Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 6 de 6
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30649587

RESUMEN

Fireflies (Coleoptera: Lampyridae) have distinct visual systems at different stages of development. Larvae have stemmata and adults have compound eyes. Adults use compound eyes to mediate photic communication during courtship. Larvae do not manifest this behavior, yet they are bioluminescent. We investigated the structure of stemmata in Photuris firefly larvae to identify anatomical substrates (i.e., rhabdomeres) conferring visual function. Stemmata were located bilaterally on the antero-lateral surfaces of the head. Beneath the ~ 130 µm diameter lens, we identified a pigmented eye-cup. At its widest point, the eye-cup was ~ 150 µm in diameter. The optic nerve exited the eye-cup opposite the lens. Two distinct regions, asymmetric in size and devoid of pigmentation, were characterized in stemmata cross-sections. We refer to these regions as lobes. Each lobe contained a rhabdom of a radial network of rhabdomeres. Pairs of rhabdomeres formed interdigitating microvilli contributed from neighboring photoreceptor cell bodies. The optic nerve contained 88 axons separable into two populations based on size. The number of axons in the optic nerve together with distinct rhabdoms suggests these structures were formed from 'fusion stemmata.' This structural specialization provides an anatomical substrate for future studies of visually mediated behaviors in Photuris larvae.


Asunto(s)
Axones/ultraestructura , Ojo Compuesto de los Artrópodos/ultraestructura , Luciérnagas/ultraestructura , Nervio Óptico/ultraestructura , Células Fotorreceptoras/ultraestructura , Animales , Ojo Compuesto de los Artrópodos/embriología , Luciérnagas/embriología , Larva/ultraestructura , Nervio Óptico/embriología
2.
Science ; 329(5988): 181, 2010 Jul 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20616271

RESUMEN

Most firefly species (Coleoptera: Lampyridae) use bioluminescent flashes for signaling. In some species, the flashing between males occurs rhythmically and repeatedly (synchronically) with millisecond precision. We studied synchrony's behavioral role in the North American firefly, Photinus carolinus. We placed a female in a virtual environment containing artificial males that flashed at varying degrees of synchrony. Females responded to an average of 82% of synchronous flashes compared with as few as 3% of asynchronous flashes. We conclude that one function of flash synchrony is to facilitate a female's ability to recognize her conspecific male's flashing by eliminating potential visual clutter from other flashing males.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación Animal , Luciérnagas/fisiología , Luz , Periodicidad , Animales , Conducta Animal , Femenino , Masculino , Especificidad de la Especie , Visión Ocular/fisiología
3.
Acta Otolaryngol ; 128(4): 382-7, 2008 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18368570

RESUMEN

CONCLUSIONS: We conclude that: (1) among several cues examined, the monaural cue of direct-to-reverberant (D/R) ratio in the ipsilateral ear provides the most information about sound-source distance; (2) interaural level difference (ILD) provides less information about sound-source distance; and (3) a comprehensive theory of three-dimensional auditory localization must incorporate the fact that all of the major acoustic cues change with distance. OBJECTIVE: Neural mechanisms underlying auditory localization of distance are poorly understood. The present study was an initial step toward filling this gap in knowledge. MATERIALS AND METHODS: The binaural room impulse responses of adult barn owls were measured. The sound source was placed at various distances (up to 80 cm) and azimuths (0-90 degrees) relative to the owl's head, with the elevation kept at 0 degrees . RESULTS: We determined the value of each cue for a 3-10 kHz band, and found that: (1) D/R ratio of signal amplitudes provided the most information about sound-source distance; (2) the ipsilateral D/R ratio represented distance more clearly than the contralateral or binaural-average D/R ratios; (3) ILD of direct signals increased with decreasing distance under certain conditions; (3) interaural time difference (ITD) of direct signals increased with decreasing distance at 90 degrees azimuth; and (4) the spectral patterns of ILD and the monaural direct signals changed with distance in complex ways.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Audición/fisiología , Localización de Sonidos/fisiología , Estrigiformes/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Acústica/instrumentación , Animales
4.
J Undergrad Neurosci Educ ; 5(2): A28-34, 2007.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23494074

RESUMEN

The principles of neurophysiology continue to be challenging topics to teach in the context of undergraduate neuroscience education. Laboratory classes containing neurophysiological demonstrations and exercises are, therefore, an important and necessary complement for covering those subjects taught in lecture-based courses. We developed a number of simple yet very instructive exercises, described below, which make use of extracellular recordings from different sensory systems of the cockroach (Periplanta americana). The compendium we developed provides students with hands-on demonstrations of several commonly taught topics of neurophysiology including sensory coding by neural activity.

5.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15164219

RESUMEN

Blue-throated hummingbirds produce elaborate songs extending into the ultrasonic frequency range, up to 30 kHz. Ultrasonic song elements include harmonics and extensions of audible notes, non-harmonic components of audible syllables, and sounds produced at frequencies above 20 kHz without corresponding hearing range sound. To determine whether ultrasonic song elements function in intraspecific communication, we tested the hearing range of male and female blue-throated hummingbirds. We measured auditory thresholds for tone pips ranging from 1 kHz to 50 kHz using auditory brainstem responses. Neither male nor female blue-throated hummingbirds appear to be able to hear above 7 kHz. No auditory brainstem responses could be detected between 8 and 50 kHz at 90 dB. This high-frequency cutoff is well within the range reported for other species of birds. These results suggest that high-frequency song elements are not used in intraspecific communication. We propose that the restricted hummingbird hearing range may exemplify a phylogenetic constraint.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Umbral Auditivo/fisiología , Audición/fisiología , Ultrasonido , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Animales , Audiometría , Percepción Auditiva/efectos de la radiación , Umbral Auditivo/efectos de la radiación , Conducta Animal , Aves , Relación Dosis-Respuesta en la Radiación , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos del Tronco Encefálico/efectos de la radiación , Femenino , Masculino , Sonido , Análisis Espectral , Estadísticas no Paramétricas , Vocalización Animal/efectos de la radiación
6.
Integr Comp Biol ; 44(3): 259-63, 2004 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21676705

RESUMEN

Synchronous flashing occurs in certain species of Southeast Asian and North American fireflies. Most Southeast Asian synchrony involves stationary congregating fireflies, but North American synchrony occurs in flying fireflies that do not congregate. Southeast Asian synchrony is usually continuous, but North American synchrony is interrupted. Photuris frontalis, the only member of the North American genus Photuris to synchronize, shows an intermittent synchrony. This involves synchronization and repeated re-synchronizations while in flight. The precision that occurs at the start of synchrony was studied in Ph. frontalis using caged fireflies and photometry. Barrier experiments (using two fireflies) or flash entrainment experiments (using one LED and one firefly) were performed to measure the temporal precision of the first entrained flash. In both cases, the first entrained flash was close to unison synchrony (phase = 1.0) and showed little variability. The behavioral implications of the ability to synchronize with the first entrained flash are not known, but it might facilitate male-male interactions during brief, transient encounters such as maintaining distance between closely flying males in search of females.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA