Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 137
Filtrar
1.
Perception ; : 3010066241258969, 2024 Jun 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38863276

RESUMEN

Vibrotactile feedback can be built into clothing such as vests. This means that often vibrotactile information is presented to the back. It is known that the back has a relatively low spatial acuity. Spatial acuity varies across different limbs and sometimes with different locations on a limb. These known anisotropies suggest that there might be systematic variations in vibrotactile spatial acuity for different areas of the back and also for different orientations (i.e. horizontal vs. vertical). Here we systematically measured spatial acuity in four areas of the back for both horizontal and vertical orientations. The results show no significant differences in spatial acuity for the back areas that were tested. Spatial acuity was, however, higher in the horizontal direction than in the vertical direction by roughly a factor of two. This means that when designing vibrotactile displays for the back the tactor density can be lower in the vertical direction than in the horizontal direction and density should be constant for different areas of the back.

2.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; PP2024 May 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38801695

RESUMEN

In this survey, we give an overview of hands-free haptic devices specifically designed for navigation guidance while walking. We present and discuss the devices by body part, namely devices for the arm, foot and leg, back, belly and shoulders, waist and finally the head. Although the majority of the experimental tests were successful in terms of reaching the target while being guided by the device, the experimental requirements were wide-ranging. The distances to be covered ranged from just a few meters to more than a kilometer, and while some of the devices worked autonomously, others required the experimenter to act as Wizard of Oz. To compare the usefulness and potential of these devices, we created a table in which we rated several relevant aspects such as autonomy, conspicuity and compactness. Major conclusions are that outdoor devices have the highest technology readiness level, because these allow autonomous navigation through GPS, and that the most compact devices still require the action of an experimenter. Unfortunately, none of the hands-free devices are at a level of readiness where they could be useful to people with visual impairments. The most important factor that should be improved is localization accuracy, which should be high and available at all times.

3.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; PP2024 Apr 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38587965

RESUMEN

In this study, we were interested in the question whether vibrotactile thresholds on the back, in terms of the Just Noticeable Difference (JND), scale with back length. Although there exists only indirect physiological evidence, it could be assumed that the density of mechanoreceptors is lower if back size is larger. As a consequence, the JND would increase with back length. We measured psychophysical curves for 40 male participants with back lengths in the range of 36 to 55 cm. Nine equally spaced vibration motors were placed in vertical direction on their back, 2 cm left of the spine. For each stimulus pair, participants had to decide whether the second vibration was above or below the first vibration. It was found that the slope of the fit of the JND as a function of back length was not significantly different from zero, so contrary to our expectation, we did not find an influence of back length on JND. This means that when customizing a wearable haptic device for the back, measuring back length is not the way to go.

4.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; 15(4): 655-666, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36173775

RESUMEN

In this survey, we give an overview of hand-held haptic navigation devices specifically designed for and tested with pedestrians. We distinguish devices for indoor use and for outdoor use as the implementation is usually quite different. Outdoor devices make use of Global Positioning Systems (GPS) tracking built-in in smartphones; indoor devices use a variety of sensors, and tracking and localization systems and these are usually restricted to a small part of a building. Overall, the high success rates reported in the studies show that vibrotactile stimulation via a hand-held user interface is suitable for navigation instructions, as in all experiments (almost) all participants reached their goal. An issue for several of the indoor devices is that walking speeds were (much) lower than normal walking speeds and path efficiency was relatively low. However, these issues might be overcome with some training as in most studies there was hardly any practice time. Several of the outdoor devices seem quite close to taking the last step before commercial use. In the Discussion, we evaluate the suitability of the devices for persons with visual and/or hearing impairments. Especially devices that provide very specific instructions, such as, 'go straight' or 'go right,' seem valuable for this population.


Asunto(s)
Tecnología Háptica , Percepción del Tacto , Humanos , Caminata , Sistemas de Información Geográfica
5.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; 15(2): 458-463, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34928804

RESUMEN

Vibrations on the back of a person can convey information about direction through sequentially switching on two vibration motors. For perception of direction the oblique effect can occur, meaning that perception of cardinal directions is more precise than perception of oblique directions. We investigated the role of the positioning of the vibrations with respect to the spine. In the first condition all vibration motors were placed in a circle around the spine ('Circle' condition) and direction was conveyed by switching on vibration motors on opposite sides of the circle. In the second condition the vibrations were placed in two semi-circles of which the centers were on the left and right sides of the back ('Semi-circle' condition). We found that participants showed larger deviations as well as a larger spread for oblique directions than for cardinal directions in both conditions. This indicates that the oblique effect occurred. Therefore, the oblique effect can occur irregardless of the positioning of the vibration motors with respect to the spine. Both deviations and spread were larger in the 'Semi-circle' condition than in the 'Circle' condition suggesting an advantage for centering motors around the spine, although this might have been influenced by the distance between vibrations.


Asunto(s)
Percepción , Vibración , Humanos
6.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; 14(3): 465-478, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33444143

RESUMEN

Communication is an essential part of human life. In this article, we give an overview of hands-free tactual devices that have been developed and tested for conveying speech or language. We opted for "hands-free" because especially in the case of individuals with impaired vision, in many situations their hands will be occupied with other essential tasks. We start this survey with presenting the various word building blocks that have been tested. These blocks vary from units based on the actual speech signal, via patterns representing phonemes, to letters, or letters coded via Morse or Braille-like patterns. In the second part of this article, studies that use these building blocks to create words are discussed. General findings are that successful devices do not necessarily depend on underlying speech characteriscs, dynamic patterns give better results than static patterns, and more vibrators do not generally give better results. Moreover, some of the most successful devices required only limited training time. Most of the recent devices are still in a quite early state of development and are tested only with a limited number of patterns. However, many of these recent devices give promising results and are worthwhile to further investigate and develop.


Asunto(s)
Percepción del Habla , Percepción del Tacto , Humanos , Lenguaje , Habla , Tacto
7.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 17876, 2020 10 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33087741

RESUMEN

Vibrotactile displays worn on the back can be used as sensory substitution device. Often vibrotactile stimulation is chosen because vibration motors are easy to incorporate and relatively cheap. When designing such displays knowledge about vibrotactile perception on the back is crucial. In the current study we investigated distance perception. Biases in distance perception can explain spatial distortions that occur when, for instance, tracing a shape using vibration. We investigated the effect of orientation (horizontal vs vertical), the effect of positioning with respect to the spine and the effect of switching vibration motors on sequentially versus simultaneously. Our study includes four conditions. The condition which had a horizontal orientation with both vibration motors switching on sequentially on the same side of the spine was chosen is the baseline condition. The other three conditions were compared to this baseline condition. We found that distances felt longer in the vertical direction than in the horizontal direction. Furthermore, distances were perceived to be longer when vibration motors were distributed on both sides of the spine compared to when they were on the same side. Finally, distances felt shorter when vibration motors were switched on simultaneously compared to sequentially. In the simultaneous case a distance of 4 cm was not clearly perceived differently than a distance of 12 cm. When designing vibrotactile displays these anisotropies in perceived distance need to be taken into account because otherwise the intended shape will not match the perceived shape. Also, dynamically presented distances are more clearly perceived than static distances. This finding supports recommendations made in previous studies that dynamic patterns are easier to perceive than static patterns.


Asunto(s)
Dorso/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Tacto/fisiología , Vibración , Adulto , Anisotropía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Física/métodos , Columna Vertebral/fisiología , Adulto Joven
8.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; 13(4): 691-698, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32324567

RESUMEN

It can be useful to display information about numerosity haptically. For instance, to display the time of day or distances when visual or auditory feedback is not possible or desirable. Here, we investigated the possibility of displaying numerosity information by means of a sequence of vibration pulses. From previous studies on numerosity perception in vision, haptics and audition it is known that numerosity judgment can be facilitated by grouping. Therefore, we investigated whether perception of the number of vibration pulses in a sequence can be facilitated by temporally grouping the pulses. We found that indeed temporal grouping can lead to considerably smaller errors and lower error rates indicating that this facilitated the task, but only when participants knew in advance whether the pulses would be temporally grouped. When grouped and ungrouped series of pulses were presented randomly interleaved, there was no difference in performance. This means that temporally grouping vibration sequences can allow the sequence to be displayed at a faster rate while it remains possible to perceive the number of vibration pulses accurately if the users is aware of the temporal grouping.


Asunto(s)
Juicio , Vibración , Percepción Auditiva , Humanos , Visión Ocular , Percepción Visual
9.
R Soc Open Sci ; 6(3): 181563, 2019 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31032013

RESUMEN

In this paper, we assess the importance of visual and haptic information about materials for scaling the grasping force when picking up an object. We asked 12 participants to pick up and lift objects with six different textures, either blindfolded or with visual information present. We measured the grip force and estimated the load force from the object's weight and vertical acceleration. The coefficient of friction of the materials was measured separately. Already at an early phase in the grasp (before lift-off), the grip force correlated highly with the textures' static coefficient of friction. However, no strong influence on the presence of visual information was found. We conclude that the main mechanism for modulation of grip force in the early phase of grasping is the real-time sensation of the texture's friction.

10.
PLoS One ; 14(1): e0211407, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30699197

RESUMEN

Previous studies have demonstrated that skin vibration is an important factor affecting the roughness perception of fine textures. For coarse textures, the determining physical factor is much less clear and there are indications that this might be participant-dependent. In this paper, we focused on roughness perception of both coarse and fine textures of different materials (glass particle surfaces and sandpapers). We investigated the relationship between subjective roughness ratings and three physical parameters (skin vibration, friction coefficient, and particle size) within a group of 30 participants. Results of the glass particle surfaces showed both spatial information (particle size) and temporal information (skin vibration) had a high correlation with subjective roughness ratings. The former correlation was slightly but significantly higher than the latter. The results also indicated different weights of temporal information and spatial information for roughness ratings among participants. Roughness ratings of a different material (sandpaper versus glass particles) could be either larger, similar or smaller, indicating differences among individuals. The best way to describe our results is that in their perceptual evaluation of roughness, different individuals weight temporal information, spatial information, and other mechanical properties differently.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Dedos/fisiología , Fricción/fisiología , Vidrio/química , Individualidad , Papel , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Animales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Piel , Propiedades de Superficie , Vibración , Adulto Joven
11.
Iperception ; 9(3): 2041669518781141, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29977491

RESUMEN

A miniature hair clip set-up presented to the first author gave inspiration for this study. After a number of studies investigating what is haptically perceived as parallel on horizontal, frontoparallel or midsagittal planes, the present study focusses on what is felt as parallel behind your head. The results show convincingly that also in this condition physically parallel is not the same as haptically parallel. Moreover, the deviations are large, idiosyncratic and in a direction predicted by assuming a biasing influence of an egocentric reference frame.

12.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; 11(4): 485-497, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30004889

RESUMEN

We aim to achieve intraoperative localization of an early-stage gastric tumor that cannot be visually detected during laparoscopic surgery. In this study, we developed and evaluated a pneumatic tactile ring, which is a clinically applicable tactile device to provide instantaneous feedback from a tactile sensor directly manipulated by a surgeon. It was designed to be worn on the finger of the manipulating hand and to present pressure to the finger pad. It is lightweight, cost-effective, disposable, and sterilizable. We also developed a compact pneumatic drive unit to control the pressure and investigated its fundamental performance. The bandwidth of the pressure control was at least 1.3 Hz with a controllable range of up to 79.7 kPa. Moreover, a psychophysical experiment was performed to obtain the Weber ratio of the pressure and evaluate the effectiveness of the instantaneous tactile feedback of the sensor output through the tactile ring. The Weber ratio was 0.40 at the reference pressure of 22.7 kPa. The provided tactile feedback significantly reduced the absolute localization error and increased participants' confidence in their answers. It was shown that the tactile feedback through the ring is effective in laparoscopic tumor localization.


Asunto(s)
Diseño de Equipo , Retroalimentación Sensorial/fisiología , Dedos/fisiología , Laparoscopía/instrumentación , Neoplasias Gástricas/cirugía , Cirujanos , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Humanos
13.
Sci Rep ; 8(1): 3001, 2018 02 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29445191

RESUMEN

Many studies have already shown that a large idiosyncratic orientation difference is needed to perceive two bars that are far apart as haptically parallel. There exist also strong indications that if such bars are imagined to be minute hands of clocks, errors made in clock time estimates and clock time settings are much smaller. The current study investigated this seemingly discrepancy. Participants partook in three experiments: parallel setting, clock time estimate and clock time setting, in this order. As the individual parallel settings were used in the subsequent clock time estimate experiment, and the estimated clock times in the clock time setting experiment, the deviations could be compared directly. In all three experiments, the deviations were systematic and idiosyncratic, and consistent with a biasing influence of an egocentric reference frame. However, the deviations in the two clock time experiments were indeed much smaller than in the parallel setting experiment. Task instruction and strengthened focus on an allocentric reference frame are the most likely explanations. These findings provide fundamental insights in the processing of spatial information. Taking these findings into account when designing haptic devices may make these more intuitive.

14.
Sci Rep ; 8(1): 377, 2018 01 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29321557

RESUMEN

In three experiments participants haptically discriminated object shape using unimanual (single hand explored two objects) and bimanual exploration (both hands were used, but each hand, left or right, explored a separate object). Such haptic exploration (one versus two hands) requires somatosensory processing in either only one or both cerebral hemispheres; previous studies related to the perception of shape/curvature found superior performance for unimanual exploration, indicating that shape comparison is more effective when only one hemisphere is utilized. The current results, obtained for naturally shaped solid objects (bell peppers, Capsicum annuum) and simple cylindrical surfaces demonstrate otherwise: bimanual haptic exploration can be as effective as unimanual exploration, showing that there is no necessary reduction in ability when haptic shape comparison requires interhemispheric communication. We found that while successive bimanual exploration produced high shape discriminability, the participants' bimanual performance deteriorated for simultaneous shape comparisons. This outcome suggests that either interhemispheric interference or the need to attend to multiple objects simultaneously reduces shape discrimination ability. The current results also reveal a significant effect of age: older adults' shape discrimination abilities are moderately reduced relative to younger adults, regardless of how objects are manipulated (left hand only, right hand only, or bimanual exploration).


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Mano/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Desempeño Psicomotor , Adulto Joven
15.
Int J Med Robot ; 14(2)2018 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29266695

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: A surgeon's tactile sense can contribute to intraoperative tumor detection, but it is limited by laparoscopic surgery. METHODS: We have developed a simple and biocompatible tactile sensor. This study aimed to design and evaluate visual and tactile feedback from the sensor for laparoscopic tumor detection. A line graph was offered through a monitor as the visual feedback. A normal force was presented to the user's foot as the tactile feedback. Twelve novices conducted a task of detecting a phantom tumor under 4 conditions (no feedback, visual feedback, tactile feedback and a combination of both types of feedback). RESULTS: The visual feedback was significantly more effective in detection than no feedback. Moreover, both visual and tactile feedback led to safer manipulation with significantly smaller load and lower scanning speed, respectively. CONCLUSIONS: The results suggest that visual and tactile feedback can be useful for laparoscopic palpation; however, their effects depend on the means in which they are presented.


Asunto(s)
Retroalimentación Sensorial , Laparoscopía/métodos , Palpación , Tacto , Adolescente , Retroalimentación , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
16.
Sci Rep ; 7: 43868, 2017 03 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28252015

RESUMEN

We can efficiently detect whether there is a rough object among a set of smooth objects using our sense of touch. We can also quickly determine the number of rough objects in our hand. In this study, we investigated whether the perceptual processing of rough and smooth objects is influenced if these objects are connected. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to identify whether there were exactly two rough target spheres among smooth distractor spheres, while we recorded their response times. The spheres were connected to form pairs: rough spheres were paired together and smooth spheres were paired together ('within pairs arrangement'), or a rough and a smooth sphere were connected ('between pairs arrangement'). Participants responded faster when the spheres in a pair were identical. In Experiment 2, we found that the advantage for within pairs arrangements was not driven by feature saliency. Overall our results show that haptic information is processed faster when targets were connected together compared to when targets were connected to distractors.

17.
PLoS One ; 11(7): e0158709, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27438009

RESUMEN

Visuo-haptic biases are observed when bringing your unseen hand to a visual target. The biases are different between, but consistent within participants. We investigated the usefulness of adjusting haptic guidance to these user-specific biases in aligning haptic and visual perception. By adjusting haptic guidance according to the biases, we aimed to reduce the conflict between the modalities. We first measured the biases using an adaptive procedure. Next, we measured performance in a pointing task using three conditions: 1) visual images that were adjusted to user-specific biases, without haptic guidance, 2) veridical visual images combined with haptic guidance, and 3) shifted visual images combined with haptic guidance. Adding haptic guidance increased precision. Combining haptic guidance with user-specific visual information yielded the highest accuracy and the lowest level of conflict with the guidance at the end point. These results show the potential of correcting for user-specific perceptual biases when designing haptic guidance.


Asunto(s)
Fenómenos Fisiológicos Oculares , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Mano/fisiología , Humanos , Imagenología Tridimensional , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología
18.
Exp Brain Res ; 234(11): 3367-3379, 2016 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27450079

RESUMEN

In this study, we investigated the integration of force and position information in a task in which participants were asked to estimate the center of a weak force field. Two hypotheses, describing how participants solved this task, were tested: (1) by only using the position(s) where the force reaches the detection threshold, and (2) by extrapolating the force field based on perceived stiffness. Both hypotheses were also described formally, assuming a psychophysical function obeying a power law with an exponent smaller than one. The hypotheses were tested in two psychophysical experiments, in which 12 participants took part. In Experiment 1, an asymmetric force field was used and the presence of visual feedback about hand position was varied. In Experiment 2, a unilateral force field was used. For both experiments, hypothesis 1 predicts biases between (Experiment 1) or at (Experiment 2) the position(s) of the force detection threshold, while hypothesis 2 predicts smaller biases. The measured data show significant biases in both experiments that coincide with the biases predicted by using force detection thresholds from the literature. The average measured responses and their variabilities also fitted very well with the mathematical model of hypothesis 1. These results underline the validity of hypothesis 1. So, participants did not use a percept of the stiffness of the force field, but based their estimation of the center of the force field on the position(s) where the force reached the detection threshold. This shows that force and position information were not integrated in this task.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Psicológicos , Percepción/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Mano , Humanos , Masculino , Propiocepción/fisiología , Psicofísica , Umbral Sensorial , Tacto , Adulto Joven
20.
Sci Rep ; 6: 25473, 2016 05 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27137417

RESUMEN

Our tactual perceptual experiences occur when we interact, actively and passively, with environmental objects and surfaces. Previous research has demonstrated that active manual exploration often enhances the tactual perception of object shape. Nevertheless, the factors that contribute to this enhancement are not well understood. The present study evaluated the ability of 28 younger (mean age was 23.1 years) and older adults (mean age was 71.4 years) to discriminate curved surfaces by actively feeling objects with a single index finger and by passively feeling objects that moved relative to a restrained finger. While dynamic cutaneous stimulation was therefore present in both conditions, active exploratory movements only occurred in one. The results indicated that there was a significant and large effect of age, such that the older participants' thresholds were 43.8 percent higher than those of the younger participants. Despite the overall adverse effect of age, the pattern of results across the active and passive touch conditions was identical. For both age groups, the curvature discrimination thresholds obtained for passive touch were significantly lower than those that occurred during active touch. Curvature discrimination performance was therefore best in the current study when dynamic cutaneous stimulation occurred in the absence of active movement.


Asunto(s)
Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Fenómenos Fisiológicos de la Piel , Tacto/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Envejecimiento/patología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Femenino , Dedos/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Movimiento
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...