Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 28
Filtrar
1.
Public Underst Sci ; 32(2): 159-174, 2023 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36003037

RESUMEN

Advances in gene-editing technology have important implications for the treatment and prevention of disease. Accordingly, it is important to understand public perceptions towards gene editing, as the public's willingness to endorse gene editing may be as important as technological breakthroughs themselves. Previous research has almost exclusively examined attitudes towards gene editing on specific issues, but has not addressed how attitudes towards gene editing across a range of issues coalesce in individuals: that is, the degree to which discrete, heterogeneous attitudinal profiles exist versus a simple support/oppose continuum. Here, we addressed this issue using latent class analysis on data from The Pew Research Center (N = 4726; US residents) across a wide range of gene-editing topics. We found that attitudes towards gene editing cohere into 10 distinct latent classes that showed some evidence of a support/oppose continuum, but also for clear qualitative differences between each class, even with support or oppose classes, on a number of issues. The most opposed classes significantly differed from the supporter classes in age, sex, political ideology and self-rated knowledge. These findings provide evidence that attitudes towards gene editing are heterogeneous and public discourse, as well as policy making need to consider a range of arguments when evaluating this technology.


Asunto(s)
Edición Génica , Opinión Pública , Análisis de Clases Latentes , Actitud , Tecnología
2.
Vaccine ; 40(32): 4488-4495, 2022 07 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35710507

RESUMEN

Vaccines are a powerful and relatively safe tool to protect against a range of serious diseases. Nonetheless, a sizeable minority of people express 'vaccination hesitancy'. Accordingly, understanding the bases of this hesitancy represents a significant public health opportunity. In the present study we sought to examine the role of Big Five personality traits and general intelligence as predictors of vaccination hesitancy across two vaccination types in a large (N = 9667) sample of UK adults drawn from the Understanding Society longitudinal household study. We found that lower levels of general intelligence were associated with COVID-19 and seasonal flu vaccination hesitancy, and lower levels of neuroticism was associated with COVID-19 vaccination hesitancy. Although the self-reported reasons for being vaccine hesitant indicated a range of factors were important to people, lower general intelligence was associated with virtually all of these reasons. In contrast, Big Five personality traits showed more nuanced patterns of association.


Asunto(s)
COVID-19 , Vacunas contra la Influenza , Adulto , COVID-19/prevención & control , Vacunas contra la COVID-19 , Humanos , Inteligencia , Aceptación de la Atención de Salud , Personalidad , Estaciones del Año , Reino Unido , Vacunación
3.
Cogn Emot ; 35(5): 890-901, 2021 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33734017

RESUMEN

It is well-documented that face perception - including facial expression and identity recognition ability - declines with age. To date, however, it is not yet well understood whether this age-related decline reflects face-specific effects, or instead can be accounted for by well-known declines in general intelligence. We examined this issue using a relatively large, healthy, age-diverse (18-88 years) sample (N = 595) who were assessed on well-established measures of face perception and general intelligence. Replicating previous work, we observed that facial expression recognition, facial identity recognition, and general intelligence all showed declines with age. Of importance, the age-related decline of expression and identity recognition was present even when the effects of general intelligence were statistically controlled. Moreover, facial expression and identity ability each showed significant unique associations with age. These results indicate that face perception ability becomes poorer as we age, and that this decline is to some extent relatively focal in nature. Results are in line with a hierarchical structure of face perception ability, and suggest that age appears to have independent effects on the general and specific face processing levels within this structure.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Expresión Facial , Humanos , Inteligencia , Longevidad , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
4.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 121(5): 1079-1094, 2021 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32969676

RESUMEN

Decades of research have shown that about half of individual differences in personality traits is heritable. Recent studies have reported that heritability is not fixed, but instead decreases across the life span. However, findings are inconsistent and it is yet unclear whether these trends are because of a waning importance of heritable tendencies, attributable to cumulative experiential influences with age, or because of nonlinear patterns suggesting Gene × Environment interplay. We combined four twin samples (N = 7,026) from Croatia, Finland, Germany, and the United Kingdom, and we examined age trends in genetic and environmental variance in the six HEXACO personality traits: Honesty-Humility, Emotionality, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness. The cross-national sample ranges in age from 14 to 90 years, allowing analyses of linear and nonlinear age differences in genetic and environmental components of trait variance, after controlling for gender and national differences. The amount of genetic variance in Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Openness followed a reversed U-shaped pattern across age, showed a declining trend for Honesty-Humility and Conscientiousness, and was stable for Emotionality. For most traits, findings provided evidence for an increasing relative importance of life experiences contributing to personality differences across the life span. The findings are discussed against the background of Gene × Environment transactions and interactions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Longevidad , Personalidad , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Extraversión Psicológica , Humanos , Individualidad , Persona de Mediana Edad , Personalidad/genética , Trastornos de la Personalidad , Adulto Joven
5.
Politics Life Sci ; 39(2): 154-166, 2020 11 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33231034

RESUMEN

Recent advances in gene editing technology promise much for medical advances and human well-being. However, in parallel domains, there have been objections to the use of such biotechnologies. Moreover, the psychological factors that govern the willingness to use gene editing technology have been underexplored to date. In this registered report, we sought to test whether pathogen disgust sensitivity is linked with opposition to gene editing. U.K.-based adult participants (N = 347) were recruited to this study. Gene editing attitudes reflected two largely distinct latent factors concerning enhancing human traits and treating medical disorders. In contrast to prediction, pathogen disgust sensitivity was related to greater support for gene editing in both of these domains. This result suggests that gene editing, at least in the current study, is not viewed as pathogenic, or that the perceived benefits of gene editing outweigh any perceived pathogen risk.


Asunto(s)
Asco , Disentimientos y Disputas , Edición Génica , Adulto , Emociones , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
6.
Cogn Emot ; 34(8): 1621-1631, 2020 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32623969

RESUMEN

Alexithymia is a personality construct characterised most notably by a difficulty in identifying and expressing feelings. Although the emotional difficulties in alexithymia are well established, to date little work has examined its relationship to broader cognitive abilities, such as general intelligence. Across three independent, healthy adult samples (Ns = 389, 318, & 273), we examined whether alexithymia was associated with general intelligence. In all three samples, we observed a significant negative association between alexithymia and general intelligence. In two of the samples, general intelligence was a significant predictor of alexithymia even when accounting for performance on tests of facial emotion recognition ability and supramodal emotion recognition ability (measured with faces, bodies, and voices). From a theoretical perspective, these results suggest that models of alexithymia need to incorporate a role for more generalised cognitive functioning. From a practical perspective, studies examining links between alexithymia and clinical disorders, many of which have known links to general intelligence, should consider including a measure of general intelligence in order to adjust for this potentially confounding factor.


Asunto(s)
Síntomas Afectivos/fisiopatología , Síntomas Afectivos/psicología , Inteligencia/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Expresión Facial , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
7.
Cognition ; 197: 104166, 2020 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31951857

RESUMEN

Accurate recognition of others' emotions is an important skill for successful social interaction. Unsurprisingly, it has been an enduring topic of interest, and notable individual differences have been observed. Despite this focus, the underlying functional architecture of this ability has not been thoroughly investigated, particularly concerning emotion recognition across different sensory domains and stimulus modalities. Using a structural equation modelling approach, Study 1 (N = 284) established the structure of emotion recognition ability across three expressive domains - face, body and voice - and observed strong evidence for a superordinate 'supramodal' emotion recognition factor, over and above domain-specific factors. Additionally, we observed a significant moderate negative association between this superordinate factor and alexithymia. In Study 2 (N = 218), findings indicated that supramodal emotion recognition ability and face identity recognition are two related but independent constructs. In Study 3 (N = 249), we examined links from both supramodal emotion recognition and face identity recognition to broader cognitive ability, and observed that general intelligence was a significant predictor of supramodal emotion recognition ability. In contrast, there was no association between intelligence and face identity recognition ability. Across three independent samples, then, our findings offer strong support for an emotion recognition ability factor existing across visual and auditory domains encompassing social signals conveyed by face, body and voice, and outline its associations to broader cognitive and affective traits.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial , Cognición Social , Emociones , Expresión Facial , Humanos , Reconocimiento en Psicología
8.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 72(11): 2565-2573, 2019 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31035862

RESUMEN

Previous research has indicated that faces with higher levels of skin carotenoid colouration are perceived as healthier and more attractive. However, it is not known whether this preference is specific to faces or reflects a more generalised preference in biological stimuli: for example, non-face body parts. Moreover, it is not yet well established whether the preference for carotenoid colouration extends beyond Caucasian populations. Three studies were conducted to address these issues. In Studies 1 and 2, Caucasian and Hong Kong Chinese participants, respectively, selected the more attractive image in pairs of high and low carotenoid colouration versions of images of Caucasian faces and body parts, and also for non-face/body-part control stimuli (scrambled faces). In Study 3, a similar study protocol was used with an independent sample of Hong Kong Chinese participants using stimuli generated from Chinese individuals. The results showed that high carotenoid colouration was preferred across all the three studies in faces and body parts, but not in the control stimuli. In addition, there was a stronger preference for high carotenoid colouration in faces compared with body parts in Studies 1 and 2-although this preference was not observed in Study 3. Overall, these findings demonstrate that higher levels of skin carotenoid colouration are preferred both in face and body parts, but not in non-face stimuli, and that these preferences are evident in Caucasian and Hong Kong Chinese individuals.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Cuerpo Humano , Pigmentación de la Piel , Adulto , Pueblo Asiatico/psicología , Belleza , Imagen Corporal/psicología , Comparación Transcultural , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Población Blanca/psicología
9.
Emotion ; 19(3): 455-464, 2019 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29781645

RESUMEN

Although it is widely believed that females outperform males in the ability to recognize other people's emotions, this conclusion is not well supported by the extant literature. The current study sought to provide a strong test of the female superiority hypothesis by investigating sex differences in emotion recognition for five basic emotions using stimuli well-calibrated for individual differences assessment, across two expressive domains (face and body), and in a large sample (N = 1,022: Study 1). We also assessed the stability and generalizability of our findings with two independent replication samples (N = 303: Study 2, N = 634: Study 3). In Study 1, we observed that females were superior to males in recognizing facial disgust and sadness. In contrast, males were superior to females in recognizing bodily happiness. The female superiority for recognition of facial disgust was replicated in Studies 2 and 3, and this observation also extended to an independent stimulus set in Study 2. No other sex differences were stable across studies. These findings provide evidence for the presence of sex differences in emotion recognition ability, but show that these differences are modest in magnitude and appear to be limited to facial disgust. We discuss whether this sex difference may reflect human evolutionary imperatives concerning reproductive fitness and child care. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Caracteres Sexuales , Asco , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
10.
Cogn Emot ; 33(6): 1119-1128, 2019 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30336725

RESUMEN

Recognising identity and emotion conveyed by the face is important for successful social interactions and has thus been the focus of considerable research. Debate has surrounded the extent to which the mechanisms underpinning face emotion and face identity recognition are distinct or share common processes. Here we use an individual differences approach to address this issue. In a well-powered (N = 605) and age-diverse sample we used structural equation modelling to assess the association between face emotion recognition and face identity recognition ability. We also sought to assess whether this association (if present) reflected visual short-term memory and/or general intelligence (g). We observed a strong positive correlation (r = .52) between face emotion recognition ability and face identity recognition ability. This association was reduced in magnitude but still moderate in size (r = .28) and highly significant when controlling for measures of g and visual short-term memory. These results indicate that face emotion and face identity recognition abilities in part share a common processing mechanism. We suggest that face processing ability involves multiple functional components and that modelling the sources of individual differences can offer an important perspective on the relationship between these components.


Asunto(s)
Expresión Facial , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Inteligencia/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Estudios de Cohortes , Estudios Transversales , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reino Unido , Adulto Joven
11.
Neuroimage ; 176: 22-28, 2018 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29665419

RESUMEN

The neural correlates of human personality have been of longstanding interest; however, most studies in the field have relied on modest sample sizes and few replicable results have been reported to date. We investigated relationships between personality and brain gray matter in a sample of generally healthy, older (mean age 73 years) adults from Scotland drawn from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Participants (N = 578) completed a brain MRI scan and self-reported Big Five personality trait measures. Conscientiousness trait scores were positively related to brain cortical thickness in a range of regions, including bilateral parahippocampal gyrus, bilateral fusiform gyrus, left cingulate gyrus, right medial orbitofrontal cortex, and left dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. These associations - most notably in frontal regions - were modestly-to-moderately attenuated by the inclusion of biomarker variables assessing allostatic load and smoking status. None of the other personality traits showed robust associations with brain cortical thickness, nor did we observe any personality trait associations with cortical surface area and gray matter volume. These findings indicate that brain cortical thickness is associated with conscientiousness, perhaps partly accounted for by allostatic load and smoking status.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/anatomía & histología , Sustancia Gris/anatomía & histología , Personalidad/fisiología , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Vías Nerviosas/anatomía & histología , Inventario de Personalidad
12.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 48(8): 2758-2765, 2018 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29549550

RESUMEN

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and autism-like traits are associated with deficits in face memory ability, although it is not yet clear whether this deficit reflects a specific aspect of the ASD/autism-like phenotype. We addressed this issue using a neurotypical sample of adolescent twins (Ncomplete pairs = 782) drawn from the Twins Early Development Study who were assessed on face and object memory performance alongside two core aspects of autism-like traits: (i) difficulties with social behavior/interactions, and (ii) attention to detail. We observed a negative association between face memory ability and difficulties with social behavior/interactions. This association reflected an overlapping genetic etiology: heritable influences acting on face memory ability are associated with the social difficulties aspects of autism-like traits.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/genética , Reconocimiento Facial , Relaciones Interpersonales , Fenotipo , Adolescente , Atención , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/fisiopatología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria , Gemelos
13.
Psychol Sci ; 29(5): 711-722, 2018 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29451460

RESUMEN

Longstanding interest has been directed toward the etiology of sociopolitical attitudes. Personality traits have been posited as antecedents; however, most work addressing such links has been limited to cross-sectional study designs. The current study used data from two large (both Ns > 8,700), longitudinal cohorts of individuals from the United Kingdom who were parent-assessed on a measure of temperament (assessing anxiety, conduct problems, and hyperactivity) at age 5 or 7 years and on a range of sociopolitical attitudes at age 30 or 33 years. In both cohorts, higher levels of childhood conduct problems predicted higher levels of economic and political discontent in adulthood. These associations were still evident when controlling for sex, childhood intelligence, and parental social class. In both cohorts, this pathway was partially mediated by educational attainment and achieved social class. These findings are consistent with the perspective that early-life temperament gives rise to adult political sentiment.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Actitud , Conducta Infantil/fisiología , Personalidad/fisiología , Política , Problema de Conducta , Temperamento/fisiología , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Reino Unido
14.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 58(3): 292-304, 2017 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27861883

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Childhood behavior problems predict subsequent educational achievement; however, little research has examined the etiology of these links using a longitudinal twin design. Moreover, it is unknown whether genetic and environmental innovations provide incremental prediction for educational achievement from childhood to adolescence. METHODS: We examined genetic and environmental influences on parental ratings of behavior problems across childhood (age 4) and adolescence (ages 12 and 16) as predictors of educational achievement at age 16 using a longitudinal classical twin design. RESULTS: Shared-environmental influences on anxiety, conduct problems, and peer problems at age 4 predicted educational achievement at age 16. Genetic influences on the externalizing behaviors of conduct problems and hyperactivity at age 4 predicted educational achievement at age 16. Moreover, novel genetic and (to a lesser extent) nonshared-environmental influences acting on conduct problems and hyperactivity emerged at ages 12 and 16, adding to the genetic prediction from age 4. CONCLUSIONS: These findings demonstrate that genetic and shared-environmental factors underpinning behavior problems in early childhood predict educational achievement in midadolescence. These findings are consistent with the notion that early-childhood behavior problems reflect the initiation of a life-course persistent trajectory with concomitant implications for social attainment. However, we also find evidence that genetic and nonshared-environment innovations acting on behavior problems have implications for subsequent educational achievement, consistent with recent work arguing that adolescence represents a sensitive period for socioaffective development.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Trastorno de la Conducta , Escolaridad , Conducta Social , Adolescente , Ansiedad/etiología , Ansiedad/genética , Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/etiología , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Niño , Preescolar , Trastorno de la Conducta/etiología , Trastorno de la Conducta/genética , Trastorno de la Conducta/fisiopatología , Inglaterra , Ambiente , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Grupo Paritario , Pronóstico , Gales
15.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 11(8): 1255-61, 2016 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27013101

RESUMEN

Establishing the neural bases of individual differences in personality has been an enduring topic of interest. However, while a growing literature has sought to characterize grey matter correlates of personality traits, little attention to date has been focused on regional white matter correlates of personality, especially for the personality traits agreeableness, conscientiousness and openness. To rectify this gap in knowledge we used a large sample (n > 550) of older adults who provided data on both personality (International Personality Item Pool) and white matter tract-specific fractional anisotropy (FA) from diffusion tensor MRI. Results indicated that conscientiousness was associated with greater FA in the left uncinate fasciculus (ß = 0.17, P < 0.001). We also examined links between FA and the personality meta-trait 'stability', which is defined as the common variance underlying agreeableness, conscientiousness, and neuroticism/emotional stability. We observed an association between left uncinate fasciculus FA and stability (ß = 0.27, P < 0.001), which fully accounted for the link between left uncinate fasciculus FA and conscientiousness. In sum, these results provide novel evidence for links between regional white matter microstructure and key traits of human personality, specifically conscientiousness and the meta-trait, stability. Future research is recommended to replicate and address the causal directions of these associations.


Asunto(s)
Conciencia , Personalidad/fisiología , Sustancia Blanca/anatomía & histología , Anciano , Imagen de Difusión Tensora , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Sustancia Blanca/diagnóstico por imagen
16.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 145(5): 589-602, 2016 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26986040

RESUMEN

Emotion recognition has been a focus of considerable attention for several decades. However, despite this interest, the underlying structure of individual differences in emotion recognition ability has been largely overlooked and thus is poorly understood. For example, limited knowledge exists concerning whether recognition ability for one emotion (e.g., disgust) generalizes to other emotions (e.g., anger, fear). Furthermore, it is unclear whether emotion recognition ability generalizes across modalities, such that those who are good at recognizing emotions from the face, for example, are also good at identifying emotions from nonfacial cues (such as cues conveyed via the body). The primary goal of the current set of studies was to address these questions through establishing the structure of individual differences in visual emotion recognition ability. In three independent samples (Study 1: n = 640; Study 2: n = 389; Study 3: n = 303), we observed that the ability to recognize visually presented emotions is based on different sources of variation: a supramodal emotion-general factor, supramodal emotion-specific factors, and face- and within-modality emotion-specific factors. In addition, we found evidence that general intelligence and alexithymia were associated with supramodal emotion recognition ability. Autism-like traits, empathic concern, and alexithymia were independently associated with face-specific emotion recognition ability. These results (a) provide a platform for further individual differences research on emotion recognition ability, (b) indicate that differentiating levels within the architecture of emotion recognition ability is of high importance, and (c) show that the capacity to understand expressions of emotion in others is linked to broader affective and cognitive processes.


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Afecto/fisiología , Síntomas Afectivos/fisiopatología , Señales (Psicología) , Cara , Expresión Facial , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Inteligencia , Masculino
17.
Proc Biol Sci ; 282(1811)2015 07 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26136449

RESUMEN

Understanding the underpinnings of behavioural disturbances following brain injury is of considerable importance, but little at present is known about the relationships between different types of behavioural disturbances. Here, we take a novel approach to this issue by using confirmatory factor analysis to elucidate the architecture of verbal aggression, physical aggression and inappropriate sexual behaviour using systematic records made across an eight-week observation period for a large sample (n = 301) of individuals with a range of brain injuries. This approach offers a powerful test of the architecture of these behavioural disturbances by testing the fit between observed behaviours and different theoretical models. We chose models that reflected alternative theoretical perspectives based on generalized disinhibition (Model 1), a difference between aggression and inappropriate sexual behaviour (Model 2), or on the idea that verbal aggression, physical aggression and inappropriate sexual behaviour reflect broadly distinct but correlated clinical phenomena (Model 3). Model 3 provided the best fit to the data indicating that these behaviours can be viewed as distinct, but with substantial overlap. These data are important both for developing models concerning the architecture of behaviour as well as for clinical management in individuals with brain injury.


Asunto(s)
Agresión , Lesiones Encefálicas/etiología , Conducta Sexual , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Neurológicos , Reino Unido , Adulto Joven
18.
Behav Genet ; 45(2): 181-99, 2015 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25534512

RESUMEN

This study quantified genetic and environmental roots of variance in prejudice and discriminatory intent toward foreign nationals and examined potential mediators of these genetic influences: right-wing authoritarianism (RWA), social dominance orientation (SDO), and narrow-sense xenophobia (NSX). In line with the dual process motivational (DPM) model, we predicted that the two basic attitudinal and motivational orientations-RWA and SDO-would account for variance in out-group prejudice and discrimination. In line with other theories, we expected that NSX as an affective component would explain additional variance in out-group prejudice and discriminatory intent. Data from 1,397 individuals (incl. twins as well as their spouses) were analyzed. Univariate analyses of twins' and spouses' data yielded genetic (incl. contributions of assortative mating) and multiple environmental sources (i.e., social homogamy, spouse-specific, and individual-specific effects) of variance in negativity toward strangers. Multivariate analyses suggested an extension to the DPM model by including NSX in addition to RWA and SDO as predictor of prejudice and discrimination. RWA and NSX primarily mediated the genetic influences on the variance in prejudice and discriminatory intent toward foreign nationals. In sum, the findings provide the basis of a behavioral genetic framework integrating different scientific disciplines for the study of negativity toward out-groups.


Asunto(s)
Motivación , Prejuicio , Xenofobia , Adulto , Actitud , Autoritarismo , Femenino , Alemania , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis Multivariante , Personalidad , Política , Predominio Social , Esposos , Gemelos Dicigóticos , Gemelos Monocigóticos
19.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 55(8): 865-75, 2014 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24256444

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Understanding the etiology of adolescent problem behavior has been of enduring interest. Only relatively recently, however, has this issue been examined within a normal personality trait framework. Research suggests that problem behaviors in adolescence and beyond may be adequately explained by the taxonomy provided by the basic dimensions of normal personality: Such problem behaviors are suggested to be extreme points on a distribution of the full range of the underlying traits. We extend work in this field examining the extent to which genetic factors underlying the five-factor model of personality are common with genetic influences on adolescent behavior problems (namely, anxiety, peer problems, conduct, hyperactivity, and low prosociality). METHOD: A nationally representative twin sample (Twins Early Development Study) from the general population of England and Wales, including 2031 pairs of twins aged 16 years old, was used to decompose variation into genetic and environmental components. Behavioral problems in adolescence were assessed by self-report with the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. RESULTS: Adolescent behavior problems were moderately associated with normal personality: Specifically, a fifth to a third of phenotypic variance in problem behaviors was accounted for by five-factor model personality traits. Of central importance here, genetic influences underpinning personality were entirely overlapping with those genetic factors underlying adolescent behavior problems. CONCLUSIONS: These findings suggest that adolescent behavior problems can be understood, at least in part, within a model of normal personality trait variation, with the genetic bases of these behavior problems the same as those genetic influences underpinning normal personality.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Personalidad/genética , Adolescente , Ansiedad/genética , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/genética , Femenino , Humanos , Hipercinesia/genética , Masculino , Grupo Paritario , Pruebas de Personalidad , Conducta Social , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Gemelos Dicigóticos/genética , Gemelos Dicigóticos/psicología , Gemelos Monocigóticos/genética , Gemelos Monocigóticos/psicología
20.
J Pers ; 82(4): 297-309, 2014 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23799969

RESUMEN

Research has shown that in-group favoritism is associated with concerns over the maintenance of social norms. Here we present two studies examining whether genetic factors underpin this association. A classical twin design was used to decompose phenotypic variance into genetic and environmental components in two studies. Study 1 used 812 pairs of adult U.S. twins from the nationally representative MIDUS II sample. Study 2 used 707 pairs of middle-age twins from the Minnesota Twin Registry. In-group favoritism was measured with scales tapping preferences for in-group (vs. out-group) individuals; norm concerns were measured with the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire-Traditionalism (Study 1) and Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA; Study 2) scales. In Study 1, heritable effects underlying traditionalism were moderately (c. 35%) overlapping with the genetic variance underpinning in-group favoritism. In Study 2, heritable influences on RWA were entirely shared with the heritable effects on in-group favoritism. Moreover, we observed that Big Five Openness shared common genetic links to both RWA and in-group favoritism. These results suggest that, at the genetic level, in-group favoritism is linked with a system related to concern over normative social practices, which is, in turn, partially associated with trait Openness.


Asunto(s)
Autoritarismo , Jerarquia Social , Principios Morales , Personalidad/genética , Normas Sociales , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Psicológicos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Gemelos/genética
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...