Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 30
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
BMC Ecol Evol ; 24(1): 60, 2024 May 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38734594

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Foraging behavior in insects is optimised for locating scattered resources in a complex environment. This behavior can be exploited for use in pest control. Inhibition of feeding can protect crops whereas stimulation can increase the uptake of insecticides. For example, the success of a bait spray, depends on either contact or ingestion, and thus on the insect finding it. METHODS: To develop an effective bait spray against the invasive pest, Drosophila suzukii, we investigated aspects of foraging behavior that influence the likelihood that the pest interacts with the baits, in summer and winter morphotypes. We video-recorded the flies' approach behavior towards four stimuli in a two-choice experiment on strawberry leaflets. To determine the most effective bait positioning, we also assessed where on plants the pest naturally forages, using a potted raspberry plant under natural environmental conditions. We also studied starvation resistance at 20 °C and 12 °C for both morphs. RESULTS: We found that summer morph flies spent similar time on all baits (agar, combi-protec, yeast) whereas winter morphs spent more time on yeast than the other baits. Both morphs showed a preference to feed at the top of our plant's canopy. Colder temperatures enhanced survival under starvation conditions in both morphs, and mortality was reduced by food treatment. CONCLUSIONS: These findings on feeding behavior support informed decisions on the type and placement of a bait to increase pest control.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila , Conducta Alimentaria , Control de Insectos , Animales , Drosophila/fisiología , Control de Insectos/métodos , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Insecticidas/farmacología , Insecticidas/administración & dosificación , Rubus , Fragaria , Femenino , Estaciones del Año
2.
Infect Prev Pract ; 6(2): 100352, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38510847

RESUMEN

Background: Taurolidine containing lock solutions (TL) are a promising method for the prevention of central line associated bloodstream infections. Per accident, the TL may not always be aspirated from the central venous catheter (CVC) before blood cultures are obtained. The TL could, unintentionally, end up in a blood culture vial, possibly altering the results. The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of the TLs on the detection of microbial growth in blood culture vials. Methods: Different lock solutions (taurolidine-citrate-heparin (TCHL), taurolidine, heparin, citrate or NaCl) were added to BD BACTECTM blood culture vials (Plus Aerobic/F, Lytic/10 Anaerobic/F or Peds Plus/F) before spiking with Staphylococcus aureus (ATCC 29213 or a clinical strain) or Escherichia coli (ATCC 25922 or a clinical strain) in the presence and absence of blood. Subsequently, blood culture vials were incubated in the BD BACTEC FX instrument with Time-to-positivity (TTP) as primary outcome. In addition, the effect of the TCHL on a variety of other micro-organisms was tested. Discussion: In the presence of taurolidine, the TTP was considerably delayed or vials even remained negative as compared to vials containing heparin, citrate or NaCl. This effect was dose-dependent. The delayed TTP was much less pronounced in the presence of blood, but still notable. Conclusion: This study stresses the clinical importance of discarding TLs from the CVC before obtaining a blood culture.

3.
Vision Res ; 45(4): 485-96, 2005 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15610752

RESUMEN

To correctly perceive visual directions relative to the head, one needs to compensate for the eye's orientation in the head. In this study we focus on compensation for the eye's torsion regarding objects that contain the line of sight and objects that do not pass through the fixation point. Subjects judged the location of flashed probe points relative to their binocular plane of regard, the mid-sagittal or the transverse plane of the head, while fixating straight ahead, right upward, or right downward at 30 cm distance, to evoke eye torsion according to Listing's law. In addition, we investigated the effects of head-tilt and monocular versus binocular viewing. Flashed probe points were correctly localized in the plane of regard irrespective of eccentric viewing, head-tilt, and monocular or binocular vision in nearly all subjects and conditions. Thus, eye torsion that varied by +/-9 degrees across these different conditions was in general compensated for. However, the position of probes relative to the midsagittal or the transverse plane, both true head-fixed planes, was misjudged. We conclude that judgment of the orientation of the plane of regard, a plane that contains the line of sight, is veridical, indicating accurate compensation for actual eye torsion. However, when judgment has to be made of a head-fixed plane that is offset with respect to the line of sight, eye torsion that accompanies that eye orientation appears not to be taken into account correctly.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Movimientos de la Cabeza/fisiología , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Reflejo Vestibuloocular/fisiología , Rotación , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Visión Monocular/fisiología
4.
Vision Res ; 43(2): 117-33, 2003 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12536135

RESUMEN

Strength of the motion aftereffect (MAE) is most often quantified by its duration, a high-variance and rather 'subjective' measure. With the help of an automatic gain-control model we quantitatively relate nulling-thresholds, adaptation strength, direction discrimination threshold, and duration of the dynamic MAE (dMAE). This shows how the nulling threshold, a more objective two-alternative forced-choice measure, relates to the same system property as MAE-durations. Two psychophysical experiments to test the model use moving random-pixel-arrays with an adjustable luminance signal-to-noise ratio. We measure MAE-duration as a function of adaptation strength and compare the results to the model prediction. We then do the same for nulling-thresholds. Model predictions are strongly supported by the psychophysical findings. In a third experiment we test formulae coupling nulling threshold, MAE-duration, and direction-discrimination thresholds, by measuring these quantities as a function of speed. For the medium-to-high speed range of these experiments we found that nulling thresholds increase and dMAE-durations decrease about linearly, whereas direction discrimination thresholds increase exponentially with speed. The model description then suggests that the motion-gain decreases, while the noise-gain and model's threshold increase with speed.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Psicológicos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Humanos , Psicofísica , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
5.
Vision Res ; 42(1): 65-73, 2002 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11804632

RESUMEN

We studied effects of dark adaptation on spatial and temporal tuning for motion coherence detection. We compared tuning for step size and delay for moving random pixel arrays (RPAs) at two adaptation levels, one light adapted (50 cd/m(2)) and the other relatively dark adapted (0.05 cd/m(2)). To study coherence detection rather than contrast detection, RPAs were scaled for equal contrast detection at each luminance level, and a signal-to-noise ratio paradigm was used in which the RPA is always at a fixed, supra-threshold contrast level. The noise consists of a spatio-temporally incoherent RPA added to the moving RPA on a pixel-by-pixel basis. Spatial and temporal limits for coherence detection were measured using a single step pattern lifetime stimulus, in which patterns on alternate frames make a coherent step and are being refreshed. Therefore, the stimulus contains coherent motion at a single combination of step size and delay only. The main effect of dark adaptation is a large shift in step size, slightly less than the adjustment of spatial scale required for maintaining equal contrast sensitivity. A similar change of preferred step size occurs also for scaled stimuli at a light-adapted level, indicating that the spatial effect is not directly linked to dark adaptation, but more generally related to changes in the available low-level spatial information. Dark-adaptation shifts temporal tuning by about a factor of 2. Long delays are more effective at low luminance levels, whereas short delays no longer support motion coherence detection. Luminance-invariant velocity tuning curves, as reported previously [Lankheet, M.J.M., van Doorn, A.J., Bouman, M.A., & van de Grind, W.A. (2000) Motion coherence detection as a function of luminance in human central vision. Vision Research, 40, 3599-3611], result from recruitment of different sets of motion detectors, and an adjustment of their temporal properties.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación a la Oscuridad/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reclutamiento Neurofisiológico
6.
Vision Res ; 40(26): 3599-611, 2000.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11116164

RESUMEN

We studied the changes and invariances of foveal motion detection upon dark adaptation. It is well-documented that dark adaptation affects both spatial and temporal aspects of visual processing. The question we were interested in is how this alters motion coherence detection for moving random texture. To compare motion sensitivity at different adaptation levels, we adjusted the viewing distance for equal detectability of a stationary pattern. At these viewing distances we then measured velocity tuning curves for moving random pixel arrays (RPAs). Mean luminance levels ranged from 50 down to 0.005 cd m-2. Our main conclusion is that foveal velocity tuning is amazingly close to luminance-invariant, down to a level of 0.05 cd m-2. Because different viewing distances, and hence, retinal image sizes were used, we performed two control experiments to assess variations of these two parameters separately. We examined the effects of retinal inhomogeneities using discs of different size and annuli filled with RPAs. Our conclusion is that the central visual field, including the near periphery is still rather homogeneous for motion detection at 0.05 cd m-2, but the fovea becomes unresponsive at the lowest luminance level. Variations in viewing distance had marked effects on velocity tuning, both at the light adapted level and the 0.05 cd m-2 level. The size and type of these changes indicated the effectiveness of distance scaling, and show that deviations from perfect invariance of motion coherence detection were not due to inaccurate distance scaling.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Ocular/fisiología , Iluminación , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Umbral Diferencial , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Fóvea Central/fisiología , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad
7.
J Neurosci ; 20(5): 2043-53, 2000 Mar 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10684905

RESUMEN

Optical blurring in the eye prevents conventional physiological techniques from revealing the fine structure of the small parvocellular receptive fields in the primate fovea in vivo. We explored the organization of receptive fields in macaque parvocellular lateral geniculate nucleus cells by using sinusoidal interference fringes formed directly on the retina to measure spatial frequency tuning at different orientations. Most parvocellular cells in and near the fovea respond reliably to spatial frequencies up to and beyond 100 cycles/ degrees of visual angle, implying center input arising mainly from a single cone. Temporal frequency and contrast response characteristics were also measured at spatial frequencies up to 130 cycles/degrees. We compared our spatial frequency data with the frequency responses of model receptive fields that estimate the number, configuration, and weights of cones that feed the center and surround. On the basis of these comparisons, we infer possible underlying circuits. Most cells had irregular spatial frequency-response curves that imply center input from more than one cone. The measured responses are consistent with a single cone center together with weak input from nearby cones. By exposing a fine structure that cannot be discerned by conventional techniques, interferometry allows functional measurements of the early neural mechanisms in spatial vision.


Asunto(s)
Fóvea Central/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Tecnología de Fibra Óptica , Análisis de Fourier , Fóvea Central/citología , Interferometría/instrumentación , Interferometría/métodos , Rayos Láser , Luz , Macaca , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Distribución Normal , Agudeza Visual/fisiología
8.
Vision Res ; 38(5): 659-68, 1998 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9604098

RESUMEN

Stereoscopic segregation in depth was studied using two superimposed frontoparallel surfaces displayed in dynamic random dot stereograms. The two patterns were positioned symmetrically in front of and behind a binocular fixation point. They were either stationary, or they could move relative to each other. Sensitivity for segregation was established by adding gaussian distributed disparity noise to the disparities specifying the two planes, and finding the noise amplitude that gave threshold segregation performance. Observers easily segregate the two surfaces for disparity differences between approximately 6 and 30-40 arcmin. Motion contrast, which by itself provides no cue to perform the task, greatly improves sensitivity for segregation. Noise tolerance rises by a factor of two or more when the patterns move at different speeds, or in different (frontoparallel) directions. The effect increases with directional difference, but the optimal directional difference deviated from 180 deg. The optimal speed varies with disparity difference. Thus, motion and disparity must interact in order to resolve the two transparent planes.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Psicofísica , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Disparidad Visual
9.
Vis Neurosci ; 15(1): 37-46, 1998.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9456503

RESUMEN

We characterized the chromatic and temporal properties of a sample of 177 red-green parvocellular neurons in the LGN of Macaca nemestrina, using large-field stimuli modulated along different directions through a white point in color space. We examined differences among the properties of the four subclasses of red-green P-cells (on- and off-center, red and green center). The responses of off-center cells lag the stimulus more than do those of on-center cells. At low temporal frequencies, this causes the phase difference between responses of the two kinds of cells to be considerably less than 180 deg. For isoluminant modulations the phases of on- and off-responses were more nearly 180 deg apart. A cell's temporal characteristics did not depend on the class of cone driving its center. Red center and green center cells have characteristically different chromatic properties, expressed either as preferred elevations in color space, or as weights with which cells combine inputs from L- and M-cones. Red center cells are relatively more responsive to achromatic modulation, and attach relatively more weight to input from the cones driving the center. Off-center cells also attach relatively more weight than do on-center cells to input from the class of cone driving the center.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Macaca nemestrina/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Animales , Cuerpos Geniculados/citología , Estimulación Luminosa , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología
10.
Vis Neurosci ; 15(1): 47-54, 1998.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9456504

RESUMEN

We studied the interaction between the chromatic and temporal properties of parvocellular (P) neurons in the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) of macaque monkeys. We measured the amplitudes and phases of responses to stimulation by spatially uniform fields modulated sinusoidally about a white point in a three-dimensional color space, at a range of temporal frequencies between 1 and 25 Hz. Below about 4 Hz, temporal frequency had relatively little effect on chromatic tuning. At higher frequencies chromatic opponency was weakened in almost all cells. The complex interactions between temporal and chromatic properties are represented by a linear filter model that describes response amplitude and phase as a function of temporal frequency and direction in color space along which stimuli are modulated. The model stipulates the cone inputs to center and surround, their temporal properties, and the linear combination of center and surround signals. It predicts the amplitudes and phases of responses of P-cells, and the change of chromatic properties with temporal frequency. We used the model to investigate whether or not the chromatic signature of the surround in a red-green cell could be estimated from the change in the cell's chromatic properties with temporal frequency. Our findings could be equally well described by mixed cone surrounds as by pure cone surrounds, and we conclude that, with regard to temporal properties, there is no benefit to be gained by segregating cone classes in center and surround.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Animales , Electrofisiología , Macaca nemestrina/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología
11.
Vision Res ; 37(7): 839-52, 1997 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9156181

RESUMEN

The characteristics of directionally selective cells in area 17 of the cat are studied using moving random pixel arrays (RPAs) with 50% white and 50% black pixels. The apparent motion stimulus is similar to that used in human psychophysics [Fredericksen et al. (1993). Vision Research, 33, pp. 1193-1205]. We compare motion sensitivity measured with single-step pixel lifetimes and unlimited pixel lifetimes. A motion stimulus with a single-step pixel lifetime contains directional motion energy primarily at one combination of spatial displacement and temporal delay. We recorded the responses of complex cells to different combinations of displacement and delay to describe their spatio-temporal correlation characteristics. The response to motion of RPAs with unlimited lifetime is strongest along the preferred speed line in a delay vs displacement size diagram. When using an RPA with a single-step pixel lifetime, the cells are responsive to a much smaller range of spatial displacements and temporal delays of the stimulus. The maximum displacement that still gives a directionally selective response is larger when the preferred speed of the cell is higher. It is on average about three times smaller than the receptive field size.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Gatos , Potenciales Evocados Visuales , Femenino , Masculino , Ilusiones Ópticas/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
12.
Vision Res ; 36(24): 3955-67, 1996 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9068849

RESUMEN

We studied the change of spatial and temporal response properties for cat horizontal (H-) cells during prolonged dark adaptation. H-cell responses were recorded intracellularly in the optically intact, in vivo eye. Spatial and temporal properties were first measured for light-adapted H-cells, followed by a period of dark adaptation, after which the same measurements were repeated. During dark adaptation threshold sensitivity was measured at regular intervals. Stable, long lasting recordings allowed us to measure changes of sensitivity and receptive field characteristics for adaptation periods up to 45 min. Although cat H-cells showed no signs of dark suppression or light sensitization, they remained insensitive in the scotopic range, even after prolonged dark adaptation. Absolute thresholds were in the low mesopic range. The sensitization was brought about by a shift from cone to rod input, and by substantial increases of both spatial and temporal integration upon dark adaptation. The length constant in the light-adapted state was on average about 4 deg. After dark adaptation it was up to a factor of three larger, with a median ratio of 1.85. Response delays, latencies and durations for (equal amplitude) threshold flash responses substantially increased during dark adaptation.


Asunto(s)
Gatos/fisiología , Adaptación a la Oscuridad/fisiología , Retina/citología , Animales , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción , Retina/fisiología , Retina/efectos de la radiación , Umbral Sensorial , Factores de Tiempo
13.
Vision Res ; 36(24): 3969-85, 1996 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9068850

RESUMEN

First a model is presented that accurately summarizes the dynamic properties of cat horizontal (H-) cells under photopic conditions as measured in our previous work. The model predicts that asymmetries in response to dark as compared to light flashes are flash-duration dependent. This somewhat surprising prediction is tested and confirmed in intracellular recordings from the optically intact in vivo eye of the cat (Experiment 1). The model implies that the gain of H-cells should be related rather directly to the sustained (baseline) membrane potential. We performed three additional experiments to test this idea. Experiment 2 concerns response vs intensity (R-I-) curves for various flash-diameters and background-sizes with background luminance varying over a 4 log unit range. Results support the assumption of a rather strict coupling between flash sensitivity (gain) and the sustained level of hyperpolarization. In Experiment 3 we investigate this relation for both dark and light flashes given on each of four background light levels. The results suggest that there are fixed minimum and maximum hyperpolarization levels, and that the baseline hyperpolarization for a given illumination thus also sets the available range for dark and light flash-responses. The question then arises whether, or how this changes during dark adaptation, when the rod contribution to H-cell responses gradually increases. The fourth experiment therefore studies the relationship between gain and hyperpolarization level during prolonged dark-adaptation. The results show that the rod contribution increases the polarization range of H-cells, but that the gain and polarization level nevertheless remain directly coupled. H-cell models relying on a close coupling between polarization level and gain thus remain attractive options.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Ocular/fisiología , Gatos/fisiología , Retina/citología , Animales , Adaptación a la Oscuridad , Electrofisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulación Luminosa , Retina/fisiología , Retina/efectos de la radiación , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Bastones/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial
14.
Nature ; 384(6607): 358-60, 1996 Nov 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8934521

RESUMEN

The motion after-effect occurs after prolonged viewing of motion; a subsequent stationary scene is perceived as moving in the opposite direction. This illusion is thought to arise because motion is represented by the differential activities of populations of cortical neurons tuned to opposite directions; fatigue in one population leads to an imbalance that favours the opposite direction once the stimulus ceases. Following adaptation to multiple directions of motion, the after-effect is unidirectional, indicating that motion signals are integrated across all directions. Yet humans can perceive several directions of motion simultaneously. The question therefore arises as to how the visual system can perform both sharp segregation and global integration of motion signals. Here we show in computer simulations that this can occur if excitatory interactions between different directions are sharply tuned while inhibitory interactions are broadly tuned. Our model predicts that adaptation to simultaneous motion in opposite directions will lead to an orthogonal motion after-effect. This prediction was confirmed in psychophysical experiments. Thus, broadly tuned inhibitory interactions are likely to be important in the integration and segregation of motion signals. These interactions may occur in the cortical area MT, which contains motion-sensitive neurons with properties similar to those required by our model.


Asunto(s)
Postimagen , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Ilusiones Ópticas , Simulación por Computador , Humanos , Neuronas/fisiología
15.
Vis Neurosci ; 13(5): 885-96, 1996.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8903031

RESUMEN

The effects of dark adaptation on the response properties of ganglion cells have been documented extensively in the cat retina. To pinpoint the different retinal mechanisms that underlie these effects, we studied the response characteristics of cat horizontal (H) cells during prolonged dark adaptation. H-cell responses were recorded intracellularly in the optically intact, in vivo eye. To disentangle rod and cone contributions, sensitivity changes during dark adaptation were tracked with white light and with monochromatic lights that favored either rod or cone excitation. Stable, long-lasting recordings allowed us to measure changes of sensitivity for adaptation periods up to 45 min. Thresholds for white light and 503-nm monochromatic light decreased steadily and in parallel. The maximum increase of sensitivity, after extinguishing a photopic adaptation light, was 1.8 log units only, reached after about 35 min. Sensitivity for 581-nm lights also increased steadily, but at a shallower slope. The steady increase of sensitivity was concomitant with a linear shift in resting membrane potential and with an increase in relative rod contribution to the threshold responses. Even though small-amplitude responses were rod dominated after prolonged dark adaptation, sensitivity to rod signals remained relatively low, compared to sensitivity of cone responses or to the absolute sensitivity of ganglion cells. This suggests that the cone-H-cell pathway plays no role in the dark-adapted cat retina.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación a la Oscuridad/fisiología , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología , Células Ganglionares de la Retina/fisiología , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Bastones/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Adaptación Ocular , Animales , Gatos , Femenino , Masculino , Retina/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología
16.
Vision Res ; 36(18): 2805-13, 1996 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8917784

RESUMEN

We examined the responses to transparent motion of complex cells in cat area 17 which show directional selectivity to moving random pixel arrays (RPAs). The response to an RPA moving in the cell's preferred direction is inhibited when a second RPA is transparently moving in another direction. The inhibition by the second pattern is quantified as a function of its direction. The response to a pattern moving in the preferred direction is never completely suppressed, not even when a second pattern is moving transparently in the opposite direction. To the extent that supra-spontaneous firing rates signal the presence of the optimal velocity vector, these cells therefore still signal the presence of this line-label stimulus despite additional opposing, or otherwise directed, motion components. The results confirm previous suggestions that, for the computation of motion energy in cat area 17 complex cells, a full opponent stage is not plausible. Furthermore, we show that the response to a combination of two motion vectors can be predicted by the average of the responses to the individual components.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Gatos , Femenino , Masculino , Inhibición Neural , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Técnicas Estereotáxicas
17.
Biol Cybern ; 74(4): 299-308, 1996 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8936381

RESUMEN

We recorded intracellular responses from cat retinal ganglion cells to sinusoidal flickering lights, and compared the response dynamics with a theoretical model based on coupled nonlinear oscillators. Flicker responses for several different spot sizes were separated in a "smooth" generator (G) potential and corresponding spike trains. We have previously shown that the G-potential reveals complex, stimulus-dependent, oscillatory behavior in response to sinusoidally flickering lights. Such behavior could be simulated by a modified van der Pol oscillator. In this paper, we extend the model to account for spike generation as well, by including extended Hodgkin-Huxley equations describing local membrane properties. We quantified spike responses by several parameters describing the mean and standard deviation of spike burst duration, timing (phase shift) of bursts, and the number of spikes in a burst. The dependence of these response parameters on stimulus frequency and spot size could be reproduced in great detail by coupling the van der Pol oscillator and Hodgkin-Huxley equations. The model mimics many experimentally observed response patterns, including non-phase-locked irregular oscillations. Our findings suggest that the information in the ganglion cell spike train reflects both intraretinal processing, simulated by the van der Pol oscillator, and local membrane properties described by Hodgkin-Huxley equations. The interplay between these complex processes can be simulated by changing the coupling coefficients between the two oscillators. Our simulations therefore show that irregularities in spike trains, which normally are considered to be noise, may be interpreted as complex oscillations that might carry information.


Asunto(s)
Células Ganglionares de la Retina/efectos de la radiación , Detección de Señal Psicológica/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción , Animales , Gatos , Canales Iónicos/metabolismo , Modelos Neurológicos , Técnicas de Placa-Clamp , Periodicidad , Estimulación Luminosa , Células Ganglionares de la Retina/fisiología , Especificidad de la Especie
18.
Vision Res ; 36(4): 527-38, 1996 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8854998

RESUMEN

We measured sensitivity to binocular correlation in dynamic random-dot stereograms that defined moving sinusoidal gratings-in-depth. At a range of spatial frequencies and drift rates we established sensitivity by adding Gaussian distributed disparity noise to the modulation of disparity that defined a cyclopean grating, and finding the noise amplitude that rendered the grating just detectable. This permitted correlation thresholds to be measured at a range of suprathreshold disparity amplitudes. Spatial requirements for binocular correlation depend little on temporal frequency, and vice versa. This suggests that binocular correlation mechanisms can be characterized by independent spatial and temporal sensitivity functions. The temporal frequency function has a low pass characteristic. Sensitivity declines above about 1 c/sec, reaching its limit at 4-8 c/sec. The spatial characteristic depends greatly on the amplitude of disparity modulation, changing from band pass at low amplitude to low pass at high amplitude. The maximum resolvable spatial frequency is 4-6 c/deg, but declines sharply for relatively high amplitudes. The interaction between amplitude and spatial frequency cannot be explained by fixed high or low limits on detectable disparity gradients.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Disparidad Visual/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo , Visión Binocular/fisiología
19.
Vision Res ; 36(3): 421-4, 1996 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8746231

RESUMEN

The motion aftereffect (MAE) is an illusory drift of a physically stationary pattern induced by prolonged viewing of a moving pattern. Depending on the nature of the test pattern the MAE can be phenomenally different. This difference in appearance has led to the suggestion that different underlying mechanisms may be responsible and several reports show that this might be the case. Here, we tested whether differences in MAE duration obtained with stationary test patterns and dynamic test patterns can be explained by a single underlying mechanism. We find the results support the existence of (at least) two mechanisms. The two mechanisms show different characteristics: the static MAE (i.e. the MAE tested with a static test pattern) is almost completely stored when the static test is preceded by a dynamic test; in contradistinction, the dynamic MAE is not stored when dynamic testing is preceded by a static test pattern.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Ocular/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Ilusiones Ópticas/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
20.
Vision Res ; 35(10): 1401-12, 1995 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7645269

RESUMEN

We have studied the effects of voluntary attention on the induction of motion aftereffects (MAEs). While adapting, observers paid attention to one of two transparently displayed random dot patterns, moving concurrently in opposite directions. Selective attention was found to modulate the susceptibility to motion adaptation very substantially. To measure the strength of the induced MAEs we modulated the signal-to-noise ratio of a real motion signal in a random dot pattern that was used to balance the aftereffect. Results obtained for adapting to single motion vectors show that the MAE can be represented as a shift of the psychometric function for motion direction discrimination. Selective attention to the different components of transparent motion altered the susceptibility to adaptation. Shifting attention from one component to the other caused a large shift of the psychometric curves, about 70-75% of the shift measured for the separate components of the transparent adapting stimulus. We conclude that attention can differentiate between spatially superimposed motion vectors and that attention modulates the activity of motion mechanisms before or at the level where adaptation gives rise to MAEs. The results are discussed in light of the role of attention in visual perception and the physiological site for attentional modulation of MAEs.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica , Atención/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Discriminación en Psicología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA