Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 22
Filtrar
1.
Neurol Sci ; 28(4): 188-95, 2007 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17690850

RESUMEN

This detailed clinical report of a typical Capgras delusion (CD) in a demented patient is presented in order to foster future descriptions in neurological cases. In the framework of a recently developed model of familiar person processing, it is suggested that CD might be due to a dysfunction at the level of Person Identity Nodes. Prefrontal impairment is held to represent a critical factor leading to a failure of belief evaluation.


Asunto(s)
Síndrome de Capgras/fisiopatología , Deluciones , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
2.
Neurol Sci ; 28(1): 16-30, 2007 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17385091

RESUMEN

Ninety-eight healthy participants were examined with a new test of Famous Name Comprehension which, in the framework of a serial model of person processing, sequentially assessed Name Recognition (i.e., the ability to classify items as familiar or unfamiliar) and Person Identification (i.e., the ability to provide biographical knowledge of recognised items). Names were presented in a written format. A perfectly equivalent face version of the test allowed a comparison of familiarity and identification of people from name and from face input. Furthermore, the effect of the "age" of the items, i.e. the time elapsed from the presumed first exposure to the stimulus to the time of testing, was also investigated. Normative data are provided. Education was the only significant variable for recognition, while education, age and gender turned out to be significant for identification. Recognition was significantly better with name than with face input, while on identification names and faces did not differ significantly. "Oldest" items were both recognised and identified significantly worse than recent ones. The results of face-name comparison are interpreted in terms of the different opportunities to be exposed to names and faces, the relevance of visuoperceptual attributes linked to faces and the evidence of shared knowledge from different inputs. The relative advantage of recent celebrities supports the semantic characterisation of knowledge of famous people.


Asunto(s)
Comprensión/fisiología , Nombres , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/normas , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estándares de Referencia
3.
Neurol Sci ; 28(6): 315-22, 2007 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18175078

RESUMEN

Naming celebrities from visual input (i.e., face presentation) was examined in 98 healthy participants. Normative data are provided both for a 12-and a 63-item test. The first one is a brief screening tool, while the latter is to be administered whenever a pathological performance turns out in the former. Age adversely affected both performances. In both tests the most frequent errors were no responses, followed by semantic errors. The study makes explicit reference to current serial models of familiar person processing and naming. Findings allow to complement previously reported testing procedures, with the specific aim of qualifying proper name anomia by identifying the cognitive locus of lesion.


Asunto(s)
Nombres , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/normas , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción , Estándares de Referencia
4.
Neurol Sci ; 26(3): 161-7, 2005 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16086129

RESUMEN

Temporal gradient (TG), i.e., differential recall of recent and old memories, is a well known feature of amnesia. A recent study provided evidence of a classical TG for media-mediated events in elderly healthy people, showing that they recall remote events significantly better than recent ones, while a reverse TG, i.e., better recall of more recent events, was demonstrated in younger normal subjects. In the present study we present normative data which, using the same test, allow evaluation of TG in single cases and their qualification as classical or reverse. The normative procedure was also applied to a small sample of subjects with probable Alzheimer's disease or mild cognitive impairment. Norms for TG may be helpful not only to assess healthy people's performance, but also to judge any apparent TG in pathological subjects.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/complicaciones , Trastornos del Conocimiento/complicaciones , Trastornos de la Memoria/diagnóstico , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Anciano , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/fisiopatología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/fisiopatología , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/complicaciones , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Psicológicas , Valores de Referencia , Factores de Tiempo
5.
Neurol Sci ; 25(4): 205-15, 2004 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15549506

RESUMEN

We devised a new test for assessing remote memory for media-mediated events, i.e. events that are reported by and known through the media. The test consists of a verbal questionnaire covering famous events that occurred over a long period of time, from 1976 to 2000, specifically designed for use with the Italian population. A free recall procedure was adopted which makes use of progressive cues. The test can be easily updated for inclusion of future events. Normative and test-retest reliability data are presented. The test provides a new tool for assessing media-mediated memory whenever an extensive assessment of retrograde memory performance is required, both in normal people and in brain-damaged patients.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Memoria/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Memoria/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/normas , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Amnesia Retrógrada/diagnóstico , Amnesia Retrógrada/psicología , Aniversarios y Eventos Especiales , Lesiones Encefálicas/diagnóstico , Lesiones Encefálicas/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Masculino , Medios de Comunicación de Masas , Persona de Mediana Edad , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
6.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord ; 11(1): 17-24, 2000.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10629357

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: To evaluate the efficacy and safety of eptastigmine as a treatment for patients with mild-to-moderate Alzheimer's disease (AD). PATIENTS AND METHODS: The study was designed as a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group study. It was performed in 26 Italian and American geriatric and neurological centers. The study group comprised 349 outpatients with a diagnosis of probable AD according to the criteria of the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the AD and Related Disorders Association. Patients were assigned to one of the three study groups: placebo (n = 119), eptastigmine 10 mg t.i.d. (n = 115) or eptastigmine 12 mg t.i.d. (n = 115) for 25 weeks. The AD Assessment Cognitive Subscale (ADAS-Cog) and the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale-Sum of the Boxes (CDR-SB) were the primary outcome measures for efficacy. RESULTS: The two doses of eptastigmine produced similar results and are presented together. Percentages of patients completing double-blind treatment were 82 and 87% in the placebo and eptastigmine groups, respectively. At the end of treatment, the intent to-treat analysis on 342 patients showed a statistically significant effect of eptastigmine compared to placebo on both ADAS-Cog (p = 0.047) and CDR-SB (p = 0.010). Patients on eptastigmine performed significantly better than placebo-treated patients also on the Mini-Mental State Examination (p < 0.001). The drug was well tolerated with 5% of patients withdrawing due to adverse events versus 3% on placebo. Adverse events were recorded in 46% of the patients on placebo compared to 52% of the patients taking eptastigmine. Cholinergic side effects (nausea, vomiting, diarrhea and abdominal pain) were reported with similar frequency in the eptastigmine and placebo-treated patients. CONCLUSION: Eptastigmine doses up to 12 mg t.i.d. for 25 weeks are well tolerated. The drug positively affects cognitive performance and global function of patients with mild-to-moderate AD.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/tratamiento farmacológico , Inhibidores de la Colinesterasa/administración & dosificación , Fisostigmina/análogos & derivados , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Inhibidores de la Colinesterasa/efectos adversos , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Método Doble Ciego , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Fisostigmina/administración & dosificación , Fisostigmina/efectos adversos , Resultado del Tratamiento
7.
Neurology ; 52(4): 700-8, 1999 Mar 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10078713

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To evaluate the efficacy and safety of eptastigmine in patients with moderate to moderately severe AD. BACKGROUND: Eptastigmine is a centrally acting cholinesterase inhibitor. METHODS: The study was carried out according a multicenter, randomized, double-blinded, placebo-controlled, parallel-group design. Patients received a 24-week treatment with placebo or eptastigmine 15 mg or 20 mg three times daily after a 4-week, stepwise dose escalation. The effects of treatment on cognition, global function, and activities of daily living were evaluated with the Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Cognitive Subscale (ADAS-Cog), the Clinician's Interview-Based Impression of Change Plus (CIBIC-Plus), and the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living scale (IADL), respectively. RESULTS: Thirty-six centers recruited 491 patients: 164 on placebo, 166 on eptastigmine 15 mg three times daily, and 161 on eptastigmine 20 mg three times daily. Percentages of patients completing double-blinded treatment were 87% in the placebo group and 86% in both the eptastigmine-treated groups. At the end of treatment, the intent-to-treat analysis on 463 patients showed a dose-dependent effect of eptastigmine on all efficacy variables, with a statistically significant effect of the 20 mg three times daily dose compared with placebo on the ADAS-Cog, CIBIC-Plus, and IADL. Patients on eptastigmine 15 mg three times daily performed significantly better than placebo-treated patients only on the ADAS-Cog. Eleven patients on placebo (7%), 13 patients on eptastigmine 15 mg three times daily (8%), and 12 patients on eptastigmine 20 mg three times daily (8%) discontinued study treatment because of adverse events. Adverse events were recorded in 49% of patients on placebo compared with 54% on eptastigmine 15 mg three times daily and 48% on eptastigmine 20 mg three times daily. Cholinergic side effects (nausea, vomiting, diarrhea, and abdominal pain) were reported with similar frequency in the eptastigmine- and placebo-treated patients. There was a dose-dependent transient and mild neutropenic effect associated with eptastigmine treatment, and one patient on 20 mg three times daily had an asymptomatic pancytopenia. CONCLUSIONS: Eptastigmine produces significant cognitive, clinical, and functional benefits in patients with probable AD. Although the cholinergic tolerability of eptastigmine was found to be favorable, its potential adverse hematologic effects limit its clinical utility.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/tratamiento farmacológico , Inhibidores de la Colinesterasa/efectos adversos , Inhibidores de la Colinesterasa/uso terapéutico , Fisostigmina/análogos & derivados , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Inhibidores de la Colinesterasa/administración & dosificación , Método Doble Ciego , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fisostigmina/administración & dosificación , Fisostigmina/efectos adversos , Fisostigmina/uso terapéutico , Resultado del Tratamiento
8.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord ; 12(4): 313-22, 1998 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9876959

RESUMEN

The efficacy and safety of eptastigmine in patients with probable Alzheimer disease was evaluated in a double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel group study. Patients with mild to moderate dementia were randomly assigned to placebo, eptastigmine 10 mg three times a day (t.i.d.), or eptastigmine 15 mg t.i.d. over 25 weeks. The Alzheimer Disease Assessment Cognitive Subscale (ADAS-Cog) and the Clinician's Interview-Based Impression of Change Plus (CIBIC-Plus) were the primary outcome measures for efficacy. Twenty-six centers recruited 320 patients: 106 on placebo, 105 on eptastigmine 10 mg t.i.d., and 109 on eptastigmine 15 mg t.i.d. Six patients on placebo (6%), 18 patients on eptastigmine 10 mg t.i.d. (17%), and 10 patients on eptastigmine 15 mg t.i.d. (9%) discontinued study treatment. The intent-to-treat analysis on 315 patients showed a statistically significant (p=0.047) difference of 2.0 points on ADAS-Cog between the placebo and the eptastigmine 15 mg t.i.d. group at the end of treatment. Patients in the 10 mg t.i.d. group performed better than did placebo-treated patients on the Spontaneous Behavior Interview (SBI) total scores (p=0.015) and on the Activities of Daily Living (ADL, p=0.043) and Behavioral Problems (BP, p=0.028) subscales. The differences in favor of the eptastigmine groups on the CIBIC-Plus did not reach statistical significance. In a post hoc subgroup analysis by staging, the effect size of eptastigmine was found to be greater in the most severely impaired patients (Global Deterioration Scale rating of 4 and 5 at screening) reaching statistical significance in both ADAS-Cog (p=0.007) and CIBIC-Plus (p=0.038). In this patient subgroup (n=222), there was also a significant effect of eptastigmine on SBI (p=0.019). The drug was generally well tolerated, with 8% of patients withdrawing due to adverse events versus 5% on placebo. Adverse events were recorded in 35 patients (33%) on placebo compared with 41 (39%) on eptastigmine 10 mg t.i.d. and 38 (35%) on eptastigmine 15 mg t.i.d. This study shows that eptastigmine doses up to 15 mg t.i.d. for 25 weeks are well tolerated. The drug positively affects cognitive performance of Alzheimer patients. This effect appears greater in more severely impaired patients and also impacts on their behavioral performance.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/tratamiento farmacológico , Inhibidores de la Colinesterasa/uso terapéutico , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Fisostigmina/análogos & derivados , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Inhibidores de la Colinesterasa/efectos adversos , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Método Doble Ciego , Esquema de Medicación , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Escala del Estado Mental , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fisostigmina/efectos adversos , Fisostigmina/uso terapéutico , Resultado del Tratamiento
9.
Neuropsychologia ; 35(6): 781-94, 1997 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9204485

RESUMEN

Following a car accident, a patient remained unconscious for approximately 20 min and confused for a few hours. When he could be questioned, he was found to have lost all past memories. The retrograde amnesia covered his whole life and concerned autobiographic events as well as famous facts and encyclopaedic knowledge. It also partially involved the verbal and visual lexicon. Reading, writing and counting were no longer possible. The profound impairment of retrograde memory contrasted with the preservation of anterograde memory, which permitted the patient to reacquire some of the notions he had lost, without, however, recovering the feeling of a personal experience of autobiographical information. Four years later, the retrograde deficit was unmodified, except for what had been relearnt. The search for data in support of an organic or psychological aetiology was negative. No signs of brain damage were apparent at the neurological examination and on CT, MRI and SPECT. On the other hand, there was no evidence of a psychiatric history, psychological stress or emotional precipitants that could substantiate the hypothesis that the patient derived a primary or secondary gain from amnesia. We propose that cases of focal retrograde amnesia, similar to the present one, deserve to be classified separately from organic and psychogenic forms under the label of 'functional' retrograde amnesia, a syndrome in which the threshold of activation of premorbid memories is abnormally raised by the trauma, leaving the encoding and retrieval of new memories unaffected.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia Retrógrada/etiología , Encéfalo/patología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Traumatismos Craneocerebrales/complicaciones , Agrafia/etiología , Amnesia Retrógrada/fisiopatología , Amnesia Retrógrada/psicología , Anomia/etiología , Apraxias/etiología , Dislexia Adquirida/etiología , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estrés Psicológico/etiología , Inconsciencia/complicaciones
10.
Cortex ; 31(3): 531-42, 1995 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8536480

RESUMEN

A 19-year-old man showed dense retrograde amnesia (RA) for autobiographical and public events covering his entire life, following a motor car accident that caused no apparent brain damage. His learning abilities remained excellent and permitted him to recover knowledge of the past, based on information gathered from other people and the media, although he never reacquired the sense of personal experience. At the onset he also showed occasional gaps in his semantic memory, e.g., he failed to recognize a few objects and was unable to provide factual information about sports he had long been playing. Eventually, his amnesia was predominantly restricted to autobiographical events or facts. After 29 months the deficit has remained unchanged. CT, MRI and PET were negative. Psychogenic amnesia was considered, but eventually ruled out for want of any evidence pointing to emotional problems, stressful situations or secondary gains. We posit that for reasons unknown a mild trauma can cause functional inhibition of the access to the engrams that are already stored, leaving intact the ability to encode and retrieve new memories.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia Retrógrada/fisiopatología , Conmoción Encefálica/fisiopatología , Daño Encefálico Crónico/fisiopatología , Adulto , Amnesia Retrógrada/diagnóstico , Amnesia Retrógrada/psicología , Conmoción Encefálica/diagnóstico , Conmoción Encefálica/psicología , Daño Encefálico Crónico/diagnóstico , Daño Encefálico Crónico/psicología , Corteza Cerebral/lesiones , Corteza Cerebral/fisiopatología , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Humanos , Potenciación a Largo Plazo/fisiología , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Retención en Psicología/fisiología
11.
Brain ; 118 ( Pt 1): 167-83, 1995 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7895003

RESUMEN

We report two cases of recovery from retrograde amnesia, which occurred almost suddenly, 1 year and 1 month from onset, respectively. Amnesia followed a left thalamic infarction in one patient and a mild head trauma in the other. Full and permanent recovery occurred within a short time after the spontaneous emergence to consciousness of a single autobiographical event, triggered by the specific experience of a very similar new event. Recovery was limited to retrograde amnesia, leaving unaffected the anterograde deficit present in one of the patients. The nature (psychogenic versus organic) of our patients' memory loss is commented upon in the light of a review of previously reported cases of retrograde amnesia. The transient defect of retrograde memory is discussed in terms of a reversible distortion of the neuronal 'patterned matrices' suggested by Gloor (Brain 1990; 113: 1673-94).


Asunto(s)
Amnesia/etiología , Amnesia/psicología , Adulto , Anciano , Amnesia/fisiopatología , Traumatismos Craneocerebrales/complicaciones , Humanos , Infarto/complicaciones , Masculino , Memoria , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tálamo/irrigación sanguínea
12.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 57(11): 1366-70, 1994 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7964813

RESUMEN

A patient had a slowly developing amnesic syndrome that remained substantially unchanged during the two and a half years of observation. Intellectual skills were excellent and there was no language, perception, praxis, or calculation deficit. The memory impairment involved verbal and visual learning, sparing spatial learning and, to a large extent, retrograde memory. Magnetic resonance imaging was normal, but PET showed a hypometabolism of the left temporal mesial region and thalamus. This case extends the spectrum of monosymptomatic cognitive disorders, previously reported in the area of language, praxis, and visual recognition, to amnesia.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia Retrógrada/diagnóstico , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Tomografía Computarizada de Emisión , Anciano , Amnesia Retrógrada/metabolismo , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Femenino , Humanos , Aprendizaje , Enfermedades Metabólicas/diagnóstico por imagen , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tálamo/diagnóstico por imagen , Tálamo/metabolismo
13.
Cortex ; 30(1): 3-25, 1994 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8004989

RESUMEN

Following herpes encephalitis, a patient showed impaired knowledge of animals, fruits and vegetables, flowers and food (so called living things categories), whatever the modality in which stimuli were presented and responses were given. A series of experiments showed that the deficit specifically affected the ability to retrieve the perceptual features of the living stimuli defining their shape, while knowledge of their functional-encyclopedic properties was preserved. The patient had no problems with man-made objects, except when the recall of their colour, or the identification of their sound was requested. It is argued that the retrieval of the perceptual features was potentially disrupted for every type of category, but that the block was compensated for man-made objects, because the close correspondence between shape and function that characterises them provided an alternative route to access their structured form representations. On this account, the selective deficit for living categories seems contingent on the interaction between an overall cognitive impairment--the deficit in retrieving perceptual features--and some intrinsic properties of the stimulus--the factors that have modelled its form--and cannot be taken as evidence that semantic systems are allotted to separate cerebral areas.


Asunto(s)
Anomia/fisiopatología , Daño Encefálico Crónico/fisiopatología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiopatología , Formación de Concepto/fisiología , Encefalitis/fisiopatología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Semántica , Anomia/diagnóstico , Anomia/psicología , Daño Encefálico Crónico/diagnóstico , Daño Encefálico Crónico/psicología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Encefalitis/diagnóstico , Encefalitis/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Aprendizaje por Asociación de Pares/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Conducta Verbal
14.
Cortex ; 29(3): 449-66, 1993 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8258285

RESUMEN

Following a thoracic trauma, which caused brain hypoxia, a 24-year-old man presented with a dense retrograde amnesia for events, persons and environments that spanned his whole life before injury. Knowledge acquired at school or through the media were equally lost, with the exception of arithmetical skills and some geographical knowledge. No deficit was apparent in language, object recognition, motor skills and intellectual tests. Anterograde memory was marked by very efficient learning capacity, an almost perfect retention of information at delay interval of 4 hours and pathologically rapid forgetting at longer delays. Informal observations agreed with test performance in showing that he could relearn facts of the past and easily acquire new information, but tended to lose these memories if the information was not frequently rehearsed. PET showed a hypometabolism of the posterior temporal lobes. Though some points remain obscure, we propose that a consolidation deficit hypothesis provides the most sensible interpretation of this peculiar pattern of memory deficit.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia Retrógrada/psicología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Adulto , Amnesia Retrógrada/patología , Amnesia Retrógrada/terapia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/patología , Cognición/fisiología , Humanos , Hipoxia Encefálica/etiología , Hipoxia Encefálica/patología , Hipoxia Encefálica/psicología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/patología , Trastornos de la Memoria/terapia , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Traumatismos Torácicos/complicaciones , Tomografía Computarizada de Emisión , Aprendizaje Verbal/fisiología
15.
Cortex ; 29(2): 187-215, 1993 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8348820

RESUMEN

Following a trauma that mainly involved the right hemisphere, a 21-year-old girl showed a profound impairment in visual object recognition, without language and intellectual deficit. Her elementary sensory functions were preserved and she performed in the normal range on visual matching tasks, on taks requiring to detect small differences between similar complex shapes and in copying drawings, without any evidence of a line by line approach. Her deficit emerged with tests that, though not implying identification of meaning, demanded to disentangle a form from a confused background and to achieve a highly structured description of the stimulus. In addition to this high-level perceptual processing disorder, there was a deficit in recovering from the visual store the shape of an object, also when the performance did not involve perceptual discrimination, e.g., in drawing from memory or telling the physical difference between two named stimuli. Knowledge of the semantic and contextual attributes of objects was intact. The case is taken as evidence that the borders of apperceptive agnosia may be ampler than usually thought and its distinction from associative agnosia less rigid, with some patients laying in-between the two syndromes.


Asunto(s)
Agnosia/fisiopatología , Daño Encefálico Crónico/fisiopatología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Agnosia/diagnóstico , Agnosia/psicología , Daño Encefálico Crónico/diagnóstico , Daño Encefálico Crónico/psicología , Hemorragia Cerebral/diagnóstico , Hemorragia Cerebral/fisiopatología , Hemorragia Cerebral/psicología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Dislexia Adquirida/diagnóstico , Dislexia Adquirida/fisiopatología , Dislexia Adquirida/psicología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Femenino , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Traumatismos Cerrados de la Cabeza/diagnóstico , Traumatismos Cerrados de la Cabeza/fisiopatología , Traumatismos Cerrados de la Cabeza/psicología , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Lóbulo Occipital/lesiones , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiopatología , Orientación/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/lesiones , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología
16.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 56(3): 304-7, 1993 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7681473

RESUMEN

A patient had an infarct in the territory of the left anterior cerebral artery, which destroyed the medial cortex of the frontal lobe, and presented with a picture of primary dyscalculia. Lexical and syntactic processing of verbal and arabic numbers and comprehension of operation symbols were intact, but retrieval of basic, over-learned facts was mildly impaired and execution of calculation procedures was more severely impaired. As the same type of procedure could be passed or sometimes failed it suggests a deficit of activation of the appropriate procedures. The location of the lesion was unusual and suggested participation of medial frontal areas in calculation processes.


Asunto(s)
Afasia/fisiopatología , Infarto Cerebral/fisiopatología , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiopatología , Matemática , Adulto , Afasia/diagnóstico , Daño Encefálico Crónico/diagnóstico , Daño Encefálico Crónico/fisiopatología , Neoplasias Cerebelosas/fisiopatología , Neoplasias Cerebelosas/cirugía , Infarto Cerebral/diagnóstico , Glioma/fisiopatología , Glioma/cirugía , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Complicaciones Posoperatorias/diagnóstico , Complicaciones Posoperatorias/fisiopatología , Solución de Problemas/fisiología
17.
Cortex ; 28(2): 221-30, 1992 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1499308

RESUMEN

Following a left thalamic infarct, a patient showed a marked impairment in retrieving person proper names in response to faces and to verbal description, despite being able to provide precise information about the persons he could not name and to point to their photograph when the name was provided by the examiner. The patient was also impaired in generating proper names, but could easily retrieve common names as well as geographical names and names of monuments. It is hypothesized that the arbitrary nature of the link between proper names and their referents makes access of phonological forms from the semantic store particularly labile. In agreement with this interpretation is the patient's inability to recall telephone numbers and to learn semantically arbitrary paired associates.


Asunto(s)
Anomia/psicología , Anciano , Anomia/etiología , Infarto Cerebral/complicaciones , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Semántica
18.
Brain ; 113 ( Pt 5): 1337-45, 1990 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2245300

RESUMEN

A 22-yr-old subject with moderate developmental dyslexia experienced poor memory from an early age. In spite of normal intelligence he had great difficulty in learning historical and geographical names, mathematical formulae, poems and songs, and verbal series such as the letters of the alphabet and the months of the year. His recognition of familiar faces was also faulty at times. A battery of memory tests confirmed a moderate deficit of verbal as well as visual memory, whereas spatial memory was preserved. Other cognitive functions were intact, except for a mild reading disability. No evidence of brain pathology was provided by his history, neurological examination and CT scanning.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia/complicaciones , Trastornos de la Memoria/complicaciones , Adulto , Dislexia/psicología , Humanos , Inteligencia , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Memoria , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
19.
J Gerontol ; 43(6): P157-63, 1988 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3183312

RESUMEN

Gottschaldt's Hidden Figure Test (GHFT) calls for figure-ground discrimination abilities. The aim of this study was to provide a perceptual attention test for the cognitive assessment of demented patients. In Experiment 1, GHFT normal baselines on 190 healthy controls are achieved, and age-education score correction as well as transformation of the original scores into "Equivalent Scores" is established. On the inferential percentile allocation of 45 mildly-moderately deteriorated DAT patients, 65% of them performed in the critically low range of the score distribution. In Experiment 2, the discriminant power of GHFT between DAT patients and healthy controls matched by age and Experiment 2, the discriminant power of GHFT between DAT patients and healthy controls matched by age and education is assessed. Overall correct classification reached 90%. This experiment shows that GHFT in demented patients works chiefly as an attentional task. The discussion is based largely on methodological and statistical issues.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Demencia/parasitología , Percepción de Forma , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Percepción/fisiología , Pruebas Psicológicas , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Análisis de Regresión , Percepción Espacial , Percepción Visual
20.
Brain ; 111 ( Pt 5): 1173-85, 1988 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3179688

RESUMEN

Ideational apraxia was investigated in 20 left brain-damaged patients with tests requiring the demonstration of how objects are used. On a multiple object use test the most frequent errors were those of omission, misuse and mislocation, while sequence errors were rare. Patients also failed on a single object use test, which showed a correlation of 0.85 with the multiple object use test. Neither of these tests was significantly correlated with an ideomotor apraxia test (imitation of movements). Ideational apraxia was frequently, but not exclusively, associated with damage to the left posterior temporoparietal junction. These findings support the view that ideational apraxia is an autonomous syndrome, linked to left hemisphere damage and pertaining to the area of semantic memory disorders rather than to that of defective motor control.


Asunto(s)
Apraxias/clasificación , Apraxias/diagnóstico por imagen , Apraxias/psicología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Daño Encefálico Crónico/complicaciones , Daño Encefálico Crónico/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tomografía Computarizada por Rayos X
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...