating a more 'pessimistic' bias. Results, partially consistent with previous findings in humans, indicate that personality plays a role in the cognitive processing of environmental stimuli in the domestic dog.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Perros/psicología , Juicio , Personalidad/fisiología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Animales , Sesgo , Tiempo de Reacción
2.
Behav Processes ; 149: 1-7, 2018 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29391215

RESUMEN

This study explored the relationship between four measures of canine paw preference to establish whether the distribution, direction or strength of motor bias was consistent between tasks. Thirty-two dogs had their paw preferences tested using the Kong ball, tape, lift paw and First-stepping tests. A smaller sample were re-tested 6 months later. The distribution of the dogs' paw preferences was not significantly different from that expected by chance for the Kong ball and lift paw tests; dogs were significantly more inclined towards ambilaterality on the tape and First-stepping tests. More female dogs employed their right paw on the lift paw test; males were more likely to be ambilateral or left-pawed. There was no significant correlation in the direction of dogs' paw use for any tests. The First-stepping and lift paw tests were positively correlated for strength of paw use. Analysis revealed a significant correlation in direction and strength of dogs' paw use between the first and second attempts of all measures, except the tape test. Findings suggest that paw preference in the dog is not consistent between tasks, although stable over time. The study raises questions as to which test of paw preference is the most appropriate to employ.


Asunto(s)
Perros , Pie/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Masculino , Caracteres Sexuales
3.
J Comp Psychol ; 131(4): 317-325, 2017 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28517942

RESUMEN

Limb use, an indicator of hemispheric functioning, may be a useful predictor of cognitive bias and hence vulnerability to welfare risk. The relationship between cognitive bias and motor asymmetry, however, has been subject to little investigation. This study explored the association between motor asymmetry and cognitive bias in the domestic dog, a species that displays lateral bias in the form of paw preferences and exhibits positive and negative affective states. Thirty pet dogs had their paw preferences assessed using the Kong ball test. The subjects' affective state was assessed using a cognitive bias test in which the animals' latency to approach a bowl placed in 1 of 3 ambiguous positions was recorded. Animals veering more toward a left-paw preference were found to be significantly slower to approach the bowl placed in 1 of the ambiguous positions than ambilateral or right-pawed dogs. Left-pawed subjects approached the bowl located at the 3 ambiguous positions at roughly the same speed, while ambilateral and right-pawed animals became increasingly slower to approach the bowl the further it was located from the baited food bowl. The study points to a possible relationship between cognitive bias and paw preference in the dog, with left-pawed animals being more negative or "pessimistic" in their cognitive outlook than right-pawed or ambilateral individuals. It is proposed that limb preference testing might offer a more practical and straightforward way of identifying individuals at risk from poor welfare by virtue of how they perceive the world than more time-consuming cognitive bias tests. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Perros/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Conducta Espacial/fisiología , Temperamento/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Masculino
4.
J Comp Psychol ; 131(3): 246-256, 2017 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28414470

RESUMEN

Behavioral laterality reflects the cerebral functional asymmetry. Measures of laterality have been associated with emotional stress, problem-solving, and personality in some vertebrate species. Thus far, the association between laterality and personality in the domestic dog has been largely overlooked. In this study, we investigated whether lateralized (left or right) and ambilateral dogs differed in their behavioral response to a standardized personality test. The dog's preferred paw to hold a Kong ball filled with food and the first paw used to step-off from a standing position were scored as laterality measures. The Dog Mentality Assessment (DMA) test was used to assess 5 personality traits (e.g., sociability, aggressiveness) and a broader shy-boldness dimension. No differences emerged between left- and right-biased dogs on any personality trait. Instead, ambilateral dogs, scored using the Kong test, scored higher on their playfulness (Z = -1.98, p = .048) and Aggressiveness (Z = -2.10, p = .036) trait scores than did lateralized (irrespective of side) dogs. Also, ambilateral dogs assessed by using the First-Stepping test scored higher than lateralized dogs on the Sociability (Z = -2.83, p = .005) and Shy-Boldness (Z = -2.34, p = .019) trait scores. Overall, we found evidence of a link between canine personality and behavioral laterality, and this was especially true for those traits relating to stronger emotional reactivity, such as aggressiveness, fearfulness, and sociability. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Miedo/psicología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Personalidad , Solución de Problemas/fisiología , Agresión , Animales , Perros
5.
J Comp Psychol ; 130(4): 313-320, 2016 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27359075

RESUMEN

Research points to a relationship between lateralization and emotional functioning in humans and many species of animal. The present study explored the association between paw preferences and emotional functioning, specifically temperament, in a species thus far overlooked in this area, the domestic cat. Thirty left-pawed, 30 right-pawed, and 30 ambilateral pet cats were recruited following an assessment of their paw preferences using a food-reaching challenge. The animals' temperament was subsequently assessed using the Feline Temperament Profile (FTP). Cats' owners also completed a purpose-designed cat temperament (CAT) scale. Analysis revealed a significant relationship between lateral bias and FTP and CAT scale scores. Ambilateral cats had lower positive (FTP+) scores, and were perceived as less affectionate, obedient, friendly, and more aggressive, than left or right-pawed animals. Left and right pawed cats differed significantly on 1 trait on the CAT scale, namely playfulness. The strength of the cats' paw preferences was related to the animals' FTP and CAT scores. Cats with a greater strength of paw preference had higher FTP+ scores than those with a weaker strength of paw preference. Animals with stronger paw preferences were perceived as more confident, affectionate, active, and friendly than those with weaker paw preferences. Results suggest that motor laterality in the cat is strongly related to temperament and that the presence or absence of lateralization has greater implications for the expression of emotion in this species than the direction of the lateralized bias. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Gatos/psicología , Lateralidad Funcional , Personalidad/fisiología , Temperamento , Animales , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Fenotipo
6.
BMJ Open ; 6(6): e008916, 2016 06 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27311899

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To determine whether an elevated fetal umbilical artery Doppler (UAD) pulsatility index (PI) at 28 weeks' gestation, in the absence of fetal growth restriction (FGR) and prematurity, is associated with adverse neurocognitive outcome in children aged 12 years. METHODS: Prospective cohort study, comparing children with a normal fetal UAD PI (<90th centile) (n=110) and those with an elevated PI (≥90th centile) (n=40). UAD was performed at 28, 32 and 34 weeks gestation. At 12 years of age, all children were assessed under standardised conditions at Queen's University, Belfast, UK to determine cognitive and behavioural outcomes using the British Ability Score-II and Achenbach Child Behavioural Checklist Parent Rated Version under standardised conditions. Regression analysis was performed, controlling for confounders such as gender, socioeconomic status and age at assessment. RESULTS: The mean age of follow-up was 12.4 years (±0.5 SD) with 44% of children male (n=63). When UAD was assessed at 28 weeks, the elevated fetal UAD group had lower scores in cognitive assessments of information processing and memory. Parameters included (1) recall of objects immediate verbal (p=0.002), (2) delayed verbal (p=0.008) and (3) recall of objects immediate spatial (p=0.0016). There were no significant differences between the Doppler groups at 32 or 34 weeks' gestation. CONCLUSIONS: An elevated UAD PI at 28 weeks' gestation in the absence of FGR or prematurity is associated with lower scores of declarative memory in children aged 12 years. A potential explanation for this is an element of placental insufficiency in the presence of the appropriately grown fetus, which affects the development of the fetal hippocampus and information processing and memory long-term. These findings, however, had no impact on overall academic ability, mental processing and reasoning or overall behavioural function.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Neurocognitivos/etiología , Arterias Umbilicales/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto , Biomarcadores , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Edad Gestacional , Humanos , Modelos Lineales , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Circulación Placentaria , Embarazo , Estudios Prospectivos , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Flujo Pulsátil , Ultrasonografía Doppler , Ultrasonografía Prenatal , Arterias Umbilicales/fisiología , Reino Unido
8.
Dev Psychobiol ; 55(6): 588-95, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23765736

RESUMEN

This paper reviews the evidence for lateralized motor behavior in the fetus around a number of key questions: does the fetus exhibit signs of laterality? when does lateralized motor behavior begin? is the lateralized preference consistently displayed? does prenatal handedness relate to postnatal handedness? and, does prenatal handedness relate to brain functioning? The evidence indicates that the fetus exhibits lateralized behavior from 10 weeks gestation, as soon as it independently moves its arms, and this is the precursor of lateralized motor behavior observed post-natally. Data is presented suggesting that the strength of laterality decreases with advancing gestation and this correlates with more efficient information processing as assessed by habituation. However extreme caution is warranted in attempting to link asymmetric motor behavior and brain function prenatally. The paper concludes that the initial developmental emergence of lateralized behavior is under genetic control and is a fundamental feature of prenatal human development.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Desarrollo Fetal/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Humanos
9.
Dev Psychobiol ; 55(5): 568-74, 2013 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22753112

RESUMEN

The human fetus learns about its chemosensory environment and this influences its behavior at birth and during the nursing period. This study examined whether prenatal experience could influence behavior much later in life. The dietary preference of two groups of children (8- to 9-years old) was examined. Mothers of one group had consumed garlic during pregnancy, mothers of the control group had not. Children received two tests, 1 month apart, of a meal containing two portions of potato gratin, one flavored with garlic. The total amount of potato, and the percentage of garlic flavored potato, eaten was calculated and examined separately by ANOVA for factors of prenatal exposure, the child's sex, and trial. Children prenatally exposed to garlic ate significantly more garlic flavored potato and a significantly greater overall amount of potato on trial 2, compared to controls. The results demonstrate prenatal experience may affect behavior well into childhood.


Asunto(s)
Preferencias Alimentarias/fisiología , Ajo , Efectos Tardíos de la Exposición Prenatal/psicología , Gusto/fisiología , Lactancia Materna , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Embarazo , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología
10.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res ; 36(12): 2168-75, 2012 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22978459

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Studies of the adverse neurobehavioral effects of maternal alcohol consumption on the fetus have been largely confined to the postnatal period, after exposure to alcohol has finished. This study explored the brain function of the fetus, at the time of exposure to alcohol, to examine its effect on information processing and stability of performance. METHODS: Five groups of fetuses, defined by maternal alcohol consumption patterns, were examined: control (no alcohol); moderate (5 to 10 units/wk either drunk evenly across the week or as a binge, in 2 to 3 days); heavy (20+ units/wk drunk evenly or as a binge). Fetal habituation performance was examined on 3 occasions, separated by 7 days, beginning at 35 weeks of gestation. The number of trials required to habituate on each test session and the difference in performance across test sessions were recorded. RESULTS: Fetuses exposed to heavy binge drinking required significantly more trials to habituate and exhibited a greater variability in performance across all test sessions than the other groups. Maternal drinking, either heavily but evenly or moderately as a binge, resulted in poorer habituation, and moderate binge drinking resulted in greater variability compared with no, or even, drinking. CONCLUSIONS: Decreased information processing, reflected by poorer habituation, and increased variability in performance may reflect the initial manifestations of structural damage caused by alcohol to the brain. These results will lead to a greater understanding of the effects of alcohol on the fetus's brain, enable the antenatal identification of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders, and lead to the early implementation of better management strategies.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/efectos adversos , Alcoholismo/complicaciones , Encéfalo/embriología , Enfermedades Fetales/inducido químicamente , Feto/efectos de los fármacos , Complicaciones del Embarazo/fisiopatología , Adulto , Encéfalo/efectos de los fármacos , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Ecoencefalografía , Femenino , Edad Gestacional , Humanos , Masculino , Embarazo , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Ultrasonografía Prenatal , Adulto Joven
11.
Physiol Behav ; 107(1): 76-81, 2012 Aug 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22691707

RESUMEN

Prenatal exposure to alcohol may exert a significant detrimental effect on the functioning of the individual's brain, however few studies have examined this before birth. This longitudinal study examined the effect of maternal alcohol consumption on the elicited startle response of the fetus. Two groups of fetuses were examined: one whose mothers drank alcohol (approximately 10 units per week); the other whose mothers did not drink alcohol. Fetuses were examined at 29, 32 and 35 weeks gestation and their startle response observed using ultrasound in response to 2 presentations of a pink noise (70-250Hz) at 90dB(A) separated by 30s. Fetuses exposed to alcohol exhibited a weaker startle response at 29 weeks gestation than did fetuses not exposed to alcohol. There was no difference in the response at 32 and 35 weeks gestation. To ensure that the effects were not due to a more general effect of alcohol on fetal movement, a second experiment compared the spontaneous movements (observed on ultrasound for 45 min) of fetuses whose mothers drank alcohol and fetuses of mothers who didn't drink alcohol. There were no differences in movements exhibited by the fetuses. The results suggest that exposure to alcohol delays the emergence of the elicited startle response at 29 weeks gestation but this delay has disappeared by 32 weeks gestation. The possible role of altered neural development, acute exposure to alcohol and disruptions to the fetus's behavioural repertoire, in mediating these effects are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Depresores del Sistema Nervioso Central/farmacología , Etanol/farmacología , Feto/efectos de los fármacos , Efectos Tardíos de la Exposición Prenatal/fisiopatología , Reflejo de Sobresalto/efectos de los fármacos , Estimulación Acústica/efectos adversos , Factores de Edad , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/fisiopatología , Animales , Animales Recién Nacidos , Femenino , Movimiento Fetal/efectos de los fármacos , Feto/fisiología , Edad Gestacional , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Ratones , Embarazo , Efectos Tardíos de la Exposición Prenatal/inducido químicamente , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Factores de Tiempo
12.
Dev Sci ; 15(3): 373-83, 2012 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22490177

RESUMEN

There is some evidence for sex differences in habituation in the human fetus, but it is unknown whether this is due to differences in central processing (habituation) or in more peripheral processes, sensory or motor, involved in the response. This study examined whether the sex of the fetus influenced auditory habituation at 33 weeks of gestation, and whether this was due to differences in habituation or in the sensory or motor components using a set of four experiments. The first experiment found that female fetuses required significantly fewer stimulus presentations to habituate than males. The second experiment revealed no difference in the spontaneous motor behaviour of male and female fetuses. The third experiment examined auditory intensity thresholds for the stimuli used to habituate the fetus. No differences in thresholds were found between males and females, although there was inter-individual variability in thresholds. A final experiment, using stimuli individualized for that particular fetus' auditory intensity threshold, found that female fetuses habituated faster than males. In combination, the studies reveal that habituation in the human fetus is affected by sex and this is due to a difference in central 'information processing' of the stimuli rather than peripheral aspects of the response. It is argued that male and female fetuses present different neurobehavioural developmental trajectories, with females more advanced at 33 weeks than males. This study suggests that research examining prenatal behaviour should consider the factor of fetal sex. This may be particularly pertinent where there is an intention to use the results diagnostically.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Feto/fisiología , Habituación Psicofisiológica/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Umbral Auditivo/fisiología , Femenino , Edad Gestacional , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Física , Factores Sexuales
13.
Primates ; 53(2): 121-6, 2012 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22261746

RESUMEN

The olfactory abilities of great apes have been subject to little empirical investigation, save for a few observational reports. This study, using an habituation/dishabituation task, provides experimental evidence for a core olfactory ability, namely, olfactory discrimination, in the gorilla. In Experiment 1, six zoo-housed western lowland gorillas were individually presented with the same odour on four trials, and with a novel odour on the fifth trial. Odours (almond and vanilla) were presented on plastic balls, and behavioural responses of sniffing and chewing/licking the balls were recorded. A second experiment presented the same odour on four trials and no odour on the fifth to examine whether any dishabituation was due to the presence of a new odour or the absence of the familiar odour. Gorillas habituated their behaviour with repeated presentation of the same odour, but dishabituated, i.e. increased sniffing and chewing/licking, when presented with the novel odour. No dishabituation was noted when using water as the stimulus across all trials or when used as the novel odour. Overall, results show that gorillas are able to discriminate between odours.


Asunto(s)
Discriminación en Psicología , Gorilla gorilla/fisiología , Habituación Psicofisiológica , Odorantes , Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal , Femenino , Masculino
14.
Early Hum Dev ; 88(6): 431-6, 2012 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22100057

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Neurodevelopment outcomes of children conceived by Assisted Reproductive Technology (ART)have been the subject of much recent attention. To date there are no reports of neurodevelopmental performance before birth in this group. AIMS: To compare habituation (a measure of brain function) in fetuses conceived by assisted reproduction techniques (ART) with naturally conceived (NC) fetuses. STUDY DESIGN: Case control study. SUBJECTS: Women with singleton pregnancies matched for maternal age, parity and smoking were recruited in 2 groups: ART (n=20) and NC (n=20). OUTCOME MEASURES: Sound stimuli (250 Hz, 110 dB) at 10 second intervals lasting 2 s were administered to the fetus. The end point was habituation (cessation of movement for five consecutive stimuli) or a maximum of 30 stimuli. Responses of the fetus were observed with ultrasound at 28, 32 and 36 weeks' gestation, video-recorded and anonymised for analysis. RESULTS: At 28 weeks' gestation significantly more ART fetuses responded to sound of 250 Hz, 110 dB (p=0.02) but this difference did not persist at 32 and 36 weeks'. There was a significant increase in nonresponders as gestation advanced in the ART group. There was no difference in habituation or mean number of trials to habituate at all three gestations. CONCLUSIONS: ART fetuses demonstrated no differences in habituation suggesting that there is no neurodevelopment delay. However, a decrease in response to sound as gestation advances might be a harbinger for poor perinatal outcomes and needs exploration.


Asunto(s)
Feto/fisiología , Habituación Psicofisiológica/fisiología , Trastornos Psicomotores/etiología , Técnicas Reproductivas Asistidas/efectos adversos , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Sistema Nervioso Central/embriología , Sistema Nervioso Central/fisiopatología , Femenino , Monitoreo Fetal , Movimiento Fetal/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Embarazo , Trastornos Psicomotores/diagnóstico , Tiempo de Reacción , Ultrasonografía Prenatal , Grabación en Video
15.
Chem Senses ; 35(4): 263-8, 2010 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20190006

RESUMEN

Many species produce odor cues that enable them to be identified individually, as well as providing other socially relevant information. Study of the role of odor cues in the social behavior of great apes is noticeable by its absence. Olfaction has been viewed as having little role in guiding behavior in these species. This study examined whether Western lowland gorillas produce an individually identifiable odor. Odor samples were obtained by placing cloths in the gorilla's den. A delayed matching to sample task was used with human participants (n = 100) to see if they were able to correctly match a target odor sample to a choice of either: 2 odors (the target sample and another, Experiment 1) and 6 odors (the target sample and 5 others, Experiment 2). Participants were correctly able to identify the target odor when given either 2 or 6 matches. Subjects made fewest errors when matching the odor of the silverback, whereas matching the odors of the young gorillas produced most errors. The results indicate that gorillas do produce individually identifiable body odors and introduce the possibility that odor cues may play a role in gorilla social behavior.


Asunto(s)
Odorantes , Olfato/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Animales , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Gorilla gorilla , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Conducta Social
16.
Angle Orthod ; 80(2): 361-6, 2010 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19905863

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To test the hypothesis that the self-perception of dental and facial attractiveness among patients requiring orthognathic surgery is no different from that of control patients. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Happiness with dental and facial appearance was assessed using questionnaires completed by 162 patients who required orthognathic treatment and 157 control subjects. Visual analog scale, binary, and open response data were collected. Analysis was carried out using a general linear model, logistic regression, and chi-square tests. RESULTS: Orthognathic patients were less happy with their dental appearance than were controls. Class II patients and women had lower happiness scores for their dental appearance. Among orthognathic patients, the "shape" and "prominence" of their teeth were the most frequent causes of concern. Older subjects, women, and orthognathic patients were less happy with their facial appearance. Class III orthognathic patients, older subjects, and women were more likely to have looked at their own face in profile. A greater proportion of Class II subjects than Class III subjects wished to change their appearance. CONCLUSIONS: The hypothesis is rejected. The findings indicate that women and patients requiring orthognathic surgery had lower levels of happiness with their dentofacial appearance. Although Class II patients exhibited the lowest levels of happiness with their dental appearance, there was some evidence that concerns and awareness about their facial profile were more pronounced among the Class III patients.


Asunto(s)
Estética Dental/psicología , Maloclusión de Angle Clase III/psicología , Maloclusión Clase II de Angle/psicología , Autoimagen , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Distribución de Chi-Cuadrado , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Maloclusión Clase II de Angle/cirugía , Maloclusión de Angle Clase III/cirugía , Evaluación de Necesidades , Procedimientos Quirúrgicos Ortognáticos , Factores Sexuales , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
17.
Angle Orthod ; 80(1): 43-8, 2010 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19852638

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To establish the extent of psychological problems among patients who require orthognathic treatment. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Five aspects of psychological functioning were assessed for 162 patients who required orthognathic treatment and compared with 157 control subjects. RESULTS: Analysis of variance did not detect any significant difference in the five psychological scores recorded for the skeletal II, skeletal III, and control groups. The proportion of subjects with one or more psychological measure beyond the normal range was 27% for skeletal II subjects, 25% for skeletal III subjects, and 26% for control subjects. One skeletal II subject (1.5%), three skeletal III subjects (3%), and five control subjects (3%) required referral for psychological counseling. CONCLUSIONS: The orthognathic patients did not differ significantly from the control subjects in their psychological status.


Asunto(s)
Maloclusión de Angle Clase III/psicología , Maloclusión Clase II de Angle/psicología , Trastornos Mentales/psicología , Procedimientos Quirúrgicos Ortognáticos , Adolescente , Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Ansiedad/psicología , Niño , Conducta Infantil/psicología , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/psicología , Depresión/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Maloclusión Clase II de Angle/cirugía , Maloclusión de Angle Clase III/cirugía , Persona de Mediana Edad , Procedimientos Quirúrgicos Ortognáticos/psicología , Derivación y Consulta , Autoimagen , Adulto Joven
18.
Cleft Palate Craniofac J ; 44(3): 304-11, 2007 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17477751

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: (1) to determine the opinion of parents regarding the psychosocial functioning of their child with cleft lip and/or palate (CLP); (2) to identify predictors of psychosocial functioning; and (3) to determine the level of agreement between children with CLP and their parents. PARTICIPANTS: One hundred twenty-nine parents of children with CLP and 96 parents of children without CLP participated in this cross-sectional study. OUTCOME MEASURES: Parental opinion of the child's self-esteem, anxiety, happiness, and problems caused by facial appearance were assessed using visual analogue scales. Parents completed the Child Behavior Checklist and were interviewed. RESULTS: Children with CLP were more anxious (p < 0.05), less happy with their appearance (p < 0.001), and in general (p < 0.05) had lower self-esteem (p < 0.05) and greater behavioral problems (p < 0.001) compared with non-CLP children. Parents reported that their child with CLP was teased more often (p < 0.001) and was less satisfied with his/her speech (p < 0.01) compared with reports of parents in the control group. A number of factors affected parents' ratings of their child's psychosocial functioning (presence of CLP, appearance happiness, previous history of CLP, and visibility of scar). Children who had been teased were more anxious (p < or = 0.01), less happy with their appearance (p < 0.001) and had greater behavioral problems (p < 0.001). CONCLUSIONS: Parents of children with CLP reported various psychosocial problems among their children. Parents considered children who had been teased to have greater psychosocial problems.


Asunto(s)
Labio Leporino/psicología , Fisura del Paladar/psicología , Padres , Autoimagen , Ajuste Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Ansiedad/etiología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Labio Leporino/complicaciones , Labio Leporino/cirugía , Fisura del Paladar/complicaciones , Fisura del Paladar/cirugía , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , Relaciones Médico-Paciente , Estadísticas no Paramétricas , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
19.
Cleft Palate Craniofac J ; 43(5): 598-605, 2006 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16986986

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: A cross-sectional study was employed to determine the psychosocial effects of cleft lip and/or palate among children and young adults, compared with a control group of children and young adults without cleft lip and palate. PARTICIPANTS: The study comprised 160 children and young adults with cleft lip and/or palate and 113 children and young adults without cleft lip and/or palate. All participants were between 8 and 21 years of age. OUTCOME MEASURES: Psychological functioning (anxiety, self-esteem, depression, and behavioral problems) was assessed using validated psychological questionnaires. Happiness with facial appearance was rated using a visual analog scale. Social functioning, including experience of teasing/bullying and satisfaction with speech, was assessed using a semistructured interview. RESULTS: Participants with cleft lip and/or palate reported greater behavioral problems (p < .001) and more symptoms of depression (p < .01); they were teased more often (p < .001) and were less happy with their facial appearance (p < .01) and speech (p < .001), compared with controls. There were no significant difference between subjects with cleft lip and/or palate and subjects without cleft lip and/or palate in terms of anxiety (p > .05) or self-esteem (p > .05). Having been teased was a significant predictor of poor psychological functioning, more so than having a cleft lip and/or palate per se (p < .001). CONCLUSIONS: Teasing was greater among participants who had cleft lip and/ or palate and it was a significant predictor of poorer psychosocial functioning. Children and young adults with cleft lip and/or palate require psychological assessment, specifically focusing on their experience of teasing, as part of their routine cleft care.


Asunto(s)
Labio Leporino/psicología , Fisura del Paladar/psicología , Ajuste Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Ansiedad/complicaciones , Niño , Labio Leporino/complicaciones , Fisura del Paladar/complicaciones , Depresión/complicaciones , Métodos Epidemiológicos , Femenino , Felicidad , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos Mentales/complicaciones , Autoimagen
20.
Behav Processes ; 73(1): 121-3, 2006 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16682155

RESUMEN

Lateralised motor behaviour in the pinnipeds has been subject to little investigation. This study examined the swimming behaviour of seven zoo-housed California sea lions to determine whether they exhibited a directional bias in their motor behaviour. Data were collected on the direction of the animals' swimming patterns from the point of entering a pool of water from dry land. Each animal was studied for 100 episodes of swimming. All seven of the sea lions showed significant (P<0.001) bias in the direction of their swimming, although unidirectional bias was not observed at the level of the population. The direction of the sea lions' swimming patterns varied significantly according to the animals' sex. Males showed a preference at the level of the population for swimming in a clockwise direction, while females showed a population-level counterclockwise swimming preference. Overall, the findings appear to suggest that California sea lions, like other marine mammals, exhibit motor bias in the direction of their swimming patterns, although further work using larger sample sizes is needed before more firm conclusions regarding motor laterality in this species can be reached.


Asunto(s)
Animales de Zoológico/fisiología , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional , Leones Marinos/fisiología , Natación , Animales , Animales de Zoológico/psicología , Femenino , Masculino , Leones Marinos/psicología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA