Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 31
Filtrar
1.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 17509, 2024 07 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39080377

RESUMEN

Expectation of a future stimulus increases the preparedness to act once it actually appears and results in reduced latency of the appropriate motor response. Real world events are uncertain both spatially and/or temporally but this uncertainty could itself be expected. In the presence of both expected spatial and temporal uncertainty, which one should be prioritized by the motor system could depend on the context. Therefore, we investigated the relative weight of expected spatial and temporal uncertainty during the preparation of a saccadic eye movement. A reaction time task was used with a variable foreperiod between a warning and an imperative visual stimuli. Expected temporal and/or spatial uncertainty associated with the stimulus was cued. We found that before imperative stimulus onset, pupil dilation increased with expected temporal uncertainty but was unaltered by spatial uncertainty. After imperative stimulus onset, both types of expected uncertainty affected saccade latency. Maximum eye velocity was modulated by expected spatial uncertainty only. In conclusion, expected temporal and spatial uncertainty do not have the same impact on preparation and execution of a motor response. There could be a prioritization of the relevant information as a function of the evolving expected uncertainty context during the task.


Asunto(s)
Pupila , Tiempo de Reacción , Movimientos Sacádicos , Humanos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Pupila/fisiología , Incertidumbre , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Estimulación Luminosa
2.
eNeuro ; 10(9)2023 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37669857

RESUMEN

Subjective uncertainty arises because the estimation of the timing of an event into the future is error prone. This impact of stimulus-bound uncertainty on movement preparation has often been investigated using reaction time tasks where a warning stimulus (WS) predicts the occurrence of a "go" signal. The timing of the "go" signal can be chosen from a particular probability distribution with a given variance or uncertainty. It has been repeatedly shown that reaction times covary with the shape of the used "go" signal distribution. This is interpreted as evidence for temporal preparation. Moreover, the variance of the response time should always increase with the duration of the delay between the WS and the "go" signal. This increasing variance has been interpreted as a consequence of the temporal "blurring" of future events (scalar expectancy). The present paper tested the validity of the temporal "blurring" hypothesis in humans with a simple oculomotor reaction time task where subjective and stimulus-bound uncertainties were increased. Subjective uncertainty about the timing of a "go" signal was increased by lengthening the delay between the WS and the "go" signal. Objective uncertainty was altered by increasing the variance of "go" signal timing. Contrary to temporal blurring hypotheses, the study has shown that increasing the delay between events did not significantly increase movement timing variability. These results suggest that temporal blurring could not be a property of movement timing in an implicit timing context.


Asunto(s)
Movimiento , Humanos , Probabilidad , Tiempo de Reacción , Incertidumbre
3.
NPJ Parkinsons Dis ; 8(1): 125, 2022 Oct 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36184657

RESUMEN

Anticipatory actions require to keep track of elapsed time and inhibitory control. These cognitive functions could be impacted in Parkinson's disease (iPD). To test this hypothesis, a saccadic reaction time task was used where a visual warning stimulus (WS) predicted the occurrence of an imperative one (IS) appearing after a short delay. In the implicit condition, subjects were not informed about the duration of the delay, disfavoring anticipatory behavior but leaving inhibitory control unaltered. In the explicit condition, delay duration was cued. This should favor anticipatory behavior and perhaps alter inhibitory control. This hypothesis was tested in controls (N = 18) and age-matched iPD patients (N = 20; ON and OFF L-DOPA). We found that the latency distribution of saccades before the IS was bimodal. The 1st mode weakly depended on temporal information and was more prominent in iPD. Saccades in this mode were premature and could result of a lack of inhibition. The 2nd mode covaried with cued duration suggesting that these movements were genuine anticipatory saccades. The explicit condition increased the probability of anticipatory saccades before the IS in controls and iPDON but not iPDOFF patients. Furthermore, in iPD patients the probability of sequences of 1st mode premature responses increased. In conclusion, the triggering of a premature saccade or the initiation of a controlled anticipatory one could be conceptualized as the output of two independent stochastic processes. Altered time perception and increased motor impulsivity could alter the balance between these two processes in favor of the latter in iPD, particularly OFF L-Dopa.

4.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 2543, 2022 02 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35169177

RESUMEN

Expected surprise, defined as the anticipation of uncertainty associated with the occurrence of a future event, plays a major role in gaze shifting and spatial attention. In the present study, we analyzed its impact on oculomotor behavior. We hypothesized that the occurrence of anticipatory saccades could decrease with increasing expected surprise and that its influence on visually-guided responses could be different given the presence of sensory information and perhaps competitive attentional effects. This hypothesis was tested in humans using a saccadic reaction time task in which a cue indicated the future stimulus position. In the 'no expected surprise' condition, the visual target could appear only at one previously cued location. In other conditions, more likely future positions were cued with increasing expected surprise. Anticipation was more frequent and pupil size was larger in the 'no expected surprise' condition compared with all other conditions, probably due to increased arousal. The latency of visually-guided saccades increased linearly with the logarithm of surprise (following Hick's law) but their maximum velocity repeated the arousal-related pattern. Therefore, expected surprise affects anticipatory and visually-guided responses differently. Moreover, these observations suggest a causal chain linking surprise, attention and saccades that could be disrupted in attentional or impulse control disorders.


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica , Movimientos Oculares , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
5.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 238(2): 559-567, 2021 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33169200

RESUMEN

RATIONALE: Ketamine, a well-known general dissociative anesthetic agent that is a non-competitive antagonist of the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor, perturbs the perception of elapsed time and the expectation of upcoming events. OBJECTIVE: The objective of this study was to determine the influence of ketamine on temporal expectation in the rhesus monkey. METHODS: Two rhesus monkeys were trained to make a saccade between a central warning stimulus and an eccentric visual target that served as imperative stimulus. The delay between the warning and the imperative stimulus could take one of four different values randomly with the same probability (variable foreperiod paradigm). During experimental sessions, a subanesthetic low dose of ketamine (0.25-0.35 mg/kg) was injected i.m. and the influence of the drug on movement latency was measured. RESULTS: We found that in the control conditions, saccadic latencies strongly decreased with elapsed time before the appearance of the visual target showing that temporal expectation built up during the delay period between the warning and the imperative stimulus. However, after ketamine injection, temporal expectation was significantly reduced in both subjects. In addition, ketamine also increased average movement latency but this effect could be dissociated from the reduction of temporal expectation. CONCLUSION: In conclusion, a subanesthetic dose of ketamine could have two independent effects: increasing reaction time and decreasing temporal expectation. This alteration of temporal expectation could explain cognitive deficits observed during ketamine use.


Asunto(s)
Anestésicos Disociativos/farmacología , Ketamina/farmacología , Motivación/efectos de los fármacos , Tiempo de Reacción/efectos de los fármacos , Receptores de N-Metil-D-Aspartato/antagonistas & inhibidores , Movimientos Sacádicos/efectos de los fármacos , Animales , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Femenino , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Distribución Aleatoria
6.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 13: 258, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31824272

RESUMEN

Patient suffering of major depressive disorder (MDD) often complain that subjective time seems to "drag" with respect to physical time. This may point toward a generalized dysfunction of temporal processing in MDD. In the present study, we investigated temporal preparation in MDD. "Temporal preparation" refers to an increased readiness to act before an expected event; consequently, reaction time should be reduced. MDD patients and age-matched controls were required to make a saccadic eye movement between a central and an eccentric visual target after a variable duration preparatory period. We found that MDD patients produced a larger number of premature saccades, saccades initiated prior to the appearance of the expected stimulus. These saccades were not temporally controlled; instead, they seemed to reflect reduced inhibitory control causing oculomotor impulsivity. In contrast, the latency of visually guided saccades was strongly influenced by temporal preparation in controls; significantly less so, in MDD patients. This observed reduced temporal preparation in MDD was associated with a faster decay of short-term temporal memory. Moreover, in patients producing a lot of premature responses, temporal preparation to early imperative stimuli was increased. In conclusion, reduced temporal preparation and short-term temporal memory in the oculomotor domain supports the hypothesis that temporal processing was altered in MDD patients. Moreover, oculomotor impulsivity interacted with temporal preparation. These observed deficits could reflect other underlying aspects of abnormal time experience in MDD.

7.
Sci Rep ; 8(1): 7637, 2018 05 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29769545

RESUMEN

In a rapidly changing environment, we often know when to do something before we have to do it. This preparation in the temporal domain is based on a 'perception' of elapsed time and short-term memory of previous stimulation in a similar context. These functions could be perturbed in Parkinson's disease. Therefore, we investigated their role in eye movement preparation in sporadic Parkinson's disease and in a very infrequent variant affecting the Parkin gene. We used a simple oculomotor task where subjects had to orient to a visual target and movement latency was measured. We found that in spite of an increased average reaction time, the influence of elapsed time on movement preparation was similar in controls and the two groups of PD patients. However, short-term temporal memory of previous stimulation was severely affected in sporadic PD patients either ON or OFF dopaminergic therapy. We conclude that the two different contributions to temporal preparation could be dissociated. Moreover, a short-term temporal memory deficit might underlie temporal cognition deficits previously observed in PD.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva/etiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Mutación , Enfermedad de Parkinson/complicaciones , Ubiquitina-Proteína Ligasas/genética , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Disfunción Cognitiva/metabolismo , Disfunción Cognitiva/patología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/metabolismo , Trastornos de la Memoria/patología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Enfermedad de Parkinson/genética , Enfermedad de Parkinson/fisiopatología , Tiempo de Reacción
8.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 372(1718)2017 Apr 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28242734

RESUMEN

If a visual object of interest suddenly starts to move, we will try to follow it with a smooth movement of the eyes. This smooth pursuit response aims to reduce image motion on the retina that could blur visual perception. In recent years, our knowledge of the neural control of smooth pursuit initiation has sharply increased. However, stopping smooth pursuit eye movements is less well understood and will be discussed in this paper. The most straightforward way to study smooth pursuit stopping is by interrupting image motion on the retina. This causes eye velocity to decay exponentially towards zero. However, smooth pursuit stopping is not a passive response, as shown by behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Moreover, smooth pursuit stopping is particularly influenced by active prediction of the upcoming end of the target. Here, we suggest that a particular class of inhibitory neurons of the brainstem, the omnipause neurons, could play a central role in pursuit stopping. Furthermore, the role of supplementary eye fields of the frontal cortex in smooth pursuit stopping is also discussed.This article is part of the themed issue 'Movement suppression: brain mechanisms for stopping and stillness'.


Asunto(s)
Tronco Encefálico/fisiología , Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Percepción Visual , Animales , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Macaca mulatta/psicología
10.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 232(19): 3563-72, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26153067

RESUMEN

RATIONALE: It has been shown that antagonism of the glutamatergic N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor with subanesthetic doses of ketamine perturbs the perception of elapsed time. Anticipatory eye movements are based on an internal representation of elapsed time. Therefore, the occurrence of anticipatory saccades could be a particularly sensitive indicator of abnormal time perception due to NMDA receptors blockade. OBJECTIVES: The objective of this study was to determine whether the occurrence of anticipatory saccades could be selectively altered by a subanesthetic dose of ketamine. METHODS: Three Rhesus monkeys were trained in a simple visually guided saccadic task with a variable delay. Monkeys were rewarded for making a visually guided saccade at the end of the delay. Premature anticipatory saccades to the future position of the eccentric target initiated before the end of the delay were not rewarded. A subanesthetic dose of ketamine (0.25 mg/kg) or a saline solution of the same volume was injected i.m. during the task. RESULTS: We found that the injected dose of ketamine did not induce sedation or abnormal behavior. However, in ∼4 min, ketamine induced a strong reduction of the occurrence of anticipatory saccades but did not reduce the occurrence of visually guided saccades. CONCLUSION: This unexpected reduction of anticipatory saccade occurrence could be interpreted as resulting from an altered use of the perception of elapsed time during the delay period induced by NMDA receptors antagonism.


Asunto(s)
Anestésicos Disociativos/administración & dosificación , Anticipación Psicológica/efectos de los fármacos , Ketamina/administración & dosificación , Tiempo de Reacción/efectos de los fármacos , Movimientos Sacádicos/efectos de los fármacos , Animales , Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/efectos de los fármacos , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Femenino , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Recompensa , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología
11.
PLoS One ; 9(4): e93958, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24728140

RESUMEN

The passage of time can be estimated either explicitly, e.g. before leaving home in the morning, or implicitly, e.g. when catching a flying ball. In the present study, the latency of saccadic eye movements was used to evaluate differences between implicit and explicit timing. Humans were required to make a saccade between a central and a peripheral position on a computer screen. The delay between the extinction of a central target and the appearance of an eccentric target was the independent variable that could take one out of four different values (400, 900, 1400 or 1900 ms). In target trials, the delay period lasted for one of the four durations randomly. At the end of the delay, a saccade was initiated by the appearance of an eccentric target. Cue&target trials were similar to target trials but the duration of the delay was visually cued. In probe trials, the duration of the upcoming delay was cued, but there was no eccentric target and subjects had to internally generate a saccade at the estimated end of the delay. In target and cue&target trials, the mean and variance of latency distributions decreased as delay duration increased. In cue&target trials latencies were shorter. In probe trials, the variance increased with increasing delay duration and scalar variability was observed. The major differences in saccadic latency distributions were observed between visually-guided (target and cue&target trials) and internally-generated saccades (probe trials). In target and cue&target trials the timing of the response was implicit. In probe trials, the timing of the response was internally-generated and explicitly based on the duration of the visual cue. Scalar timing was observed only during probe trials. This study supports the hypothesis that there is no ubiquitous timing system in the brain but independent timing processes active depending on task demands.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Músculos Oculomotores/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología
12.
J Vis ; 12(5): 3, 2012 Jan 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22593089

RESUMEN

When two objects such as billiard balls collide, observers perceive that the action of one caused the motion of the other. We have previously shown (Badler, Lefèvre, & Missal, 2010) that this extends to the oculomotor domain: subjects make more predictive movements in the expected direction of causal motion than in a noncausal direction. However, predictive oculomotor and reactive psychophysical responses have never been directly compared. They should be correlated if they tap into the same mental processes. To test this, we recorded oculomotor responses to launching stimuli, then asked subjects to manually classify those stimuli as causal or noncausal. Overall the psychophysical classifications matched the oculomotor biases, although correlations across subjects were mostly absent. In subsequent experiments, 50% of the trials had a 300-millisecond delay after the collision to impede the perception of causality. Subjects maintained their causal oculomotor bias but used different classification strategies, usually grouping the stimuli either by delay or by direction. In addition, there was no evidence that the two response types were correlated on a trial-by-trial basis. The results suggest divergent processes underlying oculomotor responses to and judgments of causal stimuli.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Psicofísica/métodos , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Valores de Referencia
13.
PLoS One ; 6(10): e26699, 2011.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22046334

RESUMEN

Impulsivity is the tendency to act without forethought. It is a personality trait commonly used in the diagnosis of many psychiatric diseases. In clinical practice, impulsivity is estimated using written questionnaires. However, answers to questions might be subject to personal biases and misinterpretations. In order to alleviate this problem, eye movements could be used to study differences in decision processes related to impulsivity. Therefore, we investigated correlations between impulsivity scores obtained with a questionnaire in healthy subjects and characteristics of their anticipatory eye movements in a simple smooth pursuit task. Healthy subjects were asked to answer the UPPS questionnaire (Urgency Premeditation Perseverance and Sensation seeking Impulsive Behavior scale), which distinguishes four independent dimensions of impulsivity: Urgency, lack of Premeditation, lack of Perseverance, and Sensation seeking. The same subjects took part in an oculomotor task that consisted of pursuing a target that moved in a predictable direction. This task reliably evoked anticipatory saccades and smooth eye movements. We found that eye movement characteristics such as latency and velocity were significantly correlated with UPPS scores. The specific correlations between distinct UPPS factors and oculomotor anticipation parameters support the validity of the UPPS construct and corroborate neurobiological explanations for impulsivity. We suggest that the oculomotor approach of impulsivity put forth in the present study could help bridge the gap between psychiatry and physiology.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Conducta Impulsiva , Individualidad , Humanos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
14.
J Neurosci ; 31(28): 10234-40, 2011 Jul 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21753000

RESUMEN

Feeling the beat and meter is fundamental to the experience of music. However, how these periodicities are represented in the brain remains largely unknown. Here, we test whether this function emerges from the entrainment of neurons resonating to the beat and meter. We recorded the electroencephalogram while participants listened to a musical beat and imagined a binary or a ternary meter on this beat (i.e., a march or a waltz). We found that the beat elicits a sustained periodic EEG response tuned to the beat frequency. Most importantly, we found that meter imagery elicits an additional frequency tuned to the corresponding metric interpretation of this beat. These results provide compelling evidence that neural entrainment to beat and meter can be captured directly in the electroencephalogram. More generally, our results suggest that music constitutes a unique context to explore entrainment phenomena in dynamic cognitive processing at the level of neural networks.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Periodicidad , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Imaginación/fisiología , Masculino , Música , Red Nerviosa/fisiología
15.
Vision Res ; 50(24): 2721-8, 2010 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20800610

RESUMEN

Humans are very sensitive to the presence of other living persons or animals in their surrounding. Human actions can readily be perceived, even in a noisy environment. We recently demonstrated that biological motion, which schematically represents human motion, influences smooth pursuit eye movements during the initiation period (Orban de Xivry, Coppe, Lefèvre, & Missal, 2010). This smooth pursuit response is driven both by a visuomotor pathway, which transforms retinal inputs into motor commands, and by a memory pathway, which is directly related to the predictive properties of smooth pursuit. To date, it is unknown which of these pathways is influenced by biological motion. In the present study, we first use a theoretical model to demonstrate that an influence of biological motion on the visuomotor and memory pathways might both explain its influence on smooth pursuit initiation. In light of this model, we made theoretical predictions of the possible influence of biological motion on smooth pursuit during and after the transient blanking of the stimulus. These qualitative predictions were then compared with recordings of eye movements acquired before, during and after the transient blanking of the stimulus. The absence of difference in smooth pursuit eye movements during blanking of the stimuli and the stronger visually guided smooth pursuit reacceleration after reappearance of the biological motion stimuli in comparison with control stimuli suggests that biological motion influences the visuomotor pathway but not the memory pathway.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Modelos Teóricos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
16.
J Neurosci ; 30(31): 10517-25, 2010 Aug 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20685994

RESUMEN

When viewing one object move after being struck by another, humans perceive that the action of the first object "caused" the motion of the second, not that the two events occurred independently. Although established as a perceptual and linguistic concept, it is not yet known whether the notion of causality exists as a fundamental, preattentional "Gestalt" that can influence predictive motor processes. Therefore, eye movements of human observers were measured while viewing a display in which a launcher impacted a tool to trigger the motion of a second "reaction" target. The reaction target could move either in the direction predicted by transfer of momentum after the collision ("causal") or in a different direction ("noncausal"), with equal probability. Control trials were also performed with identical target motion, either with a 100 ms time delay between the collision and reactive motion, or without the interposed tool. Subjects made significantly more predictive movements (smooth pursuit and saccades) in the causal direction during standard trials, and smooth pursuit latencies were also shorter overall. These trends were reduced or absent in control trials. In addition, pursuit latencies in the noncausal direction were longer during standard trials than during control trials. The results show that causal context has a strong influence on predictive movements.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
17.
J Vis ; 10(2): 6.1-11, 2010 Feb 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20462307

RESUMEN

Presenting a few dots moving coherently on a screen can yield to the perception of human motion. This perception is based on a specific network that is segregated from the traditional motion perception network and that includes the superior temporal sulcus (STS). In this study, we investigate whether this biological motion perception network could influence the smooth pursuit response evoked by a point-light walker. We found that smooth eye velocity during pursuit initiation was larger in response to the point-light walker than in response to one of its scrambled versions, to an inverted walker or to a single dot stimulus. In addition, we assessed the proximity to the point-light walker (i.e. the amount of information about the direction contained in the scrambled stimulus and extracted from local motion cue of biological motion) of each of our scrambled stimuli in a motion direction discrimination task with manual responses and found that the smooth pursuit response evoked by those stimuli moving across the screen was modulated by their proximity to the walker. Therefore, we conclude that biological motion facilitates smooth pursuit eye movements, hence influences both perception and action.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Adulto , Algoritmos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Caminata , Adulto Joven
18.
J Vis ; 9(11): 7.1-16, 2009 Oct 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20053070

RESUMEN

Visually guided catch-up saccades during the pursuit of a moving target are highly influenced by smooth pursuit performance. For example, the decision to execute a saccade and its amplitude is driven by the difference in velocity between the eye and the target. In previous studies, we have demonstrated that the predictive saccades that occur during the blanking of the moving target compensate for the variability of the smooth pursuit response. Therefore, we wondered whether the predictive smooth pursuit response during target blanking influenced the occurrence of predictive saccades, which is the case for visually guided catch-up saccades. To answer this question, we asked subjects to track visually a target moving along a circular path. From time to time, the target was unexpectedly blanked for some randomized durations and disappeared from the screen. Surprisingly, we did not find any differences in smooth pursuit performance between the blanks that did and those that did not contain predictive saccades. In addition, during the blanks, the differences in smooth pursuit performance across the sessions or across the subjects did not correlate with the differences in the number of predictive saccades. Therefore, this study demonstrates that smooth pursuit performance does not influence the occurrence of predictive saccades. We interpret these results in light of the possible minimization of position error at target reappearance, which heavily depends on the saccadic amplitudes but not on their timing.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
19.
J Neurosci ; 28(17): 4298-310, 2008 Apr 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18434507

RESUMEN

Expectation of upcoming events is an essential cognitive function on which anticipatory actions are based. The neuronal basis of this prospective representation is poorly understood. We trained rhesus monkeys in a smooth-pursuit task in which the direction of upcoming target motion was indicated using a color cue. Under these conditions, directional expectation frequently evoked anticipatory smooth movements. We found that the activity of a population of neurons in the supplementary eye fields encoded the expected future direction of the target. Neuronal activity increased after presentation of the cue, indicating future target motion in the preferred direction. Neuronal activity either remained unaltered or was reduced if the antipreferred direction was cued. In addition, approximately 30% of these neurons were more active during trials with anticipatory pursuit in the preferred direction than during trials when monkeys did not anticipate target motion onset. This subset of recorded neurons encoded the direction of the subsequent anticipatory pursuit. We hypothesize that the neural representation of directional expectation could be conceptualized as a competitive interaction between pools of neurons representing likely future events, with the winner of this competition determining the direction of the subsequent anticipatory movement. Similar mechanisms could drive prediction before movement initiation in other motor domains.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología
20.
J Neurophysiol ; 99(4): 1857-70, 2008 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18216220

RESUMEN

Neural regions in the dorsomedial frontal cortex (DMFC), including the supplementary eye field (SEF) and the presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA), are likely candidates for generating top-down control of saccade target selection. To investigate this, we applied electrical microstimulation to these structures while saccades were being planned to visual targets. Stimulation administered to superficial and lateral DMFC sites that were within or close to the SEF delayed ipsilateral and facilitated contralateral saccades. Facilitation was limited to saccades made toward targets in a narrow, contralateral movement field that had endpoints consistent with the goal of evoked saccades. Facilitation occurred with current delivered before target onset and delay with current applied after this time. Stimulation at deeper, medial sites that encompassed the pre-SMA resulted in mostly bilateral delay. The amount of delay at these sites was usually greater for ipsilateral saccades and increased with current amplitude. Changes in saccade latency were not accompanied by altered endpoint, trajectory, or peak velocity. The spatial specificity of SEF stimulation in inducing latency changes suggests that the SEF participates in selecting saccade goals. The less specific delay with pre-SMA stimulation suggests that it is involved in postponing visually guided saccades, thus likely permitting other oculomotor structures to select saccade goals.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Estimulación Eléctrica , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Macaca , Masculino , Microelectrodos , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/anatomía & histología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA