Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 16 de 16
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Huntingtons Dis ; 12(3): 189-200, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37718849

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Huntington's disease (HD) is characterized by a loss of control of motor function that causes the presence of abnormal eye movements at early stages. OBJECTIVE: To determine if, compared to normal sheep, HD sheep have abnormal eye movements. METHODS: We measured eye movements in a transgenic sheep (Ovis aries) model of HD using a purpose-built, head-mounted sheep oculometer. This allows us to measure saccades without the need for either behavioral training or head fixation. At the age of testing (6 years old), the HD sheep were pre-manifest. We used 21 sheep (11 HD, 10 normal). RESULTS: We found small but significant differences in eye movements between normal (control) and HD sheep during vestibular ocular reflex (VOR)- and vestibular post-rotational nystagmus (PRN)-based tests. CONCLUSIONS: Two measures were identified that could distinguish normal from HD sheep; the number of PRN oscillations when tested in the dark and the gain (eye movement to head movement ratio) during the VOR when tested in the light. To our knowledge, this is the first study in which eye movements have been quantified in sheep. It demonstrates the feasibility of measuring and quantifying human-relevant eye movements in this species. The HD-relevant deficits show that even in 'premanifest' sheep there are measurable signs of neurological dysfunction that are characterized by loss of control of eye movements.

2.
Proc Biol Sci ; 289(1983): 20221311, 2022 09 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36168756

RESUMEN

Behavioural needs are highly motivated actions critical to a species survival and reproduction. Prolonged restriction of these behaviours can lead to stereotypic behaviours (SB) in captive animals, and this is particularly common in ungulate species. While risk factors for SB have been suggested for some ungulates, no study has integrated these findings to identify which aspects of ungulates' wild behavioural biology and captive husbandry are potential drivers for SB across this clade. We collated SB data from 15 236 individuals across 38 ungulate species from 95 sources, and determined species wild/free-ranging behaviour from 559 additional studies. Bayesian-phylogenetic statistical methods showed that ungulate behavioural needs relating to foraging and mating are particularly affected by captive environments, with promiscuous and browsing species showing the greatest prevalence of SB. Concentrate-only diets and lack of ad libitum feed substrates were also associated with high SB prevalence. This study identifies which ungulates are better suited to captive environments and which species require targeted husbandry, enrichment and breeding protocols in order to meet their behavioural needs. Our approach of applying Bayesian-phylogenetic inference to factors influencing SB within a clade can be used to identify other intrinsic and extrinsic risk factors of reduced animal health and welfare.


Asunto(s)
Mamíferos , Conducta Estereotipada , Animales , Teorema de Bayes , Filogenia , Factores de Riesgo
3.
Neuropharmacology ; 196: 108681, 2021 09 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34175323

RESUMEN

Acute stressors are recurrent in multiple species' lives and can facilitate or impair cognition. The use of zebrafish (Danio rerio) as a translational species to understand the mechanisms by which stress induces different behavioral phenotypes has been widely studied. Two acute stressors are recognized when using this species: (1) conspecific alarm substance (CAS); and (2) net chasing. Here, we tested if CAS or net chasing would affect working memory and cognitive flexibility by testing performance in the FMP Y-maze after exposure to stress. We observed that CAS altered zebrafish behavioral phenotypes by increasing repetitive behavior; meanwhile, animals showed different patterns of repetitive behavior when exposed to net chasing, depending on the chasing direction. Because D1 receptors were previously studied as a potential mechanism underlying stress responses in different species, here, we pretreated fish with a D1/D5 agonist (SKF-38393) to assess whether this system plays a role in repetitive behavior in the FMP Y-maze. The pretreatment with D1/D5 agonist significantly decreased repetitive behavior in CAS exposed animals, and cortisol levels for both stressed groups, suggesting that the dopaminergic system plays an important role in zebrafish stress-related responses.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Receptores de Dopamina D1/metabolismo , Receptores de Dopamina D5/metabolismo , Estrés Psicológico/metabolismo , 2,3,4,5-Tetrahidro-7,8-dihidroxi-1-fenil-1H-3-benzazepina/farmacología , Animales , Conducta Animal/efectos de los fármacos , Cognición/efectos de los fármacos , Agonistas de Dopamina/farmacología , Aprendizaje por Laberinto/efectos de los fármacos , Aprendizaje por Laberinto/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/efectos de los fármacos , Feromonas , Receptores de Dopamina D1/agonistas , Receptores de Dopamina D5/agonistas , Conducta Estereotipada/efectos de los fármacos , Conducta Estereotipada/fisiología , Pez Cebra
4.
Behav Res Methods ; 53(2): 536-557, 2021 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32748238

RESUMEN

Numerous neurodegenerative and psychiatric disorders are associated with deficits in executive functions such as working memory and cognitive flexibility. Progress in developing effective treatments for disorders may benefit from targeting these cognitive impairments, the success of which is predicated on the development of animal models with validated behavioural assays. Zebrafish offer a promising model for studying complex brain disorders, but tasks assessing executive function are lacking. The Free-movement pattern (FMP) Y-maze combines aspects of the common Y-maze assay, which exploits the inherent motivation of an organism to explore an unknown environment, with analysis based on a series of sequential two-choice discriminations. We validate the task as a measure of working memory and executive function by comparing task performance parameters in adult zebrafish treated with a range of glutamatergic, cholinergic and dopaminergic drugs known to impair working memory and cognitive flexibility. We demonstrate the cross-species validity of the task by assessing performance parameters in adapted versions of the task for mice and Drosophila, and finally a virtual version in humans, and identify remarkable commonalities between vertebrate species' navigation of the maze. Together, our results demonstrate that the FMP Y-maze is a sensitive assay for assessing working memory and cognitive flexibility across species from invertebrates to humans, providing a simple and widely applicable behavioural assay with exceptional translational relevance.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Animales , Encéfalo , Aprendizaje por Laberinto , Ratones , Motivación , Pez Cebra
5.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 21409, 2020 12 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33293559

RESUMEN

Measuring animal stress is fundamentally important for assessing animal emotional state and welfare. Conventional methods of quantifying stress (cortisol levels, heart rate/heart rate variability) require specialist equipment and are not instantly available. Spontaneous blink rate (SBR) has previously been used to measure stress responses in humans and may provide a non-invasive method for measuring stress in other animal species. Here we investigated the use of SBR as a measure of stress in the domestic horse. SBR was measured before and during a low-stress event (sham clipping) and compared with heart rate variability and salivary cortisol. For the entire sample, there was a reduction in SBR (startle response) during the first minute of clipping. For horses reactive to clipping, the initial reduction in SBR was followed by an increase above baseline whereas the SBR of the non-reactive horses quickly returned to baseline. For the entire sample, SBR correlated with heart rate variability and salivary cortisol. We have demonstrated that SBR is a valid fast alternative measure of stress in horses, but the initial 'startle' response must be considered when using this parameter as a measure of animal stress.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Parpadeo , Estrés Psicológico/diagnóstico , Animales , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Caballos , Hidrocortisona/análisis , Modelos Animales , Saliva/química , Estrés Psicológico/metabolismo , Estrés Psicológico/fisiopatología
6.
Behav Brain Res ; 367: 101-110, 2019 07 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30926483

RESUMEN

Abnormal repetitive behaviors (ARBs) are a prominent symptom of numerous human brain disorders and are commonly seen in rodent models as well. While rodent studies of ARBs continue to dominate the field, mounting evidence suggests that zebrafish (Danio rerio) also display ARB-like phenotypes and may therefore be a novel model organism for ARB research. In addition to clear practical research advantages as a model species, zebrafish share high genetic and physiological homology to humans and rodents, including multiple ARB-related genes and robust behaviors relevant to ARB. Here, we discuss a wide spectrum of stereotypic repetitive behaviors in zebrafish, data on their genetic and pharmacological modulation, and the overall translational relevance of fish ARBs to modeling human brain disorders. Overall, the zebrafish is rapidly emerging as a new promising model to study ARBs and their underlying mechanisms.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Trastornos del Neurodesarrollo/fisiopatología , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/fisiopatología , Trastorno de Movimiento Estereotipado/fisiopatología , Pez Cebra/fisiología , Animales , Disfunción Cognitiva/genética , Humanos , Trastornos del Neurodesarrollo/genética , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/genética , Trastorno de Movimiento Estereotipado/genética
7.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(12): 3379-3390, 2018 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30267138

RESUMEN

Understanding the cognitive capacities of animals is important, because (a) several animal models of human neurodegenerative disease are considered poor representatives of the human equivalent and (b) cognitive capacities may provide insight into alternative animal models. We used a three-stage process of cognitive and neuroanatomical comparison (using sheep as an example) to assess the appropriateness of a species to model human brain function. First, a cognitive task was defined via a reinforcement-learning algorithm where values/constants in the algorithm were taken as indirect measures of neurophysiological attributes. Second, cognitive data (values/constants) were generated for the example species (sheep) and compared to other species. Third, cognitive data were compared with neuroanatomical metrics for each species (endocranial volume, gyrification index, encephalisation quotient, and number of cortical neurons). Four breeds of sheep (n = 15/sheep) were tested using the two-choice discrimination-reversal task. The 'reversal index' was used as a measure of constants within the learning algorithm. Reversal index data ranked sheep as third in a table of species that included primates, dogs, and pigs. Across all species, number of cortical neurons correlated strongest against the reversal index (r2 = 0.66, p = 0.0075) followed by encephalization quotient (r2 = 0.42, p = 0.03), endocranial volume (r2 = 0.30, p = 0.08), and gyrification index (r2 = 0.16, p = 0.23). Sheep have a high predicted level of cognitive capacity and are thus a valid alternative model for neurodegenerative research. Using learning algorithms within cognitive tasks increases the resolution of methods of comparative cognition and can help to identify the most relevant species to model human brain function and dysfunction.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Modelos Animales , Modelos Psicológicos , Algoritmos , Animales , Condicionamiento Operante/fisiología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Humanos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Aprendizaje Inverso/fisiología , Ovinos , Especificidad de la Especie
8.
Behav Brain Res ; 353: 108-113, 2018 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29964072

RESUMEN

Spontaneous stereotypic behaviours are repetitive, compulsive, topographically invariant response patterns commonly observed in captive or domestic animals, which have been linked to dysfunction of basal ganglia input/output pathways. There is evidence that endogenous opioids play a key regulatory role in basal ganglia direct and indirect pathways, but their precise role, both causally and functionally, in spontaneous stereotypic behaviour is unclear. Here we examined the profile of mu- and delta-opioid receptors (density [Bmax] and affinity [Kd]) of basal ganglia structures in stereotypy (n = 10) and non-stereotypy (n = 10) animals using a competitive ligand binding approach. Mu receptor densities were significantly higher in the nucleus accumbens (p < 0.001), ventral tegmentum area (p < 0.001) and caudate nuclei (p < 0.001) of stereotypy compared to control animals. No differences were observed for delta Bmax values in any of the brain regions studied (p > 0.15). Receptor binding affinity was only found to be significantly different between control and stereotypy animals for mu receptors on the caudate region; (p < 0.001). Our findings suggest that increased inhibition (via mu-opioid receptors) of the indirect (dorsal striatopallidal) pathways are associated with spontaneous stereotypy development. Data also suggested that different types of spontaneous stereotypies (e.g. oral versus locomotor) within or a cross species may have a different neurological basis. This may have important implications for understanding the aetiology and function of these behaviours. In some instances (oral stereotypy), the behaviour may be associated with allostasis, a process that could enhance the reward value of appetitive behaviour performance (as a starting point of stereotypy development).


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/metabolismo , Boca , Movimiento/fisiología , Receptores Opioides delta/metabolismo , Receptores Opioides mu/metabolismo , Conducta Estereotipada/fisiología , Animales , Encéfalo/patología , Femenino , Enfermedades de los Caballos/metabolismo , Enfermedades de los Caballos/patología , Caballos , Masculino , Boca/fisiopatología , Vías Nerviosas/metabolismo , Vías Nerviosas/patología , Fenotipo , Trastorno de Movimiento Estereotipado/metabolismo , Trastorno de Movimiento Estereotipado/patología
9.
Anim Cogn ; 20(4): 615-626, 2017 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28389761

RESUMEN

Huntington's disease (HD) patients show reduced flexibility in inhibiting an already-started response. This can be quantified by the stop-signal task. The aim of this study was to develop and validate a sheep version of the stop-signal task that would be suitable for monitoring the progression of cognitive decline in a transgenic sheep model of HD. Using a semi-automated operant system, sheep were trained to perform in a two-choice discrimination task. In 22% of the trials, a stop-signal was presented. Upon the stop-signal presentation, the sheep had to inhibit their already-started response. The stopping behaviour was captured using an accelerometer mounted on the back of the sheep. This set-up provided a direct read-out of the individual stop-signal reaction time (SSRT). We also estimated the SSRT using the conventional approach of subtracting the stop-signal delay (i.e., time after which the stop-signal is presented) from the ranked reaction time during a trial without a stop-signal. We found that all sheep could inhibit an already-started response in 91% of the stop-trials. The directly measured SSRT (0.974 ± 0.04 s) was not significantly different from the estimated SSRT (0.938 ± 0.04 s). The sheep version of the stop-signal task adds to the repertoire of tests suitable for investigating both cognitive dysfunction and efficacy of therapeutic agents in sheep models of neurodegenerative disease such as HD, as well as neurological conditions such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Tiempo de Reacción , Ovinos , Animales , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Inhibición Psicológica
10.
J Neurosci Methods ; 292: 45-52, 2017 Dec 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28179180

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Large animal models of human neurological disorders are advantageous compared to rodent models due to their neuroanatomical complexity, longevity and their ability to be maintained in naturalised environments. Some large animal models spontaneously develop behaviours that closely resemble the symptoms of neural and psychiatric disorders. The horse is an example of this; the domestic form of this species consistently develops spontaneous stereotypic behaviours akin to the compulsive and impulsive behaviours observed in human neurological disorders such as Tourette's syndrome. The ability to non-invasively probe normal and abnormal equine brain function through cognitive testing may provide an extremely useful methodological tool to assess brain changes associated with certain human neurological and psychiatric conditions. NEW METHOD: An automated operant system with the ability to present visual and auditory stimuli as well as dispense salient food reward was developed. To validate the system, ten horses were trained and tested using a standard cognitive task (three choice serial reaction time task (3-CSRTT)). RESULTS: All animals achieved total learning criterion and performed six probe sessions. Learning criterion was met within 16.30±0.79 sessions over a three day period. During six probe sessions, level of performance was maintained at 80.67±0.57% (mean±SEM) accuracy. COMPARISON WITH EXISTING METHOD(S): This is the first mobile fully automated system developed to examine cognitive function in the horse. CONCLUSIONS: A fully-automated operant system for mobile cognitive function of a large animal model has been designed and validated. Horses pose an interesting complementary model to rodents for the examination of human neurological dysfunction.


Asunto(s)
Automatización de Laboratorios , Cognición , Caballos/psicología , Aprendizaje , Actividad Motora , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Acústica , Análisis de Varianza , Animales , Conducta Animal , Femenino , Masculino , Modelos Animales , Estimulación Luminosa
11.
Physiol Behav ; 167: 125-131, 2016 12 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27597134

RESUMEN

A relationship between dopamine and temperament has previously been described in human cases of dopaminergic dysfunction. Adjustment in temperament prior to disease manifestation can enable the early identification of individuals at risk of such conditions, and scope exists to extend this application of temperament alterations to cases of dopaminergic dysfunction in horses. A multivariate and mixed-methods approach utilising a questionnaire along with two inferred measurements of dopamine activity (Spontaneous Blink Rate [SBR] and behavioral initiation rate [BIR]) were recorded from direct observation of animals (n=99) to identify the potential relationship between dopamine and temperament in horses. Principal components analysis (PCA) of 36 temperament variables revealed nine principal components, including 'Anxiety' and 'Docility', which accounted for 72.4% of the total variance. Component scores were calculated and correlated with SBR and BIR utilising Spearman rank correlation coefficient analysis. The component 'Anxiety' was found to have a significant positive relationship with SBR, whereas 'Docility' was observed to have a significant negative relationship with SBR. These results indicate a relationship between dopamine and temperament within the horse that is certainly worthy of further study. Potential mechanisms involving neural dopaminergic and GABAergic systems are presented, in addition to how such alterations could be utilised to probe for equine dopamine dysfunction pending future research.


Asunto(s)
Parpadeo/fisiología , Caballos/fisiología , Caballos/psicología , Personalidad/fisiología , Animales , Cruzamiento , Dopamina/metabolismo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis de Componente Principal , Factores Sexuales , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
12.
J Neurosci Methods ; 265: 25-33, 2016 05 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26327320

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: For reasons of cost and ethical concerns, models of neurodegenerative disorders such as Huntington disease (HD) are currently being developed in farm animals, as an alternative to non-human primates. Developing reliable methods of testing cognitive function is essential to determining the usefulness of such models. Nevertheless, cognitive testing of farm animal species presents a unique set of challenges. The primary aims of this study were to develop and validate a mobile operant system suitable for high throughput cognitive testing of sheep. NEW METHOD: We designed a semi-automated testing system with the capability of presenting stimuli (visual, auditory) and reward at six spatial locations. Fourteen normal sheep were used to validate the system using a two-choice visual discrimination task. Four stages of training devised to acclimatise animals to the system are also presented. RESULTS: All sheep progressed rapidly through the training stages, over eight sessions. All sheep learned the 2CVDT and performed at least one reversal stage. The mean number of trials the sheep took to reach criterion in the first acquisition learning was 13.9±1.5 and for the reversal learning was 19.1±1.8. COMPARISON WITH EXISTING METHOD(S): This is the first mobile semi-automated operant system developed for testing cognitive function in sheep. CONCLUSIONS: We have designed and validated an automated operant behavioural testing system suitable for high throughput cognitive testing in sheep and other medium-sized quadrupeds, such as pigs and dogs. Sheep performance in the two-choice visual discrimination task was very similar to that reported for non-human primates and strongly supports the use of farm animals as pre-clinical models for the study of neurodegenerative diseases.


Asunto(s)
Automatización de Laboratorios/instrumentación , Cognición , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Enfermedad de Huntington/psicología , Estimulación Acústica , Animales , Condicionamiento Operante , Discriminación en Psicología , Diseño de Equipo , Femenino , Alimentos , Habituación Psicofisiológica , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Pruebas Psicológicas , Aprendizaje Inverso , Recompensa , Oveja Doméstica , Percepción Visual
13.
Behav Brain Res ; 276: 45-58, 2015 Jan 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25052167

RESUMEN

Spontaneous stereotypic behaviour (SB) is common in many captive animal species, as well as in humans with some severe psychiatric disorders, and is often cited as being related to general basal ganglia dysfunction. Despite this assertion, there is little in the literature examining SB specifically in terms of the basal ganglia mechanics. In this review, we attempt to fill this gap by offering an integrative, cross-domain perspective of SB by linking what we currently understand about the SB phenotype with the ever-growing literature on the anatomy and functionality of the basal ganglia. After outlining current models of SB from different theoretical perspectives, we offer a broad but detailed overview of normally functioning basal ganglia mechanics, and attempt to link this with current neurophysiological evidence related to spontaneous SB. Based on this we present an empirically derived theoretical framework, which proposes that SB is the result of a dysfunctional action selection system that may reflect dysregulation of excitatory (direct) and inhibitory (indirect and hyperdirect) pathways as well as alterations in mechanisms of behavioural switching. This approach also suggests behaviours that specifically become stereotypic may reflect inbuilt low selection threshold behavioural sequences associated with early development and the species-specific ethogram or, low threshold behavioural sequences that are the result of stress-induced dopamine exposure at the time of performance.


Asunto(s)
Ganglios Basales/fisiología , Ganglios Basales/fisiopatología , Neuronas Dopaminérgicas/fisiología , Conducta Estereotipada/fisiología , Trastorno de Movimiento Estereotipado/fisiopatología , Animales , Humanos , Mesencéfalo/fisiología , Mesencéfalo/fisiopatología , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Psicológicos , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología
14.
BMC Vet Res ; 8: 180, 2012 Sep 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23016987

RESUMEN

For optimal individual performance within any equestrian discipline horses must be in peak physical condition and have the correct psychological state. This review discusses the psychological factors that affect the performance of the horse and, in turn, identifies areas within the competition horse industry where current behavioral research and established behavioral modification techniques could be applied to further enhance the performance of animals. In particular, the role of affective processes underpinning temperament, mood and emotional reaction in determining discipline-specific performance is discussed. A comparison is then made between the training and the competition environment and the review completes with a discussion on how behavioral modification techniques and general husbandry can be used advantageously from a performance perspective.


Asunto(s)
Caballos/psicología , Condicionamiento Físico Animal/psicología , Crianza de Animales Domésticos , Bienestar del Animal , Animales , Conducta Animal , Deportes , Temperamento
15.
Behav Brain Res ; 200(1): 100-5, 2009 Jun 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19162081

RESUMEN

Significant similarities exist between the neural and behavioural features of environmentally and drug-induced stereotypy. For example, exposure to dopamine agonists, such as amphetamine, induces stereotypy and causes alterations in midbrain neurophysiology similar to those observed following chronic stress. An additional behavioural feature of these neural changes in the drug-induced phenotype is an enhanced rate of switching from response-outcome (R-O) to stimulus-response (S-R) learning. The aim of the current experiment was to examine R-O and S-R learning in horses displaying environmentally induced oral stereotypies. This was achieved by employing variations of the place/response paradigm. In Experiment 1, we found that crib-biting horses displayed 'response' learning after 20 learning trials, whereas non-crib-biting controls tended to display 'place' learning throughout the experiment. In Experiment 2, we used a modified version of the procedure, in which the subjects were introduced to the maze from different start points and forced always to turn the same way. We found that the crib-biters acquired the task at a faster rate suggesting again that this group was displaying 'response' learning. Finally, in Experiment 3, we carried out an arena test to ensure that crib-biters were capable of 'place' learning. These results are the first to show that horses displaying an oral stereotypy, a behavioural phenotype previously associated with stress-induced perturbations of the basal ganglia, preferentially use 'response' learning. The findings are discussed in relation to the search for an aetiological model of stereotypy.


Asunto(s)
Condicionamiento Operante/fisiología , Caballos/fisiología , Boca , Conducta Espacial/fisiología , Conducta Estereotipada/fisiología , Administración Oral , Análisis de Varianza , Animales , Conducta Animal , Distribución de Chi-Cuadrado , Femenino , Aprendizaje por Laberinto/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Esquema de Refuerzo
16.
Behav Brain Res ; 191(1): 137-40, 2008 Aug 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18430476

RESUMEN

Horses displaying an oral stereotypy were tested on an instrumental choice paradigm to examine differences in learning from non-stereotypic counterparts. Stereotypic horses are known to have dysfunction of the dorsomedial striatum, and lesion studies have shown that this region may mediate response-outcome learning. The paradigm was specifically applied in order to examine learning that requires maintenance of response-outcome judgements. The non-stereotypic horses learned, over three sessions, to choose a more immediate reinforcer, whereas the stereotypic horses failed to do so. This suggests an initial behavioural correlate for dorsomedial striatum dysregulation in the stereotypy phenotype.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Enfermedades de los Caballos/fisiopatología , Conducta Estereotipada/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal , Condicionamiento Operante , Caballos , Esquema de Refuerzo , Refuerzo en Psicología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...