Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 7 de 7
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Aerosp Med Hum Perform ; 95(7): 353-366, 2024 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38915160

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Cabin decompression presents a threat in high-altitude-capable aircraft. A chamber study was performed to compare effects of rapid (RD) vs. gradual decompression and gauge impairment at altitude with and without hypoxia, as well as to assess recovery.METHODS: There were 12 participants who completed RD (1 s) and Gradual (3 min 12 s) ascents from 2743-7620 m (9000-25000 ft) altitude pressures while breathing air or 100% O2. Physiological indices included oxygen saturation (SPo2), heart rate (HR), respiration, end tidal O2 and CO2 partial pressures, and electroencephalography (EEG). Cognition was evaluated using SYNWIN, which combines memory, arithmetic, visual, and auditory tasks. The study incorporated ascent rate (RD, gradual), breathing gas (air, 100% O2) and epoch (ground-level, pre-breathe, ascent-altitude, recovery) as factors.RESULTS: Physiological effects in hypoxic "air" ascents included decreased SPo2 and end tidal O2 and CO2 partial pressures (hypocapnia), with elevated HR and minute ventilation (V˙E); SPo2 and HR effects were greater after RD (-7.3% lower and +10.0 bpm higher, respectively). HR and V˙E decreased during recovery. SYNWIN performance declined during ascent in air, with key metrics, including composite score, falling further (-75% vs. -50%) after RD. Broad cognitive impairment was not recorded on 100% O2, nor in recovery. EEG signals showed increased slow-wave activity during hypoxia.DISCUSSION: In hypoxic exposures, RD impaired performance more than gradual ascent. Hypobaria did not comprehensively impair performance without hypoxia. Lingering impairment was not observed during recovery, but HR and V˙E metrics suggested compensatory slowing following altitude stress. Participants' cognitive strategy shifted as hypoxia progressed, with efficiency giving way to "satisficing," redistributing effort to easier tasks.Beer J, Mojica AJ, Blacker KJ, Dart TS, Morse BG, Sherman PM. Relative severity of human performance decrements recorded in rapid vs. gradual decompression. Aerosp Med Hum Perform. 2024; 95(7):353-366.


Asunto(s)
Altitud , Descompresión , Frecuencia Cardíaca , Humanos , Masculino , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Adulto , Descompresión/métodos , Cognición/fisiología , Hipoxia/fisiopatología , Femenino , Electroencefalografía , Saturación de Oxígeno/fisiología , Medicina Aeroespacial , Adulto Joven , Respiración , Dióxido de Carbono/sangre
2.
J Forensic Sci ; 62(6): 1684-1686, 2017 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29152804

Asunto(s)
Algoritmos , Humanos
3.
J Forensic Sci ; 62(6): 1516-1521, 2017 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28419455

RESUMEN

Human Electrical Muscular Incapacitation (HEMI) is used to subdue combative individuals. Changes in cardiac electrical activity have been proposed as the cause of death in a small fraction of these individuals. The current study sought to determine whether changes in QTc interval occur after HEMI exposure. Twenty-four participants had EKG readings before a 5-second HEMI exposure and within 30 min after exposure. All subject EKGs were read by a data-blinded cardiac electrophysiologist who calculated a QT corrected (QTc) interval. QTc interval was calculated using Bazett method. QTc prolongation was defined as >430 ms and a threshold of 30 ms for identifying QTc lengthening. Five participants experienced QTc prolongation and six had QTc lengthening. One participant developed QTc prolongation exceeding 500 ms, which carries a risk of developing multifocal ventricular tachycardia. These results suggest that HEMI exposure may cause EKG changes with a risk of ventricular tachycardia.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Eléctrica/instrumentación , Electrocardiografía , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Taquicardia Ventricular/etiología , Adulto Joven
4.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 79(1): 180-199, 2017 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27797006

RESUMEN

Nelson and Palmer (2007) concluded that figures/figural properties automatically attract attention, after they found that participants were faster to detect/discriminate targets appearing where a portion of a familiar object was suggested in an otherwise ambiguous display. We investigated whether these effects are truly automatic and whether they generalize to another figural property-convexity. We found that Nelson and Palmer's results do generalize to convexity, but only when participants are uncertain regarding when and where the target will appear. Dependence on uncertainty regarding target location/timing was also observed for familiarity. Thus, although we could replicate and extend Nelson and Palmer's results, our experiments showed that figures do not automatically draw attention. In addition, our research went beyond Nelson and Palmer's, in that we were able to separate figural properties from perceived figures. Because figural properties are regularities that predict where objects lie in the visual field, our results join other evidence that regularities in the environment can attract attention. More generally, our results are consistent with Bayesian theories in which priors are given more weight under conditions of uncertainty.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Incertidumbre , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
5.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 76(8): 2531-47, 2014 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25091977

RESUMEN

Traditional theories of vision assume that figures and grounds are assigned early in processing, with semantics being accessed later and only by figures, not by grounds. We tested this assumption by showing observers novel silhouettes with borders that suggested familiar objects on their ground side. The ground appeared shapeless near the figure's borders; the familiar objects suggested there were not consciously perceived. Participants' task was to categorize words shown immediately after the silhouettes as naming natural versus artificial objects. The words named objects from the same or from a different superordinate category as the familiar objects suggested in the silhouette ground. In Experiment 1, participants categorized words faster when they followed silhouettes suggesting upright familiar objects from the same rather than a different category on their ground sides, whereas no category differences were observed for inverted silhouettes. This is the first study to show unequivocally that, contrary to traditional assumptions, semantics are accessed for objects that might be perceived on the side of a border that will ultimately be perceived as a shapeless ground. Moreover, although the competition for figural status results in suppression of the shape of the losing contender, its semantics are not suppressed. In Experiment 2, we used longer silhouette-to-word stimulus onset asynchronies to test whether semantics would be suppressed later in time, as might occur if semantics were accessed later than shape memories. No evidence of semantic suppression was observed; indeed, semantic activation of the objects suggested on the ground side of a border appeared to be short-lived. Implications for feedforward versus dynamical interactive theories of object perception are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Concienciación/fisiología , Formación de Concepto/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Semántica , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
6.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 76(4): 1069-84, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24627210

RESUMEN

Past research has demonstrated that convex regions are increasingly likely to be perceived as figures as the number of alternating convex and concave regions in test displays increases. This region-number effect depends on both a small preexisting preference for convex over concave objects and the presence of scene characteristics (i.e., uniform fill) that allow the integration of the concave regions into a background object/surface. These factors work together to enable the percept of convex objects in front of a background. We investigated whether region-number effects generalize to another property, symmetry, whose effectiveness as a figure property has been debated. Observers reported which regions they perceived as figures in black-and-white displays with alternating symmetric/asymmetric regions. In Experiments 1 and 2, the displays had articulated outer borders that preserved the symmetry/asymmetry of the outermost regions. Region-number effects were not observed, although symmetric regions were perceived as figures more often than chance. We hypothesized that the articulated outer borders prevented fitting a background interpretation to the asymmetric regions. In Experiment 3, we used straight-edge framelike outer borders and observed region-number effects for symmetry equivalent to those observed for convexity. These results (1) show that display-wide information affects figure assignment at a border, (2) extend the evidence indicating that the ability to fit background as well as foreground interpretations is critical in figure assignment, (3) reveal that symmetry and convexity are equally effective figure cues and, (4) demonstrate that symmetry serves as a figural property only when it is close to fixation.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Presentación de Datos , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Campos Visuales/fisiología
7.
Psychophysiology ; 47(5): 984-8, 2010 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20233342

RESUMEN

This experiment explored whether a late positive potential (LPP) of the event-related brain potential is useful for examining attitudes that people attempt to conceal. Participants identified a set of liked, neutral, and disliked people and viewed sequences consisting of either names or pictures of these people. Disliked people appeared rarely among liked people, and participants either: (1) always accurately reported their negative attitudes toward the people; (2) misreported negative attitudes as positive when they saw a picture of a disliked person; or (3) misreported negative attitudes as positive when they saw a name of a disliked person. Rare negative stimuli evoked a larger-amplitude LPP than frequent positive stimuli. Misreporting attitudes significantly reduced the amplitude difference between rare negative and frequent positive stimuli, though it remained significant.


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Decepción , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Electroencefalografía , Electrooculografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Medio Social , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...