Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 29
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 30(12): 1883-1901, 2018 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30125221

RESUMEN

The relationship between neural activation during movement training and the plastic changes that survive beyond movement execution is not well understood. Here we ask whether the changes in resting-state functional connectivity observed following motor learning overlap with the brain networks that track movement error during training. Human participants learned to trace an arched trajectory using a computer mouse in an MRI scanner. Motor performance was quantified on each trial as the maximum distance from the prescribed arc. During learning, two brain networks were observed, one showing increased activations for larger movement error, comprising the cerebellum, parietal, visual, somatosensory, and cortical motor areas, and the other being more activated for movements with lower error, comprising the ventral putamen and the OFC. After learning, changes in brain connectivity at rest were found predominantly in areas that had shown increased activation for larger error during task, specifically the cerebellum and its connections with motor, visual, and somatosensory cortex. The findings indicate that, although both errors and accurate movements are important during the active stage of motor learning, the changes in brain activity observed at rest primarily reflect networks that process errors. This suggests that error-related networks are represented in the initial stages of motor memory formation.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Adulto , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Vías Nerviosas/diagnóstico por imagen , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Descanso , Adulto Joven
2.
Ann N Y Acad Sci ; 2018 May 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29732575

RESUMEN

We investigated whether dancing influences the emotional response to music, compared to when music is listened to in the absence of movement. Forty participants without previous dance training listened to "groovy" and "nongroovy" music excerpts while either dancing or refraining from movement. Participants were also tested while imitating their own dance movements, but in the absence of music as a control condition. Emotion ratings and ratings of flow were collected following each condition. Dance movements were recorded using motion capture. We found that the state of flow was increased specifically during spontaneous dance to groovy excerpts, compared with both still listening and motor imitation. Emotions in the realms of vitality (such as joy and power) and sublimity (such as wonder and nostalgia) were evoked by music in general, whether participants moved or not. Significant correlations were found between the emotional and flow responses to music and whole-body acceleration profiles. Thus, the results highlight a distinct state of flow when dancing, which may be of use to promote well-being and to address certain clinical conditions.

3.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 11: 572, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29238298

RESUMEN

Dancing emphasizes the motor expression of emotional experiences. The bodily expression of emotions can modulate the subjective experience of emotions, as when adopting emotion-specific postures and faces. Thus, dancing potentially offers a ground for emotional coping through emotional enhancement and regulation. Here we investigated the emotional responses to music in individuals without any prior dance training while they either freely danced or refrained from movement. Participants were also tested while imitating their own dance movements but in the absence of music as a control condition. Emotional ratings and cardio-respiratory measures were collected following each condition. Dance movements were recorded using motion capture. We found that emotional valence was increased specifically during spontaneous dance of groovy excerpts, compared to both still listening and motor imitation. Furthermore, parasympathetic-related heart rate variability (HRV) increased during dance compared to motor imitation. Nevertheless, subjective and physiological arousal increased during movement production, regardless of whether participants were dancing or imitating. Significant correlations were found between inter-individual differences in the emotions experienced during dance and whole-body acceleration profiles. The combination of movement and music during dance results in a distinct state characterized by acutely heightened pleasure, which is of potential interest for the use of dance in therapeutic settings.

4.
Front Physiol ; 8: 785, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29089898

RESUMEN

In light of theories postulating a role for music in forming emotional and social bonds, here we investigated whether endogenous rhythms synchronize between multiple individuals when listening to music. Cardiovascular and respiratory recordings were taken from multiple individuals (musically trained or music-naïve) simultaneously, at rest and during a live concert comprising music excerpts with varying degrees of complexity of the acoustic envelope. Inter-individual synchronization of cardiorespiratory rhythms showed a subtle but reliable increase during passively listening to music compared to baseline. The low-level auditory features of the music were largely responsible for creating or disrupting such synchronism, explaining ~80% of its variance, over and beyond subjective musical preferences and previous musical training. Listening to simple rhythms and melodies, which largely dominate the choice of music during rituals and mass events, brings individuals together in terms of their physiological rhythms, which could explain why music is widely used to favor social bonds.

5.
Psychophysiology ; 54(12): 1951-1966, 2017 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28840941

RESUMEN

The effects of meditation on arterial and tissue oxygenation are unknown and difficult to assess because respiration is often altered, directly or indirectly, during meditation practice. Thus, changes in respiration may affect cardiovascular responses independently from meditation. In this study, we aim to isolate the specific effect of meditation on arterial and tissue oxygenation and other cardiorespiratory indexes while systematically controlling for the role of respiration. Furthermore, we aim to clarify to what extent prior expertise in meditation practice is needed to observe reliable changes. Eighty participants, half with and half without prior meditation experience, were tested while pacing breathing at predetermined rates, in the presence or absence of mantra meditation instructions, and in a body scan meditation that did not involve controlled breathing. Continuous recordings were acquired for arterial and brain oxygenation, respiratory excursion, electrocardiogram, skin vasomotion, and blood pressure. In both groups, meditation acutely decreased arterial and cerebral oxygen saturation, reduced chemoreflex sensitivity, and prolonged the RR interval, independently of respiration. Conversely, slow breathing improved heart rate variability, independently of concurrent meditation. In addition to the immediate effects of meditation, the individuals with long-term practice of meditation had overall higher arterial and cerebral oxygen saturation, overall lower blood pressure, and slower baseline respiration. Meditation acutely lowers arterial and tissue oxygenation. A repeated exposure to this condition may lead to long-term adaptation and, through increased ventilatory efficiency and improved gas exchanges, to an increase in baseline oxygenation. Meditation induces favorable changes in cardiovascular and respiratory end points of clinical interest.


Asunto(s)
Meditación , Oxígeno/metabolismo , Respiración , Adulto , Presión Sanguínea , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Electrocardiografía , Femenino , Frecuencia Cardíaca , Humanos , Masculino , Fenómenos Fisiológicos de la Piel , Adulto Joven
6.
J Neurosci ; 36(46): 11682-11692, 2016 11 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27852776

RESUMEN

As one learns to dance or play tennis, the desired somatosensory state is typically unknown. Trial and error is important as motor behavior is shaped by successful and unsuccessful movements. As an experimental model, we designed a task in which human participants make reaching movements to a hidden target and receive positive reinforcement when successful. We identified somatic and reinforcement-based sources of plasticity on the basis of changes in functional connectivity using resting-state fMRI before and after learning. The neuroimaging data revealed reinforcement-related changes in both motor and somatosensory brain areas in which a strengthening of connectivity was related to the amount of positive reinforcement during learning. Areas of prefrontal cortex were similarly altered in relation to reinforcement, with connectivity between sensorimotor areas of putamen and the reward-related ventromedial prefrontal cortex strengthened in relation to the amount of successful feedback received. In other analyses, we assessed connectivity related to changes in movement direction between trials, a type of variability that presumably reflects exploratory strategies during learning. We found that connectivity in a network linking motor and somatosensory cortices increased with trial-to-trial changes in direction. Connectivity varied as well with the change in movement direction following incorrect movements. Here the changes were observed in a somatic memory and decision making network involving ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and second somatosensory cortex. Our results point to the idea that the initial stages of motor learning are not wholly motor but rather involve plasticity in somatic and prefrontal networks related both to reward and exploration. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: In the initial stages of motor learning, the placement of the limbs is learned primarily through trial and error. In an experimental analog, participants make reaching movements to a hidden target and receive positive feedback when successful. We identified sources of plasticity based on changes in functional connectivity using resting-state fMRI. The main finding is that there is a strengthening of connectivity between reward-related prefrontal areas and sensorimotor areas in the basal ganglia and frontal cortex. There is also a strengthening of connectivity related to movement exploration in sensorimotor circuits involved in somatic memory and decision making. The results indicate that initial stages of motor learning depend on plasticity in somatic and prefrontal networks related to reward and exploration.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Motora/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Refuerzo en Psicología , Corteza Somatosensorial/fisiología , Adulto , Conectoma/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología
8.
J Neurosci ; 35(42): 14316-26, 2015 Oct 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26490869

RESUMEN

The early stages of motor skill acquisition are often marked by uncertainty about the sensory and motor goals of the task, as is the case in learning to speak or learning the feel of a good tennis serve. Here we present an experimental model of this early learning process, in which targets are acquired by exploration and reinforcement rather than sensory error. We use this model to investigate the relative contribution of motor and sensory factors to human motor learning. Participants make active reaching movements or matched passive movements to an unseen target using a robot arm. We find that learning through passive movements paired with reinforcement is comparable with learning associated with active movement, both in terms of magnitude and durability, with improvements due to training still observable at a 1 week retest. Motor learning is also accompanied by changes in somatosensory perceptual acuity. No stable changes in motor performance are observed for participants that train, actively or passively, in the absence of reinforcement, or for participants who are given explicit information about target position in the absence of somatosensory experience. These findings indicate that the somatosensory system dominates learning in the early stages of motor skill acquisition. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: The research focuses on the initial stages of human motor learning, introducing a new experimental model that closely approximates the key features of motor learning outside of the laboratory. The finding indicates that it is the somatosensory system rather than the motor system that dominates learning in the early stages of motor skill acquisition. This is important given that most of our computational models of motor learning are based on the idea that learning is motoric in origin. This is also a valuable finding for rehabilitation of patients with limited mobility as it shows that reinforcement in conjunction with passive movement results in benefits to motor learning that are as great as those observed for active movement training.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Rango del Movimiento Articular/fisiología , Corteza Somatosensorial/fisiología , Adolescente , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Refuerzo en Psicología , Adulto Joven
9.
PLoS One ; 9(9): e107538, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25216280

RESUMEN

We tested whether pre-assigned arm movements performed in a group setting spontaneously synchronized and whether synchronization extended to heart and respiratory rhythms. We monitored arm movements, respiration and electrocardiogram at rest and during spontaneous, music and metronome-associated arm-swinging. No directions were given on whether or how the arm swinging were to be synchronized between participants or with the external cues. Synchronization within 3 groups of 10 participants studied collectively was compared with pseudo-synchronization of 3 groups of 10 participants that underwent an identical protocol but in an individual setting. Motor synchronization was found to be higher in the collective groups than in the individuals for the metronome-associated condition. On a repetition of the protocol on the following day, motor synchronization in the collective groups extended to the spontaneous, un-cued condition. Breathing was also more synchronized in the collective groups than in the individuals, particularly at rest and in the music-associated condition. Group synchronization occurs without explicit instructions, and involves both movements and respiratory control rhythms.


Asunto(s)
Brazo/fisiología , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Respiración , Adulto , Electrocardiografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Música
10.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 8: 413, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24966827

RESUMEN

Unilateral visuo-spatial neglect is a neuropsychological syndrome commonly resulting from right hemisphere stroke at the temporo-parietal junction of the infero-posterior parietal cortex. Neglect is characterized by reduced awareness of stimuli presented on patients' contralesional side of space. Inspired by evidence of increased spatial exploration of patients' left side achieved during keyboard scale-playing, the current study employed a music intervention that involved making sequential goal-directed actions in the neglected part of space, in order to determine whether this would bring about clinically significant improvement in chronic neglect. Two left neglect patients completed an intervention comprising four weekly 30-min music intervention sessions involving playing scales and familiar melodies on chime bars from right to left. Two cancellation tests [Mesulam shape, Behavioral Inattention Test (BIT) star], the neglect subtest from the computerized TAP (Test of Attentional Performance) battery, and the line bisection test were administered three times during a preliminary baseline phase, before and after the four intervention sessions during the intervention phase to investigate short-term effects, and 1 week after the last intervention session to investigate whether any changes in performance would persist. Both patients demonstrated significant short-term and longer-lasting improvements on the Mesulam shape cancellation test. One patient also showed longer-lasting effects on the BIT star cancellation test and scored in the normal range 1 week after the intervention. These findings provide preliminary evidence that active music-making with a horizontally aligned instrument may help neglect patients attend more to their affected side.

11.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 7: 451, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23970859

RESUMEN

Mental practice (MP) has been shown to improve movement accuracy and velocity, but it is not known whether MP can also optimize movement timing. We addressed this question by studying two groups of expert pianists who performed challenging music sequences after either MP or physical practice (PP). Performance and motion-capture data were collected along with responses to imagery questionnaires. The results showed that MP produced performance improvements, although to a lower degree than PP did. MP and PP induced changes in both movement velocity and movement timing, promoting the emergence of movement anticipatory patterns. Furthermore, motor imagery was associated with greater changes in movement velocity, while auditory imagery was associated with greater movement anticipation. Data from a control group that was not allowed to practice confirmed that the changes in accuracy and kinematics were not due to mere repetition of the sequence during testing. This study provides the first evidence of an anticipatory control following MP and extends the present knowledge on the effectiveness of MP to a task of unparalleled motor complexity. The practical implications of MP in the motor domain are discussed.

12.
J Neurophysiol ; 110(8): 1804-10, 2013 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23864382

RESUMEN

Observing the actions of others has been shown to affect motor learning, but does it have effects on sensory systems as well? It has been recently shown that motor learning that involves actual physical practice is also associated with plasticity in the somatosensory system. Here, we assessed the idea that observational learning likewise changes somatosensory function. We evaluated changes in somatosensory function after human subjects watched videos depicting motor learning. Subjects first observed video recordings of reaching movements either in a clockwise or counterclockwise force field. They were then trained in an actual force-field task that involved a counterclockwise load. Measures of somatosensory function were obtained before and after visual observation and also following force-field learning. Consistent with previous reports, video observation promoted motor learning. We also found that somatosensory function was altered following observational learning, both in direction and in magnitude, in a manner similar to that which occurs when motor learning is achieved through actual physical practice. Observation of the same sequence of movements in a randomized order did not result in somatosensory perceptual change. Observational learning and real physical practice appear to tap into the same capacity for sensory change in that subjects that showed a greater change following observational learning showed a reliably smaller change following physical motor learning. We conclude that effects of observing motor learning extend beyond the boundaries of traditional motor circuits, to include somatosensory representations.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepción Visual , Adulto Joven
13.
Ann N Y Acad Sci ; 1169: 417-21, 2009 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19673816

RESUMEN

Although positive effects of rhythm cueing on motor control in neurologic disorders are known, no studies have yet focused on patients suffering from impaired programming of complex actions. One patient suffering from ideomotor apraxia (a potentially ideal experimental paradigm to test the effect of rhythm on high-level motor control) underwent two rehabilitation training sets differing only for the presence or absence of rhythm cueing. Both sets of training increased the patient's proficiency, but rhythm cueing was significantly more effective, during the training as well as during the post-training uncued test. Ideomotor apraxia represents an effective model to test the effects of rhythm on high-level motor control.


Asunto(s)
Apraxia Ideomotora/terapia , Musicoterapia , Adulto , Apraxia Ideomotora/psicología , Apraxia Ideomotora/rehabilitación , Cognición , Femenino , Humanos
14.
Circulation ; 119(25): 3171-80, 2009 Jun 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19569263

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Reactions to music are considered subjective, but previous studies suggested that cardiorespiratory variables increase with faster tempo independent of individual preference. We tested whether compositions characterized by variable emphasis could produce parallel instantaneous cardiovascular/respiratory responses and whether these changes mirrored music profiles. METHODS AND RESULTS: Twenty-four young healthy subjects, 12 musicians (choristers) and 12 nonmusician control subjects, listened (in random order) to music with vocal (Puccini's "Turandot") or orchestral (Beethoven's 9th Symphony adagio) progressive crescendos, more uniform emphasis (Bach cantata), 10-second period (ie, similar to Mayer waves) rhythmic phrases (Giuseppe Verdi's arias "Va pensiero" and "Libiam nei lieti calici"), or silence while heart rate, respiration, blood pressures, middle cerebral artery flow velocity, and skin vasomotion were recorded.Common responses were recognized by averaging instantaneous cardiorespiratory responses regressed against changes in music profiles and by coherence analysis during rhythmic phrases. Vocal and orchestral crescendos produced significant (P=0.05 or better) correlations between cardiovascular or respiratory signals and music profile, particularly skin vasoconstriction and blood pressures, proportional to crescendo, in contrast to uniform emphasis, which induced skin vasodilation and reduction in blood pressures. Correlations were significant both in individual and group-averaged signals. Phrases at 10-second periods by Verdi entrained the cardiovascular autonomic variables. No qualitative differences in recorded measurements were seen between musicians and nonmusicians. CONCLUSIONS: Music emphasis and rhythmic phrases are tracked consistently by physiological variables. Autonomic responses are synchronized with music, which might therefore convey emotions through autonomic arousal during crescendos or rhythmic phrases.


Asunto(s)
Fenómenos Fisiológicos Cardiovasculares , Circulación Cerebrovascular/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Música , Psicoacústica , Adulto , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Velocidad del Flujo Sanguíneo/fisiología , Presión Sanguínea/fisiología , Femenino , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Arteria Cerebral Media/diagnóstico por imagen , Arteria Cerebral Media/fisiología , Mecánica Respiratoria/fisiología , Piel/irrigación sanguínea , Ultrasonografía , Vasodilatación/fisiología , Adulto Joven
15.
Bioorg Med Chem ; 10(4): 1077-84, 2002 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11836118

RESUMEN

Several (Z)-5-arylidene-2,4-thiazolidinediones were synthesized and tested as aldose reductase inhibitors (ARIs). The most active of the N-unsubstituted derivatives (2) exerted the same inhibitory activity of Sorbinil. The introduction of an acetic side chain on N-3 of the thiazolidinedione moiety led to a marked increase in lending inhibitory activity, conducting to the discovery of a very potent ARI (4c), whose activity level (IC50=0.13 microM) was in the same range of Tolrestat. Moreover, the corresponding methyl esters (3), devoid of any acidic functionality, showed appreciable inhibitory activity similar to that of the N-unsubstituted compounds. It was also found that the substitution pattern on the 5-benzylidene moiety markedly influenced the activity of N-unsubstituted 2,4-thiazolidinediones 2, compounds with substituents at the meta position being generally more effective than the para-substituted ones; however, this SAR was not evidenced in acetates 3 and acids 4.


Asunto(s)
Aldehído Reductasa/antagonistas & inhibidores , Inhibidores Enzimáticos/síntesis química , Imidazolidinas , Tiazoles/farmacología , Tiazolidinedionas , Animales , Bovinos , Inhibidores Enzimáticos/farmacología , Hipoglucemiantes/síntesis química , Hipoglucemiantes/farmacología , Imidazoles/química , Imidazoles/farmacología , Concentración 50 Inhibidora , Espectroscopía de Resonancia Magnética , NADP/metabolismo , Naftalenos/química , Naftalenos/farmacología , Relación Estructura-Actividad , Tiazoles/síntesis química
16.
Acta Crystallogr C ; 57(Pt 10): 1225-7, 2001 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11600794

RESUMEN

The title compound, C(17)H(16)N(2)O(3), is an antagonist for AMPA/kainate receptors. The molecule has its seven-membered oxadiazole ring in a boat conformation. Asymmetry of the two methoxy bond angles is evident, with (Me)O-C-C angles of 115.45 (12) and 124.78 (13) degrees, and 114.67 (12) and 125.31 (12) degrees. A centrosymmetric dimer involving the HN-CO moieties, with an N...O distance of 2.876 (2) A, graph set R(2)(2)(8), is further linked into chains through methoxy Csp(3)-H...N hydrogen bonds, with a C...N distance of 3.418 (2) A.


Asunto(s)
Azepinas/química , Antagonistas de Aminoácidos Excitadores/química , Receptores AMPA/antagonistas & inhibidores , Cristalografía por Rayos X , Enlace de Hidrógeno , Modelos Moleculares
17.
Bioorg Med Chem ; 9(8): 2203-11, 2001 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11504658

RESUMEN

The reaction of 2,6-diacetylpyridine (dap) and isonicotinoyl- or benzoylhydrazide leads to bishydrazones H(2)dapin (1a) and H(2)dapb (1b), respectively. The condensation can either take place as a bimolecular kinetic process between the two reactants or as a monomolecular metal-templated synthesis in the presence of nickel(II) ions. In the latter case the reaction products are charged 2,6-diacetylpyridine bis(hydrazone) nickel(II) complexes, which can be easily deprotonated to neutral hydrazonates. Diffractometric analysis of one of these [Ni(dapb)](2) (8b) has shown a binuclear structure with two octahedral nickel(II) ions bridged by two helicoidal dap (bishydrazonates) in a spheroidal structure of C(2V) symmetry. The synthesized complexes 8 are promising as antimycobacterial agents against M. tuberculosis H37Rv. In particular, 8b displays significant activity (MIC=0.025 microg/mL) 10-fold higher than rifampin and equal to isoniazid, while its ligand is ineffective. Compound 8b is also capable of reducing HIV-induced cytopathogenic effect in human T(4 )lymphocytes.


Asunto(s)
Antituberculosos/química , Hidrazinas/farmacología , Compuestos Organometálicos/farmacología , Antituberculosos/síntesis química , Antituberculosos/farmacología , Estudios de Evaluación como Asunto , Hidrazinas/química , Pruebas de Sensibilidad Microbiana , Mycobacterium tuberculosis/efectos de los fármacos , Compuestos Organometálicos/química
18.
Acta Crystallogr C ; 57(Pt 5): 572-4, 2001 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11353255

RESUMEN

The title molecules, C(15)H(9)ClF(2)N(2)S and C(16)H(12)F(2)N(2)S, respectively, display the well known butterfly-like conformation with a flat thiazolobenzimidazole system. In both compounds, the mean plane through the tricyclic system is almost perpendicular to the 2,6-difluorophenyl ring. This arrangement of the aryl group is determined by two intramolecular hydrogen bonds and by an attractive F.S interaction.


Asunto(s)
Fármacos Anti-VIH/química , Bencimidazoles/química , Tiazoles/química , Fármacos Anti-VIH/farmacología , Bencimidazoles/farmacología , Cristalografía por Rayos X , Conformación Molecular , Estructura Molecular , Relación Estructura-Actividad , Tiazoles/farmacología
19.
Acta Crystallogr C ; 57(Pt 4): 493-4, 2001 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11313606

RESUMEN

The crystal structure of the title compound, C(17)H(10)O(3), is the first example of a furocoumarin containing three fused rings. The tricyclic furocoumarin fragment is perfectly planar. The phenyl substituent forms a dihedral angle of 39.52 (8) degrees with the plane of the tricyclic system. The crystal packing involves centrosymmetric dimers interconnected by strong pi-interactions between their furo[3,2-c]coumarin fragments [at distances of 3.42 (4) A].

20.
Acta Crystallogr C ; 56 (Pt 2): 254-5, 2000 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10777908
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...