Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 6 de 6
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Clin Rehabil ; 33(4): 619-630, 2019 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30537847

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES:: To investigate immediate changes in walking performance associated with three implicit motor learning strategies and to explore patient experiences of each strategy. DESIGN:: Participants were randomly allocated to one of three implicit motor learning strategies. Within-group comparisons of spatiotemporal parameters at baseline and post strategy were performed. SETTING:: Laboratory setting. SUBJECTS:: A total of 56 community-dwelling post-stroke individuals. INTERVENTIONS:: Implicit learning strategies were analogy instructions, environmental constraints and action observation. Different analogy instructions and environmental constraints were used to facilitate specific gait parameters. Within action observation, only videotaped gait was shown. MAIN MEASURES:: Spatiotemporal measures (speed, step length, step width, step height) were recorded using Vicon 3D motion analysis. Patient experiences were assessed by questionnaire. RESULTS:: At a group level, three of the four analogy instructions ( n = 19) led to small but significant changes in speed ( d = 0.088 m/s), step height (affected side d = 0.006 m) and step width ( d = -0.019 m), and one environmental constraint ( n = 17) led to significant changes in step width ( d = -0.040 m). At an individual level, results showed wide variation in the magnitude of changes. Within action observation ( n = 20), no significant changes were found. Overall, participants found it easy to use the different strategies and experienced some changes in their walking performance. CONCLUSION:: Analogy instructions and environmental constraints can lead to specific, immediate changes in the walking performance and were in general experienced as feasible by the participants. However, the response of an individual patient may vary quite considerably.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Neurológicos de la Marcha/rehabilitación , Aprendizaje , Destreza Motora , Rehabilitación de Accidente Cerebrovascular/métodos , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Velocidad al Caminar
2.
J Med Internet Res ; 19(12): e371, 2017 12 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29233801

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: There is little evidence considering the relationship between movement-specific reinvestment (a dimension of personality which refers to the propensity for individuals to consciously monitor and control their movements) and working memory during motor skill performance. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) measuring oxyhemoglobin demands in the frontal cortex during performance of virtual reality (VR) psychomotor tasks can be used to examine this research gap. OBJECTIVE: The aim of this study was to determine the potential relationship between the propensity to reinvest and blood flow to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortices of the brain. A secondary aim was to determine the propensity to reinvest and performance during 2 dental tasks carried out using haptic VR simulators. METHODS: We used fNIRS to assess oxygen demands in 24 undergraduate dental students during 2 dental tasks (clinical, nonclinical) on a VR haptic simulator. We used the Movement-Specific Reinvestment Scale questionnaire to assess the students' propensity to reinvest. RESULTS: Students with a high propensity for movement-specific reinvestment displayed significantly greater oxyhemoglobin demands in an area associated with working memory during the nonclinical task (Spearman correlation, rs=.49, P=.03). CONCLUSIONS: This small-scale study suggests that neurophysiological differences are evident between high and low reinvesters during a dental VR task in terms of oxyhemoglobin demands in an area associated with working memory.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Simulación por Computador/estadística & datos numéricos , Odontología/normas , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Adulto , Competencia Clínica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Realidad Virtual
3.
Curr Opin Psychol ; 16: 54-57, 2017 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28813355

RESUMEN

Working-memory capacity has been implicated as an influential variable when performing and learning sport-related skills. In this review, we critically evaluate evidence linking working-memory capacity with performing under pressure, tactical decision making, motor skill acquisition, and sport expertise. Laboratory experiments link low working-memory capacity with poorer performance under pressure and poorer decision making when required to inhibit distractions or resolve conflict. However, the generalizability of these findings remains unknown. While working-memory capacity is associated with the acquisition of simple motor skills, there is no such evidence from the available data for complex motor skills. Likewise, currently there is no evidence to suggest that a larger working-memory capacity facilitates the attainment of sport expertise.

4.
Geriatr Gerontol Int ; 16(12): 1305-1311, 2016 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26531243

RESUMEN

AIM: To examine whether differences emerged when the Chinese version of the Movement-Specific Reinvestment Scale (MSRS-C) was administered to community-dwelling older adults with instructions to respond in the context of "general" movements, walking, using chopsticks or dressing. Furthermore, the difference between the six-point Likert scale and four-point Likert scale response formats of the MSRS-C was investigated. METHODS: The study was implemented in the community of Hong Kong with 52 older adults (mean age 77.4 years). Telephone interviews were carried out on two occasions for each participant. Participants provided a verbal response to each of 10 questions from the MSRS-C with different response formats (i.e., six-point or four-point Likert Scales) and different instructions in the response context (i.e. general, walking, using chopsticks, dressing). The sequence of response format and context was randomized for each participant. RESULTS: Older fallers scored significantly higher on the MSRS-C (general) with six-point or four-point response formats than non-fallers. The MSRS-C (general) and MSRS-C (walking) were not statistically different, and showed good discriminative power for previous older fall status (older fallers or older non-fallers). However, MSRS-C (chopsticks) and MSRS-C (dressing) failed to differentiate older fallers from older non-fallers. CONCLUSION: Both the MSRS-C (general) and MSRS-C (walking) with a six-point or a four-point response format showed good discrimination of older fallers from non-fallers. Older adults might respond to the MSRS-C with respect to the most challenging movements (e.g. fall-related movements) in their daily living. Geriatr Gerontol Int 2016; 16: 1305-1311.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes por Caídas , Evaluación Geriátrica/métodos , Vida Independiente , Caminata , Anciano , Femenino , Hong Kong , Humanos , Masculino , Movimiento
5.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22230189

RESUMEN

An implicit approach to motor learning suggests that relatively complex movement skills may be better acquired in environments that constrain errors during the initial stages of practice. This current concept paper proposes that reducing the number of errors committed during motor learning leads to stable performance when attention demands are increased by concurrent cognitive tasks. While it appears that this approach to practice may be beneficial for motor learning, further studies are needed to both confirm this advantage and better understand the underlying mechanisms. An approach involving error minimization during early learning may have important applications in paediatric rehabilitation.

6.
Diabetes Metab Syndr Obes ; 4: 225-8, 2011.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21760739

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: There is increasing evidence that continuous activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal axis and the central sympathetic nervous system contributes to the pathogenesis of central adiposity via increased psychological stress. The purpose of this study was to examine the link between central adiposity and the propensity for Chinese children to rehearse emotionally upsetting events, a dimension of psychological stress. Additionally, gender differences in this relationship were explored. METHODS: Waist circumference, which is a marker of central adiposity and associated risks of developing cardiovascular disease, was measured and the propensity for rehearsal was assessed twice over two consecutive years in Hong Kong Chinese children (n = 194, aged 7-9 years), using a psychometric tool. RESULTS: Children with waist circumference indicative of a risk of cardiovascular disease displayed higher rehearsal scores than children categorized as "not at risk", as did boys compared with girls. Our results suggest that central adiposity and the propensity for rehearsal of emotionally upsetting events may be linked in Chinese children. CONCLUSION: Future prospective studies examining the direction of causality between central adiposity and rehearsal can potentially have valuable clinical implications.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA