Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 26
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Res Adolesc ; 26(3): 610-616, 2016 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28581658

RESUMEN

Primary caregivers play an important role in emotion socialization. Real-time mother-daughter emotion socialization was examined in 45 mother-daughter dyads with early-adolescent daughters (age M = 11.80, SD = .27) at the first observation point. Maternal supportive emotion regulation and daughters' emotions were coded during two conflict discussions, 2 years apart. With multilevel survival analysis, the likelihood of maternal supportiveness was predicted both over time, between early and mid-adolescence, and by daughters' pubertal status. Mothers were more likely to respond to daughters' negative and positive emotions with supportiveness for daughters whose pubertal maturation occurred relatively early. Results suggest that mothers adjust their socialization of daughters' emotions according to their daughters' pubertal development.


Asunto(s)
Emociones , Relaciones Madre-Hijo , Socialización , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Madres , Núcleo Familiar
2.
Neuroimage ; 85 Pt 2: 873-87, 2014 Jan 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24007804

RESUMEN

Cognitive control functions continue to improve from infancy until early adulthood, allowing flexible adaptation to a complex environment. However, it remains controversial how this development in cognitive capabilities is mediated by changes in cortical activity: both age-related increases and decreases of mediofrontal neural activity have been observed and interpreted as neural underpinnings of this functional development. To better understand this developmental process, we examined EEG theta activity in the mediofrontal region using a Go/No-go response control task. We found that both pre-stimulus baseline theta-power and theta-power during the response control task, without baseline-correction, decreased with age. Conversely, when task-related theta-power was baseline corrected (using a ratio method), it exhibited a positive developmental trajectory. The age-related theta-power increase was source-localized to the anterior cingulate cortex. This increase in theta activity also partially mediated age-related improvements in response control and was greatest in a condition that demanded greater effort. Theta activity in older children also showed greater temporal reliability across trials as measured by inter-trial phase-coherence. Interestingly, directly subtracting baseline activity from task-related activity did not yield significant developmental effects, which highlights the necessity of separating and contrasting the pre-stimulus baseline with task-related processing in the understanding of neurodevelopmental changes.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Giro del Cíngulo/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Ritmo Teta/fisiología , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Niño , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa
3.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 41(8): 1243-55, 2013 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23690280

RESUMEN

The present study examined the cortical processes that mediate cognitive regulation in response to emotion-eliciting stimuli, before and after anxious children participated in a cognitive behavioral therapy program. Electroencephalographic activity was recorded from anxious children (n = 24, 8 males) and comparison children (n = 16, 7 males) at pre-and post-treatment sessions. The change in anxiety T-scores from pre- to post-treatment was used to signify clinical improvement among anxious children (Improvers: n = 11 vs. Non-improvers: n = 13). Event-related potential components were recorded while children performed a Go/No-go task using emotional facial expressions. For the P1 component, believed to reflect attention and/or arousal processes, Non-improvers had greater activation levels relative to Improver and comparison groups at both sessions. Greater P1 amplitudes at pre-treatment predicted non-improvement following treatment. For the frontal N2 component, thought to reflect cognitive control processing, Improvers recruited greater activation from pre- to post-treatment, a change in activation that was predictive of treatment outcome. Non-improvers showed increased cortical activation within the time window of the P1, whether at pre- or post-treatment. These data suggest that heightened perceptual vigilance may have led to poorer outcomes. Improvers showed increased prefrontal activation within the time window of the N2 from pre- to post-treatment. These data suggest that increased cognitive control may have led to improved treatment outcomes. In sum, P1 activation may serve as a predictor of treatment outcome, while N2 activation may serve as an indicator of treatment response.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Inteligencia Emocional/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Adolescente , Análisis de Varianza , Trastornos de Ansiedad/psicología , Trastornos de Ansiedad/terapia , Atención/fisiología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Electroencefalografía , Emociones/fisiología , Femenino , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiopatología , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Entrevista Psicológica , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/estadística & datos numéricos , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiopatología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Resultado del Tratamiento
4.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 54(5): 552-64, 2013 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23046115

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The present study was designed to examine the cortical processes that mediate cognitive regulation in response to emotion-eliciting stimuli in anxious children. METHODS: Electroencephalographic (EEG) activity was recorded from clinically anxious children (n = 29) and typically developing children (n = 34). Event-related potential components were recorded while children performed a go/no-go task using facial stimuli depicting angry, calm, and happy expressions. RESULTS: Anxious children had significantly greater posterior P1 and frontal N2 amplitudes, components associated with attention/arousal and cognitive control, respectively, than typically developing children. Anxious children also had significantly greater error-related negativities and correct-response negativities relative to typically developing children. For the anxious group only, there were no differences in neural activation between face (emotion) types or trial (Go vs. No-go) types. A regression analysis revealed that No-go N2 amplitudes for calm faces predicted self-reported anxiety levels. CONCLUSIONS: Anxious children appeared to show increased cortical activation regardless of the emotional content of the stimuli. Anxious children also showed greater medial-frontal activity regardless of task demands and response accuracy. Taken together, these findings suggest indiscriminate cortical processes that may underlie the hypervigilant regulatory style seen in clinically anxious individuals.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiopatología , Electroencefalografía , Inteligencia Emocional/fisiología , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador , Afecto/fisiología , Trastornos de Ansiedad/diagnóstico , Trastornos de Ansiedad/psicología , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Niño , Comorbilidad , Potenciales Evocados , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiopatología , Humanos , Masculino , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiopatología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología
5.
Dev Psychopathol ; 24(3): 1019-29, 2012 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22781869

RESUMEN

Past studies have shown that aggressive children exhibit rigid (rather than flexible) parent-child interactions; these rigid repertoires may provide the context through which children fail to acquire emotion-regulation skills. Difficulties in regulating emotion are associated with minimal activity in dorsal systems in the cerebral cortex, for example, the anterior cingulate cortex. The current study aimed to integrate parent-child and neurocognitive indices of emotion regulation and examine their associations for the first time. Sixty children (8-12 years old) referred for treatment for aggression underwent two assessments. Brain processes related to emotion regulation were assessed using dense-array EEG with a computerized go/no-go task. The N2 amplitudes thought to tap inhibitory control were recorded, and a source analysis was conducted. In the second assessment, parents and children were videotaped while trying to solve a conflict topic. State space grids were used to derive two dynamic flexibility parameters from the coded videotapes: (a) the number of transitions between emotional states and (b) the dispersion of emotional states, based on proportional durations in each state. The regression results showed that flexibility measures were not related to N2 amplitudes. However, flexibility measures were significantly associated with the ratio of dorsal to ventral source activation: for transitions, ΔR 2 = .27, F (1, 34) = 13.13, p = .001; for dispersion, ΔR 2 = .29, F (1, 35) = 14.76, p < .001. Thus, in support of our main hypothesis, greater dyadic flexibility was associated with a higher ratio of dorsomedial to ventral activation, suggesting that children with more flexible parent-child interactions are able to recruit relatively more dorsomedial activity in challenging situations.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/psicología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/psicología , Conducta Infantil/psicología , Emociones/fisiología , Agresión/fisiología , Niño , Conducta Infantil/fisiología , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/fisiopatología , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Relaciones Madre-Hijo , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
6.
Dev Cogn Neurosci ; 2(3): 340-50, 2012 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22669035

RESUMEN

The modulation of control processes by stimulus salience, as well as associated neural activation, changes over development. We investigated age-related differences in the influence of facial emotion on brain activation when an action had to be withheld, focusing on a developmental period characterized by rapid social-emotional and cognitive change. Groups of kindergarten and young school-aged children and a group of young adults performed a modified Go/Nogo task. Response cues were preceded by happy or angry faces. After controlling for task performance, left orbitofrontal regions discriminated trials with happy vs. angry faces in children but not in adults when a response was withheld, and this effect decreased parametrically with age group. Age-related changes in prefrontal responsiveness to facial expression were not observed when an action was required, nor did this region show age-related activation changes with the demand to withhold a response in general. Such results reveal age-related differences in prefrontal activation that are specific to stimulus valence and depend on the action required.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Desarrollo Humano/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Análisis de Varianza , Ira , Niño , Preescolar , Señales (Psicología) , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Cara , Femenino , Felicidad , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Corteza Prefrontal/crecimiento & desarrollo , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción , Sonrisa/psicología , Adulto Joven
7.
Brain Cogn ; 77(2): 159-69, 2011 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21940093

RESUMEN

Emotion regulation is a key social skill and children who fail to master it are at risk for clinical disorders. Specific styles of emotion regulation have been associated with particular patterns of prefrontal activation. We investigated whether anxious aggressive children would reveal a different pattern of cortical activation than non-anxious aggressive children and normally-developing children. We examined the magnitude and timing of source activation underlying the N2-an ERP associated with inhibitory control-during a go/nogo task with a negative emotion induction component (loss of earned points). We estimated cortical activation for two regions of interest-a ventral prefrontal and a dorsomedial prefrontal region-for three 100-ms windows over the range of the N2 (200-500 ms). Anxious aggressive children showed high ventral prefrontal activation in the early window; non-anxious aggressive children showed high ventral prefrontal activation in the late window, but only for the duration of the emotion induction; and normally-developing children showed low ventral prefrontal activation throughout. There were no group differences in dorsomedial prefrontal activation. These results suggest that anxious aggressive children recruit ventral prefrontal activation quickly and indiscriminately, possibly giving rise to their rigid, threat-oriented approach to conflict. The late ventral prefrontal activation seen for non-anxious aggressive children may underlie a more delayed, situation-specific, but ineffective response to frustration.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/fisiología , Ondas Encefálicas/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Agresión/psicología , Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Ansiedad/psicología , Mapeo Encefálico , Niño , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 6(1): 12-23, 2011 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20194512

RESUMEN

The present study investigated age-related differences in the amygdala and other nodes of face-processing networks in response to facial expression and familiarity. fMRI data were analyzed from 31 children (3.5-8.5 years) and 14 young adults (18-33 years) who viewed pictures of familiar (mothers) and unfamiliar emotional faces. Results showed that amygdala activation for faces over a scrambled image baseline increased with age. Children, but not adults, showed greater amygdala activation to happy than angry faces; in addition, amygdala activation for angry faces increased with age. In keeping with growing evidence of a positivity bias in young children, our data suggest that children find happy faces to be more salient or meaningful than angry faces. Both children and adults showed preferential activation to mothers' over strangers' faces in a region of rostral anterior cingulate cortex associated with self-evaluation, suggesting that some nodes in frontal evaluative networks are active early in development. This study presents novel data on neural correlates of face processing in childhood and indicates that preferential amygdala activation for emotional expressions changes with age.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Cara , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Amígdala del Cerebelo/irrigación sanguínea , Análisis de Varianza , Mapeo Encefálico , Niño , Preescolar , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Movimiento (Física) , Oxígeno/sangre , Estimulación Luminosa , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Adulto Joven
9.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 108(3): 621-37, 2011 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21044790

RESUMEN

This study examined the development of executive function (EF) in a typically developing sample from middle childhood to adolescence using a range of tasks varying in affective significance. A total of 102 participants between 8 and 15years of age completed the Iowa Gambling Task, the Color Word Stroop, a Delay Discounting task, and a Digit Span task. Age-related improvements were found on all tasks, but improvements on relatively cool tasks (Color Word Stroop and Backward Digit Span) occurred earlier in this age range, whereas improvements on relatively hot tasks (Iowa Gambling Task and Delay Discounting) were more gradual and occurred later. Exploratory factor analysis indicated that performance on all tasks could be accounted for by a single-factor model. Together, these findings indicate that although similar abilities may underlie both hot and cool EF, hot EF develops relatively slowly, which may have implications for the risky behavior often observed during adolescence. Future work should include additional measures to characterize more intensively the development of both hot and cool EF during the transition to adolescence.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/estadística & datos numéricos , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Femenino , Juego de Azar , Humanos , Masculino , Test de Stroop , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
10.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 6(2): 150-5, 2011 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26162132

RESUMEN

Rather than view addiction as a disease, Heyman sees it as a choice-one that works like other choices, whereby immediate rewards outshine long-term gains. He rejects neuroscientific explanations of addictive behavior, because he believes they cast it as involuntary or disease-like. I argue that the disease-versus-choice debate creates a false dichotomy: Neuroscience does not have to frame addiction as a disease. Rather, it can help explain how addicts make impulsive choices in the moment and distort appraisal and decision-making habits in the long run. Specifically, the salience of drug-related cues is enhanced by dopamine activity in the ventral striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, and amygdala, due to the intense hedonic impact of repeated drug experiences. Moreover, dopamine-based craving peaks when drug (or alcohol or gambling) rewards become available, in the moment, and this rapid increase in attractiveness preempts rational judgment. Finally, repeated dopamine enhancement modifies brain structures to maximize the appeal of addictive activities, minimize the appeal of competing rewards, and undermine the cognitive capacities necessary to choose between them. I conclude that addiction is not a monolithic state but a recurrent series of choices that permit negotiation, and sometimes cooperation, between immediate and long-range goals.

11.
Cogn Neurodyn ; 5(1): 45-53, 2011 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22379495

RESUMEN

The concept of a brain default network postulates that specific brain regions are more active when a person is engaged in introspective mental activity. Transient functional coordination between groups of neurons is thought to be necessary for information processing. Since children develop introspection as they mature, regions of the default network may establish increasing functional coordination with age, resulting in fewer fluctuations in synchronization patterns. We investigated the transient coordinated activity in regions of the default network in seventeen children aged 11 months to 17 years of age using EEG recordings while subjects were resting quietly with eyes closed. The temporal and spatial fluctuations in the phase synchrony patterns were estimated across sites associated with the default network pattern and compared to other regions. Lower variability of the spatio-temporal patterns of phase synchronization associated with the default network was observed in the older group as compared to the younger group. This indicates that functional coordination increases among regions of the default network as children develop.

12.
Dev Neuropsychol ; 35(2): 156-76, 2010.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20390600

RESUMEN

One of the most important tasks of childhood is learning to self-regulate in the presence of negative emotions. Until recently, almost no research has examined the neurophysiological correlates of emotional self-regulation as it develops over childhood and adolescence. We were interested in plotting a fine-grained developmental profile of the neural underpinnings of self-regulation, in the context of negative emotion, for 7- to 14-year-old children. We predicted that children would recruit less cortical activation with age in the service of self-regulation, reflecting increased neural efficiency with development. We also predicted that children would recruit more cortical activation with increased negative emotion, possibly reflecting greater demand on cortical resources. We administered a Go No-Go task with an emotion induction block and we measured the amplitude of the N2, an event related potential associated with inhibitory control, as it varied with block and with age. Furthermore, we estimated activation for a ventral prefrontal region of interest (ROI; suggestive of orbital frontal, ventromedial prefrontal, or rostral anterior cingulate activation) and a dorsomedial prefrontal ROI (suggestive of dorsal anterior cingulate activation) frequently modeled as cortical generators underlying the N2. Results revealed a marginal decrease in mediofrontal scalp activation, but a more pronounced decrease in activation of the ventromedial prefrontal ROI, with age. There were no age-related changes in dorsomedial prefrontal ROI activation. Lastly, as predicted, we found increased ventral prefrontal ROI activation during the negative emotion induction, possibly reflecting greater recruitment of frontocortical resources underlying emotion regulation, but developmental change in this activation was no different than for the other conditions. Thus, both self-regulation in general and emotion regulation in particular recruited less cortical activation with age, suggesting more efficient cortical mechanisms of response inhibition.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil , Emociones/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Controles Informales de la Sociedad , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Análisis de Varianza , Mapeo Encefálico , Niño , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
13.
Dev Psychopathol ; 20(3): 913-39, 2008.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18606038

RESUMEN

Children's behavior problems may stem from ineffective cortical mechanisms for regulating negative emotions, and the success of interventions may depend on their impact on such mechanisms. We examined neurophysiological markers associated with emotion regulation in children comorbid for externalizing and internalizing problems before and after treatment. We hypothesized that treatment success would correspond with reduced ventral prefrontal activation, and increased dorsomedial prefrontal activation, at the time point of an event-related potential (ERP) associated with inhibitory control. Twenty-seven 8- to 12-year-old children (with usable data) were tested before and after a 14-week community-based treatment program and assessed as to improvement status. Fifteen 8- to 12-year-olds from the normal population (with usable data) were tested over the same interval. All children completed an emotion-induction go/no-go task while fitted with a 128-channel electrode net at each test session. ERP amplitudes, and estimates of cortical activation in prefrontal regions of interest, were measured at the peak of the "inhibitory" N2 and compared between improvers, nonimprovers, and nonclinical children. ERP amplitudes showed no group differences. However, improvers showed an overall reduction in ventral prefrontal activation from pretreatment to posttreatment, bringing them in line with nonclinical children, whereas ventral activation remained high for nonimprovers. Both improvers and nonimprovers showed high dorsal activation relative to nonclinical children. Supplementary analyses indicated that only ventral prefrontal regions, and only within the N2 time window, showed decreased activity from pre- to posttreatment, suggesting changes in regulatory processes rather than in overall emotional arousal. These cortically mediated changes may permit a reduction in the overengaged, rigid style of emotion regulation characteristic of children with behavior problems.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/terapia , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual , Emociones/fisiología , Terapia Familiar , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiopatología , Control Interno-Externo , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiopatología , Agresión/psicología , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Niño , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/fisiopatología , Terapia Combinada , Variación Contingente Negativa , Educación , Electroencefalografía , Medicina Basada en la Evidencia , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Determinación de la Personalidad , Régimen de Recompensa
14.
Neuropsychologia ; 46(2): 595-613, 2008 Jan 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18061633

RESUMEN

To date, little is known about the neural underpinnings of social-emotional processes in young children. The present study investigated the time course of children's ERP responses to facial expression and personal familiarity, and the effect of these variables on ERP measures of effortful attention in a Go-Nogo task. Dense-array EEG was collected from 48 4-6-year-old children who were presented with pictures of their mothers' and strangers' happy and angry faces. ERPs were scored following face presentation and following a subsequent cue signaling a Go or Nogo response. Responses to face presentation showed early perceptual components that were larger following strangers' faces, suggesting facilitated rapid processing of personally important faces. A mid-latency frontocentral negativity was greatest following angry mothers' faces, indicating increased attentional monitoring and/or recognition memory evoked by an angry parent. Finally a right-lateralized late positive component was largest following angry faces, suggesting extended processing of negatively valenced social stimuli in general. Following the Go-Nogo response cue, a right-lateralized mid-latency negativity thought to measure effortful attention was larger in Nogo than Go trials, and following angry than happy faces, possibly reflecting increased effortful control required in those conditions. The present study suggests that overlapping but differentiated networks for both rapid and elaborative processing of important socio-affective information are established by 4-6 years. Moreover, the extended spatial and temporal distribution of components suggests a pattern of response to social stimuli in which more rapid processes may index personal familiarity, whereas temporally extended processes are sensitive to affective valence on both familiar and unfamiliar faces.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción Social , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Análisis de Varianza , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Niño , Desarrollo Infantil , Preescolar , Cognición/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Valores de Referencia , Factores de Tiempo
15.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 35(5): 845-57, 2007 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17549621

RESUMEN

Studies have shown that improved parenting mediates treatment outcomes for aggressive children, but we lack fine-grained descriptions of how parent-child interactions change with treatment. The current study addresses this gap by applying new dynamic systems methods to study parent-child emotional behavior patterns. These methods tap moment-to-moment changes in interaction processes within and across sessions and quantify previously unmeasured processes of change related to treatment success. Aggressive children and their parents were recruited from combined Parent Management Training and Cognitive-behavioral programs in "real world" clinical settings. Behavioral outcomes were assessed by reports from parents and clinicians. At pre- and post-treatment, home visits were videotaped while parents and children discussed consecutively: a positive topic, a mutually unresolved problem, and another positive topic. Results showed that significant improvements in children's externalizing behavior were associated with increases in parent-child emotional flexibility during the problem-solving discussion. Also, dyads who improved still expressed negative emotions, but they acquired the skills to repair conflicts, shifting out of their negative interactions to mutually positive patterns.


Asunto(s)
Agresión , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/terapia , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual , Terapia Familiar , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Agresión/psicología , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/psicología , Conflicto Familiar/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Solución de Problemas , Técnicas Sociométricas , Resultado del Tratamiento
16.
Dev Psychopathol ; 19(2): 455-80, 2007.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17459179

RESUMEN

Children referred for externalizing behavior problems may not represent a homogeneous population. Our objective was to assess neural mechanisms of emotion regulation that might distinguish subtypes of externalizing children from each other and from their normal age mates. Children with pure externalizing (EXT) problems were compared with children comorbid for externalizing and internalizing (MIXED) problems and with age-matched controls. Only boys were included in the analysis because so few girls were referred for treatment. We used a go/no-go task with a negative emotion induction, and we examined dense-array EEG data together with behavioral measures of performance. We investigated two event-related potential (ERP) components tapping inhibitory control or self-monitoring - the inhibitory N2 and error-related negativity (ERN) - and we constructed source models estimating their cortical generators. The MIXED children's N2s increased in response to the emotion induction, resulting in greater amplitudes than EXT children in the following trial block. ERN amplitudes were greatest for control children and smallest for EXT children with MIXED children in between, but only prior to the emotion induction. These results were paralleled by behavioral differences in response time and performance monitoring. ERP activity was localized to cortical sources suggestive of the dorsal anterior cingulate for control children, posterior cingulate areas for the EXT children, and both posterior cingulate and ventral cingulate/prefrontal regions for the MIXED children. These findings highlight different mechanisms of self-regulation underlying externalizing subtypes and point toward distinct developmental pathways and treatment strategies.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiopatología , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/fisiopatología , Electroencefalografía , Emociones/fisiología , Control Interno-Externo , Agresión/psicología , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Niño , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/psicología , Variación Contingente Negativa , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Giro del Cíngulo/fisiopatología , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Masculino , Motivación , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiopatología , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador , Factores Socioeconómicos
17.
Neuroreport ; 18(1): 61-5, 2007 Jan 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17259862

RESUMEN

Emotion regulation in adults may be mediated by frontal cortical activities that adjust attention in response to challenging emotions. We examined event-related potentials across emotional conditions to assess normative patterns and individual differences in cortical mechanisms of emotion regulation in 4-6-year-olds. The children viewed pictures of angry, neutral, and happy faces during a Go/No-go task. Angry faces generated the greatest (frontocentral) N2 amplitudes and fastest N2 latencies, and happy faces produced the smallest amplitudes and slowest latencies. Frontal electrodes showed larger N2s to angry faces in the Go condition. The P3b was also largest for angry faces. More fearful children showed faster latency N2s to angry faces. These results are interpreted in terms of early-developing mechanisms for regulating anxiety and processing emotional information.


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Preescolar , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
18.
Emotion ; 6(4): 656-62, 2006 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17144756

RESUMEN

Negative emotion has been shown to reduce flexibility in cognition and behavior. We examined interpersonal flexibility during negative emotional episodes within parent-child interactions. Fifty-five mothers and early-adolescent daughters were observed during a positive discussion, a negative (conflict) discussion, and another positive discussion. Codes of moment-to-moment changes in emotion expression were used to create state space grids from which measures of emotional valence and flexibility were derived. As expected, mean flexibility was lowest during the conflict discussion when negative emotion peaked, suggesting that interpersonal flexibility decreases with increasing negative emotion. Sub-groups identified as low or high in stress were also compared. Dyads with girls reporting more stressful events showed lower flexibility during the first positive discussion. However, dyads expressing more negative emotion during the conflict discussion were also more flexible, suggesting that flexible dyadic styles permit more negative emotion. These individual difference findings are discussed in terms of the suppression versus expression of negative emotions.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Conducta Espacial , Adulto , Niño , Cognición , Femenino , Humanos , Madres , Grabación de Cinta de Video
19.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 18(3): 430-43, 2006 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16513007

RESUMEN

Psychologists consider emotion regulation a critical developmental acquisition. Yet, there has been very little research on the neural underpinnings of emotion regulation across childhood and adolescence. We selected two ERP components associated with inhibitory control-the frontal N2 and frontal P3. We recorded these components before, during, and after a negative emotion induction, and compared their amplitude, latency, and source localization over age. Fifty-eight children 5-16 years of age engaged in a simple go/no-go procedure in which points for successful performance earned a valued prize. The temporary loss of all points triggered negative emotions, as confirmed by self-report scales. Both the frontal N2 and frontal P3 decreased in amplitude and latency with age, consistent with the hypothesis of increasing cortical efficiency. Amplitudes were also greater following the emotion induction, only for adolescents for the N2 but across the age span for the frontal P3, suggesting different but overlapping profiles of emotion-related control mechanisms. No-go N2 amplitudes were greater than go N2 amplitudes following the emotion induction at all ages, suggesting a consistent effect of negative emotion on mechanisms of response inhibition. No-go P3 amplitudes were also greater than go P3 amplitudes and they decreased with age, whereas go P3 amplitudes remained low. Finally, source modeling indicated a developmental decline in central-posterior midline activity paralleled by increasing activity in frontal midline regions suggestive of the anterior cingulate cortex. Negative emotion induction corresponded with an additional right ventral prefrontal or temporal generator beginning in middle childhood.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Adolescente , Envejecimiento/psicología , Niño , Preescolar , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Neurofisiología/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción , Caracteres Sexuales , Percepción del Tamaño
20.
Neuropsychologia ; 44(11): 2139-48, 2006.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16310813

RESUMEN

Dense-array (128-channel) electroencephalography (EEG) was used to record event-related potentials (ERPs) from 33 participants between 7 and 16 years of age while they performed a Go/Nogo task. The frontal (Nogo) N2 component of the ERP was taken as an index of cognitive control, and examined in relation to both age and independent assessments of executive function (EF), including the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), the Stroop task, a delay discounting task, and backward digit span. Better performance on the IGT and the Stroop task was associated with smaller N2 amplitudes, over and above effects of age. N2 latencies decreased with age but were not predicted by EF. Source modeling of the N2 revealed neural generators in areas suggestive of cingulate cortex and orbitofrontal cortex, and the locations of these generators varied systematically with EF (IGT and Stroop task): the cingulate generator was more anterior for good EF participants at all ages; the orbitofrontal generator was relatively left lateralized for younger and for poorer EF participants. Taken together, these findings suggest that age-related decreases in N2 amplitude, but not N2 latency, reflect the development of cognitive control and cannot be attributed solely to incidental changes that may affect assessments of the N2 (e.g., increases in skull thickness). Functionally relevant decreases in N2 amplitude may reflect changes in the regions of cortex giving rise to the N2.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Cognición/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adolescente , Niño , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Escalas de Wechsler
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...