Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 58
Filtrar
1.
Science ; 379(6632): 541-543, 2023 02 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36758092

RESUMEN

Understanding moral acceptability and willingness to use is crucial for informing policy.


Asunto(s)
Embrión de Mamíferos , Edición Génica , Pruebas Genéticas , Herencia Multifactorial , Pruebas Genéticas/ética , Riesgo , Humanos , Edición Génica/ética , Formulación de Políticas , Estados Unidos
2.
Am J Health Promot ; 37(3): 324-332, 2023 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36195982

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To evaluate if nudges delivered by text message prior to an upcoming primary care visit can increase influenza vaccination rates. DESIGN: Randomized, controlled trial. SETTING: Two health systems in the Northeastern US between September 2020 and March 2021. SUBJECTS: 74,811 adults. INTERVENTIONS: Patients in the 19 intervention arms received 1-2 text messages in the 3 days preceding their appointment that varied in their format, interactivity, and content. MEASURES: Influenza vaccination. ANALYSIS: Intention-to-treat. RESULTS: Participants had a mean (SD) age of 50.7 (16.2) years; 55.8% (41,771) were female, 70.6% (52,826) were White, and 19.0% (14,222) were Black. Among the interventions, 5 of 19 (26.3%) had a significantly greater vaccination rate than control. On average, the 19 interventions increased vaccination relative to control by 1.8 percentage points or 6.1% (P = .005). The top performing text message described the vaccine to the patient as "reserved for you" and led to a 3.1 percentage point increase (95% CI, 1.3 to 4.9; P < .001) in vaccination relative to control. Three of the top five performing messages described the vaccine as "reserved for you." None of the interventions performed worse than control. CONCLUSIONS: Text messages encouraging vaccination and delivered prior to an upcoming appointment significantly increased influenza vaccination rates and could be a scalable approach to increase vaccination more broadly.


Asunto(s)
Vacunas contra la Influenza , Gripe Humana , Envío de Mensajes de Texto , Adulto , Humanos , Femenino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Masculino , Gripe Humana/prevención & control , Sistemas Recordatorios , Vacunación , Atención Primaria de Salud
3.
Nat Genet ; 54(4): 437-449, 2022 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35361970

RESUMEN

We conduct a genome-wide association study (GWAS) of educational attainment (EA) in a sample of ~3 million individuals and identify 3,952 approximately uncorrelated genome-wide-significant single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). A genome-wide polygenic predictor, or polygenic index (PGI), explains 12-16% of EA variance and contributes to risk prediction for ten diseases. Direct effects (i.e., controlling for parental PGIs) explain roughly half the PGI's magnitude of association with EA and other phenotypes. The correlation between mate-pair PGIs is far too large to be consistent with phenotypic assortment alone, implying additional assortment on PGI-associated factors. In an additional GWAS of dominance deviations from the additive model, we identify no genome-wide-significant SNPs, and a separate X-chromosome additive GWAS identifies 57.


Asunto(s)
Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Herencia Multifactorial , Humanos , Herencia Multifactorial/genética , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple/genética
4.
Nat Hum Behav ; 5(12): 1744-1758, 2021 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34140656

RESUMEN

Polygenic indexes (PGIs) are DNA-based predictors. Their value for research in many scientific disciplines is growing rapidly. As a resource for researchers, we used a consistent methodology to construct PGIs for 47 phenotypes in 11 datasets. To maximize the PGIs' prediction accuracies, we constructed them using genome-wide association studies-some not previously published-from multiple data sources, including 23andMe and UK Biobank. We present a theoretical framework to help interpret analyses involving PGIs. A key insight is that a PGI can be understood as an unbiased but noisy measure of a latent variable we call the 'additive SNP factor'. Regressions in which the true regressor is this factor but the PGI is used as its proxy therefore suffer from errors-in-variables bias. We derive an estimator that corrects for the bias, illustrate the correction, and make a Python tool for implementing it publicly available.


Asunto(s)
Bases de Datos Genéticas , Herencia Multifactorial , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Análisis de Datos , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Humanos
5.
N Engl J Med ; 385(1): 78-86, 2021 07 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34192436

RESUMEN

Companies have recently begun to sell a new service to patients considering in vitro fertilization: embryo selection based on polygenic scores (ESPS). These scores represent individualized predictions of health and other outcomes derived from genomewide association studies in adults to partially predict these outcomes. This article includes a discussion of many factors that lower the predictive power of polygenic scores in the context of embryo selection and quantifies these effects for a variety of clinical and nonclinical traits. Also discussed are potential unintended consequences of ESPS (including selecting for adverse traits, altering population demographics, exacerbating inequalities in society, and devaluing certain traits). Recommendations for the responsible communication about ESPS by practitioners are provided, and a call for a society-wide conversation about this technology is made. (Funded by the National Institute on Aging and others.).


Asunto(s)
Embrión de Mamíferos , Fertilización In Vitro , Pruebas Genéticas , Variación Genética , Herencia Multifactorial/genética , Fenotipo , Diagnóstico Preimplantación , Escolaridad , Interacción Gen-Ambiente , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Humanos , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas
6.
Organ Behav Hum Decis Process ; 163: 6-16, 2021 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33986563

RESUMEN

Home-delivered prescriptions have no delivery charge and lower copayments than prescriptions picked up at a pharmacy. Nevertheless, when home delivery is offered on an opt-in basis, the take-up rate is only 6%. We study a program that makes active choice of either home delivery or pharmacy pick-up a requirement for insurance eligibility. The program introduces an implicit default for those who don't make an active choice: pharmacy pick-up without insurance subsidies. Under this program, 42% of eligible employees actively choose home delivery, 39% actively choose pharmacy pick-up, and 19% make no active choice and are assigned the implicit default. Individuals who financially benefit most from home delivery are more likely to choose it. Those who benefit least from insurance subsidies are more likely to make no active choice and lose those subsidies. The implicit default incentivizes people to make an active choice, thereby playing a key role in choice architecture.

7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 118(20)2021 05 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33926993

RESUMEN

Many Americans fail to get life-saving vaccines each year, and the availability of a vaccine for COVID-19 makes the challenge of encouraging vaccination more urgent than ever. We present a large field experiment (N = 47,306) testing 19 nudges delivered to patients via text message and designed to boost adoption of the influenza vaccine. Our findings suggest that text messages sent prior to a primary care visit can boost vaccination rates by an average of 5%. Overall, interventions performed better when they were 1) framed as reminders to get flu shots that were already reserved for the patient and 2) congruent with the sort of communications patients expected to receive from their healthcare provider (i.e., not surprising, casual, or interactive). The best-performing intervention in our study reminded patients twice to get their flu shot at their upcoming doctor's appointment and indicated it was reserved for them. This successful script could be used as a template for campaigns to encourage the adoption of life-saving vaccines, including against COVID-19.


Asunto(s)
Vacunas contra la COVID-19 , COVID-19/prevención & control , Vacunas contra la Influenza , Gripe Humana/prevención & control , Visita a Consultorio Médico/estadística & datos numéricos , Vacunación/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Médicos de Atención Primaria , Sistemas Recordatorios , Envío de Mensajes de Texto , Vacunación/psicología
8.
Exp Econ ; 23(4): 1069-1099, 2020 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33343223

RESUMEN

The compromise effect arises when being close to the "middle" of a choice set makes an option more appealing. The compromise effect poses conceptual and practical problems for economic research: by influencing choices, it can bias researchers' inferences about preference parameters. To study this bias, we conduct an experiment with 550 participants who made choices over lotteries from multiple price lists (MPLs). Following prior work, we manipulate the compromise effect to influence choices by varying the middle options of each MPL. We then estimate risk preferences using a discrete-choice model without a compromise effect embedded in the model. As anticipated, the resulting risk preference parameter estimates are not robust, changing as the compromise effect is manipulated. To disentangle risk preference parameters from the compromise effect and to measure the strength of the compromise effect, we augment our discrete-choice model with additional parameters that represent a rising penalty for expressing an indifference point further from the middle of the ordered MPL. Using this method, we estimate an economically significant magnitude for the compromise effect and generate robust estimates of risk preference parameters that are no longer sensitive to compromise-effect manipulations.

9.
Health Serv Res ; 55(4): 503-511, 2020 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32700389

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To test the effectiveness of physician incentives for increasing patient medication adherence in three drug classes: diabetes medication, antihypertensives, and statins. DATA SOURCES: Pharmacy and medical claims from a large Medicare Advantage Prescription Drug Plan from January 2011 to December 2012. STUDY DESIGN: We conducted a randomized experiment (911 primary care practices and 8,935 nonadherent patients) to test the effect of paying physicians for increasing patient medication adherence in three drug classes: diabetes medication, antihypertensives, and statins. We measured patients' medication adherence for 18 (6) months before (after) the intervention. DATA COLLECTION/EXTRACTION METHODS: We obtained data directly from the health insurer. PRINCIPAL FINDINGS: We found no evidence that physician incentives increased adherence in any drug class. Our results rule out increases in the proportion of days covered by medication larger than 4.2 percentage points. CONCLUSIONS: Physician incentives of $50 per patient per drug class are not effective for increasing patient medication adherence among the drug classes and primary care practices studied. Such incentives may be more likely to improve measures under physicians' direct control rather than those that predominantly reflect patient behaviors. Additional research is warranted to disentangle whether physician effort is not responsive to these types of incentives, or medication adherence is not responsive to physician effort. Our results suggest that significant changes in the incentive amount or program design may be necessary to produce responses from physicians or patients.


Asunto(s)
Cumplimiento de la Medicación/psicología , Cumplimiento de la Medicación/estadística & datos numéricos , Motivación , Satisfacción del Paciente/estadística & datos numéricos , Planes de Incentivos para los Médicos/organización & administración , Médicos/economía , Atención Primaria de Salud/organización & administración , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
10.
J Public Econ ; 1832020 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32189814

RESUMEN

Previous research has shown that some people voluntarily use commitment contracts that restrict their own choice sets. We study how people divide money between two accounts: a liquid account that permits unrestricted withdrawals and a commitment account that is randomly assigned in a between-subject design to have either a 10% early withdrawal penalty, or a 20% early withdrawal penalty, or not to allow early withdrawals at all (i.e., an infinite penalty). When the liquid account and the commitment account pay the same interest rate, higher early-withdrawal penalties attract more commitment account deposits. This pattern is predicted by the hypothesis that some participants are partially- or fully-sophisticated present-biased agents. Such agents perceive that higher penalties generate greater scope for commitment by disincentivizing (penalized) early withdrawals. The experiment also shows that when the commitment account pays a higher interest rate than the liquid account, the positive empirical slope relating penalties and commitment deposits is flattened, suggesting that naïve present-biased agents or agents with standard exponential discounting are also in our sample. Across all of our experimental treatments, higher early withdrawal penalties on the commitment account sometimes increase and never reduce allocations to the commitment account.

11.
J Econ Lit ; 58(2): 299-347, 2020 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37691693

RESUMEN

We review research that measures time preferences-i.e., preferences over intertemporal tradeoffs. We distinguish between studies using financial flows, which we call "money earlier or later" (MEL) decisions and studies that use time-dated consumption/effort. Under different structural models, we show how to translate what MEL experiments directly measure (required rates of return for financial flows) into a discount function over utils. We summarize empirical regularities found in MEL studies and the predictive power of those studies. We explain why MEL choices are driven in part by some factors that are distinct from underlying time preferences.

12.
J Econ Behav Organ ; 179: 743-756, 2020 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33424063

RESUMEN

How well do pre-school delay of gratification and life-course measures of self-regulation predict mid-life capital formation? We surveyed 113 participants of the 1967-1973 Bing pre-school studies on delay of gratification when they were in their late 40's. They reported 11 mid-life capital formation outcomes, including net worth, permanent income, absence of high-interest debt, forward-looking behaviors, and educational attainment. To address multiple hypothesis testing and our small sample, we pre-registered an analysis plan of well-powered tests. As predicted, a newly constructed and pre-registered measure derived from preschool delay of gratification does not predict the 11 capital formation variables (i.e., the sign-adjusted average correlation was 0.02). A pre-registered composite self-regulation index, combining preschool delay of gratification with survey measures of self-regulation collected at ages 17, 27, and 37, does predict 10 of the 11 capital formation variables in the expected direction, with an average correlation of 0.19. The inclusion of the preschool delay of gratification measure in this composite index does not affect the index's predictive power. We tested several hypothesized reasons that preschool delay of gratification does not have predictive power for our mid-life capital formation variables.

13.
Nat Genet ; 51(8): 1295, 2019 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31239548

RESUMEN

In the version of the paper initially published, no competing interests were declared. The 'Competing interests' statement should have stated that B.M.N. is on the Scientific Advisory Board of Deep Genomics. The error has been corrected in the HTML and PDF versions of the article.

15.
Nat Genet ; 50(2): 229-237, 2018 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29292387

RESUMEN

We introduce multi-trait analysis of GWAS (MTAG), a method for joint analysis of summary statistics from genome-wide association studies (GWAS) of different traits, possibly from overlapping samples. We apply MTAG to summary statistics for depressive symptoms (N eff = 354,862), neuroticism (N = 168,105), and subjective well-being (N = 388,538). As compared to the 32, 9, and 13 genome-wide significant loci identified in the single-trait GWAS (most of which are themselves novel), MTAG increases the number of associated loci to 64, 37, and 49, respectively. Moreover, association statistics from MTAG yield more informative bioinformatics analyses and increase the variance explained by polygenic scores by approximately 25%, matching theoretical expectations.


Asunto(s)
Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo/métodos , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo/estadística & datos numéricos , Herencia Multifactorial , Sitios de Carácter Cuantitativo , Algoritmos , Conjuntos de Datos como Asunto/estadística & datos numéricos , Depresión/epidemiología , Depresión/genética , Autoevaluación Diagnóstica , Estudios de Asociación Genética/métodos , Estudios de Asociación Genética/estadística & datos numéricos , Salud/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos , Metaanálisis como Asunto , Neuroticismo , Fenotipo , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Sitios de Carácter Cuantitativo/genética
17.
Psychol Sci Public Interest ; 19(3): 102-129, 2018 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30760176

RESUMEN

Almost everyone struggles to act in their individual and collective best interests, particularly when doing so requires forgoing a more immediately enjoyable alternative. Other than exhorting decision makers to "do the right thing," what can policymakers do to reduce overeating, undersaving, procrastination, and other self-defeating behaviors that feel good now but generate larger delayed costs? In this review, we synthesize contemporary research on approaches to reducing failures of self-control. We distinguish between self-deployed and other-deployed strategies and, in addition, between situational and cognitive intervention targets. Collectively, the evidence from both psychological science and economics recommends psychologically informed policies for reducing failures of self-control.


Asunto(s)
Control de la Conducta/métodos , Economía del Comportamiento , Autocontrol , Toma de Decisiones , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos
18.
J Public Econ ; 151: 84-95, 2017 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28966407

RESUMEN

Can governments increase private savings by taxing savings up front instead of in retirement? Roth 401(k) contributions are not tax-deductible in the contribution year, but withdrawals in retirement are untaxed. The more common before-tax 401(k) contribution is tax-deductible in the contribution year, but both principal and investment earnings are taxed upon withdrawal. Using administrative data from eleven companies that added a Roth contribution option to their existing 401(k) plan between 2006 and 2010, we find no evidence that total 401(k) contribution rates differ between employees hired before versus after Roth introduction, which implies that take-home pay declines and the amount of retirement consumption being purchased by 401(k) contributions increases after Roth introduction. We reject several neoclassical explanations for our null finding. Results from a survey experiment suggest two behavioral explanations: (1) employee confusion about and neglect of the tax properties of Roth balances and (2) partition dependence.

19.
AIDS ; 31(12): 1765-1769, 2017 07 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28514277

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Assess whether a commitment contract informed by behavioral economics leads to persistent virologic suppression among HIV-positive patients with poor antiretroviral therapy (ART) adherence. DESIGN: Single-center pilot randomized clinical trial and a nonrandomized control group. SETTING: Publicly funded HIV clinic in Atlanta, Georgia, USA. INTERVENTION: The study involved three arms. First, participants in the provider visit incentive (PVI) arm received $30 after attending each scheduled provider visit. Second, participants in the incentive choice arm were given a choice between the above arrangement and a commitment contract that made the $30 payment conditional on both attending the provider visit and meeting an ART adherence threshold. Third, the passive control arm received routine care and no incentives. PARTICIPANTS: A total of 110 HIV-infected adults with a recent plasma HIV-1 viral load more than 200 copies/ml despite ART. The sample sizes of the three groups were as follows: PVI, n = 21; incentive choice, n = 19; and passive control, n = 70. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURE: Virologic suppression (plasma HIV-1 viral load ≤200 copies/ml) at the end of the incentive period and at an unanticipated postincentive study visit approximately 3 months later. RESULTS: The odds of suppression were higher in the incentive choice arm than in the passive control arm at the postincentive visit (adjusted odds ratio 3.93, 95% confidence interval 1.19-13.04, P = 0.025). The differences relative to the passive control arm at the end of the incentive period and relative to the PVI arm at both points in time were not statistically significant. CONCLUSION: Commitment contracts can improve ART adherence and virologic suppression. TRIAL REGISTRATION: ClinicalTrials.gov identifier NCT01455740.


Asunto(s)
Fármacos Anti-VIH/uso terapéutico , Infecciones por VIH/tratamiento farmacológico , Cumplimiento de la Medicación , Motivación , Respuesta Virológica Sostenida , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Georgia , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Proyectos Piloto , Resultado del Tratamiento
20.
Rev Financ Stud ; 30(6): 1971-2005, 2017 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28553012

RESUMEN

Many experiments have found that participants take more investment risk if they see returns less frequently, see portfolio-level returns (rather than each individual asset's returns), or see long-horizon (rather than one-year) historical return distributions. In contrast, we find that such information aggregation treatments do not affect total equity investment when we make the investment environment more realistic than in prior experiments. Previously documented aggregation effects are not robust to changes in the risky asset's return distribution or the introduction of a multi-day delay between portfolio choice and return realizations.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA