Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 151
Filtrar
1.
J Eval Clin Pract ; 2024 May 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38818694

RESUMEN

AIMS AND OBJECTIVES: Contextual information which is implicitly available to physicians during clinical encounters has been shown to influence diagnostic reasoning. To better understand the psychological mechanisms underlying the influence of context on diagnostic accuracy, we conducted a review of experimental research on this topic. METHOD: We searched Web of Science, PubMed, and Scopus for relevant articles and looked for additional records by reading the references and approaching experts. We limited the review to true experiments involving physicians in which the outcome variable was the accuracy of the diagnosis. RESULTS: The 43 studies reviewed examined two categories of contextual variables: (a) case-intrinsic contextual information and (b) case-extrinsic contextual information. Case-intrinsic information includes implicit misleading diagnostic suggestions in the disease history of the patient, or emotional volatility of the patient. Case-extrinsic or situational information includes a similar (but different) case seen previously, perceived case difficulty, or external digital diagnostic support. Time pressure and interruptions are other extrinsic influences that may affect the accuracy of a diagnosis but have produced conflicting findings. CONCLUSION: We propose two tentative hypotheses explaining the role of context in diagnostic accuracy. According to the negative-affect hypothesis, diagnostic errors emerge when the physician's attention shifts from the relevant clinical findings to the (irrelevant) source of negative affect (for instance patient aggression) raised in a clinical encounter. The early-diagnosis-primacy hypothesis attributes errors to the extraordinary influence of the initial hypothesis that comes to the physician's mind on the subsequent collecting and interpretation of case information. Future research should test these mechanisms explicitly. Possible alternative mechanisms such as premature closure or increased production of (irrelevant) rival diagnoses in response to context deserve further scrutiny. Implications for medical education and practice are discussed.

2.
BMJ Qual Saf ; 2024 Mar 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38503488

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The consultation process, where a clinician seeks an opinion from another clinician, is foundational in medicine. However, the effectiveness of group diagnosis has not been studied. OBJECTIVE: To compare individual diagnosis to group diagnosis on two dimensions: group size (n=3 or 6) and group process (interactive or artificial groups). METHODOLOGY: Thirty-six internal or emergency medicine residents participated in the study. Initially, each resident worked through four written cases on their own, providing a primary diagnosis and a differential diagnosis. Next, participants formed into groups of three. Using a videoconferencing platform, they worked through four additional cases, collectively providing a single primary diagnosis and differential diagnosis. The process was repeated using a group of six with four new cases. Cases were all counterbalanced. Retrospectively, nominal (ie, artificial) groups were formed by aggregating individual participant data into subgroups of three and six and analytically computing scores. Presence of the correct diagnosis as primary diagnosis or included in the differential diagnosis, as well as the number of diagnoses mentioned, was calculated for all conditions. Means were compared using analysis of variance. RESULTS: For both authentic and nominal groups, the diagnostic accuracy of group diagnosis was superior to individual for both the primary diagnosis and differential diagnosis. However, there was no improvement in diagnostic accuracy when comparing a group of three to a group of six. Interactive and nominal groups were equivalent; however, this may be an artefact of the method used to combine data. CONCLUSIONS: Group diagnosis improves diagnostic accuracy. However, a larger group is not necessarily superior to a smaller group. In this study, interactive group discussion does not result in improved diagnostic accuracy.

3.
Med Educ ; 57(10): 932-938, 2023 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36860135

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Newer electronic differential diagnosis supports (EDSs) are efficient and effective at improving diagnostic skill. Although these supports are encouraged in practice, they are prohibited in medical licensing examinations. The purpose of this study is to determine how using an EDS impacts examinees' results when answering clinical diagnosis questions. METHOD: The authors recruited 100 medical students from McMaster University (Hamilton, Ontario) to answer 40 clinical diagnosis questions in a simulated examination in 2021. Of these, 50 were first-year students and 50 were final-year students. Participants from each year of study were randomised into one of two groups. During the survey, half of the students had access to Isabel (an EDS) and half did not. Differences were explored using analysis of variance (ANOVA), and reliability estimates were compared for each group. RESULTS: Test scores were higher for final-year versus first-year students (53 ± 13% versus 29 ± 10, p < 0.001) and higher with the use of EDS (44 ± 28% versus 36 ± 26%, p < 0.001). Students using the EDS took longer to complete the test (p < 0.001). Internal consistency reliability (Cronbach's alpha) increased with EDS use among final-year students but was reduced among first-year students, although the effect was not significant. A similar pattern was noted in item discrimination, which was significant. CONCLUSION: EDS use during diagnostic licensing style questions was associated with modest improvements in performance, increased discrimination in senior students and increased testing time. Given that clinicians have access to EDS in routine clinical practice, allowing EDS use for diagnostic questions would maintain ecological validity of testing while preserving important psychometric test characteristics.


Asunto(s)
Estudiantes de Medicina , Humanos , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Concesión de Licencias , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Evaluación Educacional/métodos
4.
Qual Life Res ; 32(5): 1239-1246, 2023 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36396874

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Anchor-based methods are group-level approaches used to derive clinical outcome assessment (COA) interpretation thresholds of meaningful within-patient change over time for understanding impacts of disease and treatment. The methods explore the associations between change in the targeted concept of the COA measure and the concept measured by the external anchor(s), typically a global rating, chosen as easier to interpret than the COA measure. While they are valued for providing plausible interpretation thresholds, group-level anchor-based methods pose a number of inherent theoretical and methodological conundrums for interpreting individual-level change. METHODS: This investigation provides a critical appraisal of anchor-based methods for COA interpretation thresholds and details key biases in anchor-based methods that directly influences the magnitude of the interpretation threshold. RESULTS: Five important research issues inherent with the use of anchor-based methods deserve attention: (1) global estimates of change are consistently biased toward the present state; (2) the use of static current state global measures, while not subject to artifacts of recall, may exacerbate the problem of estimating clinically meaningful change; (3) the specific anchor assessment response(s) that identify the meaningful change group usually involves an arbitrary judgment; (4) the calculated interpretation thresholds are sensitive to the proportion of patients who have improved; and (5) examination of anchor-based regression methods reveals that the correlation between the COA change scores and the anchor has a direct linear relationship to the magnitude of the interpretation threshold derived using an anchor-based approach; stronger correlations yielding larger interpretation thresholds. CONCLUSIONS: While anchor-based methods are recognized for their utility in deriving interpretation thresholds for COAs, attention to the biases associated with estimation of the threshold using these methods is needed to progress in the development of standard-setting methodologies for COAs.


Asunto(s)
Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud , Calidad de Vida , Humanos , Calidad de Vida/psicología , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud/métodos
5.
Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract ; 28(1): 47-63, 2023 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35943606

RESUMEN

Students are often encouraged to learn 'deeply' by abstracting generalizable principles from course content rather than memorizing details. So widespread is this perspective that Likert-style inventories are now routinely administered to students to quantify how much a given course or curriculum evokes deep learning. The predictive validity of these inventories, however, has been criticized based on sparse empirical support and ambiguity in what specific outcome measures indicate whether deep learning has occurred. Here we further tested the predictive validity of a prevalent deep learning inventory, the Revised Two-Factor Study Process Questionnaire, by selectively analyzing outcome measures that reflect a major goal of medical education-i.e., knowledge transfer. Students from two undergraduate health sciences courses completed the deep learning inventory before their course's final exam. Shortly after, a random subset of students rated how much each final exam item aligned with three task demands associated with transfer: (1) application of general principles, (2) integration of multiple ideas or examples, and (3) contextual novelty. We then used these ratings from students to examine performance on a subset of exam items that were collectively perceived to demand transfer. Despite good reliability, the resulting transfer outcomes were not substantively predicted by the deep learning inventory. These findings challenge the validity of this tool and others like it.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje Profundo , Educación Médica , Humanos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Curriculum , Estudiantes
6.
JMIR Hum Factors ; 9(3): e39234, 2022 Sep 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36178728

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Not thinking of a diagnosis is a leading cause of diagnostic error in the emergency department, resulting in delayed treatment, morbidity, and excess mortality. Electronic differential diagnostic support (EDS) results in small but significant reductions in diagnostic error. However, the uptake of EDS by clinicians is limited. OBJECTIVE: We sought to understand physician perceptions and barriers to the uptake of EDS within the emergency department triage process. METHODS: We conducted a qualitative study using a research associate to rapidly prototype an embedded EDS into the emergency department triage process. Physicians involved in the triage assessment of a busy emergency department were provided the output of an EDS based on the triage complaint by an embedded researcher to simulate an automated system that would draw from the electronic medical record. Physicians were interviewed immediately after their experience. Verbatim transcripts were analyzed by a team using open and axial coding, informed by direct content analysis. RESULTS: In all, 4 themes emerged from 14 interviews: (1) the quality of the EDS was inferred from the scope and prioritization of the diagnoses present in the EDS differential; (2) the trust of the EDS was linked to varied beliefs around the diagnostic process and potential for bias; (3) clinicians foresaw more benefit to EDS use for colleagues and trainees rather than themselves; and (4) clinicians felt strongly that EDS output should not be included in the patient record. CONCLUSIONS: The adoption of an EDS into an emergency department triage process will require a system that provides diagnostic suggestions appropriate for the scope and context of the emergency department triage process, transparency of system design, and affordances for clinician beliefs about the diagnostic process and addresses clinician concern around including EDS output in the patient record.

7.
Acad Med ; 97(8): 1213-1218, 2022 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35507461

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Postgraduate medical education in Canada has quickly transformed to a competency-based model featuring new entrustable professional activities (EPAs) and associated milestones. It remains unclear, however, how these milestones are distributed between the central medical expert role and 6 intrinsic roles of the larger CanMEDS competency framework. A document review was thus conducted to measure how many EPA milestones are classified under each CanMEDS role, focusing on the overall balance between representation of intrinsic roles and that of medical expert. METHOD: Data were extracted from the EPA guides of 40 Canadian specialties in 2021 to measure the percentage of milestones formally linked to each role. Subsequent analyses explored for differences when milestones were separated by stage of postgraduate training, weighted by an EPA's minimum number of observations, or sorted by surgical and medical specialties. RESULTS: Approximately half of all EPA milestones (mean = 48.6%; 95% confidence interval [CI] = 45.9, 51.3) were classified under intrinsic roles overall. However, representation of the health advocate role was consistently low (mean = 2.95%; 95% CI = 2.49, 3.41), and some intrinsic roles-mainly leader, scholar, and professional-were more heavily concentrated in the final stage of postgraduate training. These findings held true under all conditions examined. CONCLUSIONS: The observed distribution of roles in EPA milestones fits with high-level descriptions of CanMEDS in that intrinsic roles are viewed as inextricably linked to medical expertise, implying both are equally important to cultivate through curricula. Yet a fine-grained analysis suggests that a low prevalence or late emphasis of some intrinsic roles may hinder how they are taught or assessed. Future work must explore whether the quantity or timing of milestones shapes the perceived value of each role, and other factors determining the optimal distribution of roles throughout training.


Asunto(s)
Educación Médica , Internado y Residencia , Medicina , Canadá , Competencia Clínica , Educación Basada en Competencias , Curriculum , Humanos
9.
BMJ ; 376: e064389, 2022 01 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34987062

RESUMEN

Research in cognitive psychology shows that expert clinicians make a medical diagnosis through a two step process of hypothesis generation and hypothesis testing. Experts generate a list of possible diagnoses quickly and intuitively, drawing on previous experience. Experts remember specific examples of various disease categories as exemplars, which enables rapid access to diagnostic possibilities and gives them an intuitive sense of the base rates of various diagnoses. After generating diagnostic hypotheses, clinicians then test the hypotheses and subjectively estimate the probability of each diagnostic possibility by using a heuristic called anchoring and adjusting. Although both novices and experts use this two step diagnostic process, experts distinguish themselves as better diagnosticians through their ability to mobilize experiential knowledge in a manner that is content specific. Experience is clearly the best teacher, but some educational strategies have been shown to modestly improve diagnostic accuracy. Increased knowledge about the cognitive psychology of the diagnostic process and the pitfalls inherent in the process may inform clinical teachers and help learners and clinicians to improve the accuracy of diagnostic reasoning. This article reviews the literature on the cognitive psychology of diagnostic reasoning in the context of cardiovascular disease.


Asunto(s)
Cardiología/métodos , Enfermedades Cardiovasculares/diagnóstico , Toma de Decisiones Clínicas/métodos , Psicología Cognitiva , Competencia Clínica , Heurística , Humanos , Solución de Problemas
10.
BMJ Qual Saf ; 31(6): 426-433, 2022 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34611040

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Diagnostic errors unfortunately remain common. Electronic differential diagnostic support (EDS) systems may help, but it is unclear when and how they ought to be integrated into the diagnostic process. OBJECTIVE: To explore how much EDS improves diagnostic accuracy, and whether EDS should be used early or late in the diagnostic process. SETTING: 6 Canadian medical schools. A volunteer sample of 67 medical students, 62 residents in internal medicine or emergency medicine, and 61 practising internists or emergency medicine physicians were recruited in May through June 2020. INTERVENTION: Participants were randomised to make use of EDS either early (after the chief complaint) or late (after the complete history and physical is available) in the diagnostic process while solving each of 16 written cases. For each case, we measured the number of diagnoses proposed in the differential diagnosis and how often the correct diagnosis was present within the differential. RESULTS: EDS increased the number of diagnostic hypotheses by 2.32 (95% CI 2.10 to 2.49) when used early in the process and 0.89 (95% CI 0.69 to 1.10) when used late in the process (both p<0.001). Both early and late use of EDS increased the likelihood of the correct diagnosis being present in the differential (7% and 8%, respectively, both p<0.001). Whereas early use increased the number of diagnostic hypotheses (most notably for students and residents), late use increased the likelihood of the correct diagnosis being present in the differential regardless of one's experience level. CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE: EDS increased the number of diagnostic hypotheses and the likelihood of the correct diagnosis appearing in the differential, and these effects persisted irrespective of whether EDS was used early or late in the diagnostic process.


Asunto(s)
Medicina Interna , Estudiantes de Medicina , Canadá , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Errores Diagnósticos/prevención & control , Electrónica , Humanos
11.
Can Med Educ J ; 12(5): 18-23, 2021 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34804284

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Entrustment decisions may be retrospective (based on past experiences with a trainee) or real-time (based on direct observation). We investigated judgments of entrustment based on assessor prior knowledge of candidates and based on systematic direct observation, conducted in an objective structured clinical exam (OSCE). METHODS: Sixteen faculty examiners provided 287 retrospective and real-time entrustment ratings of 16 cardiology trainees during OSCE stations in 2019 and 2020. Reliability and validity of these ratings were assessed by comparing correlations across stations as a measure of reliability, differences across postgraduate years as an index of construct validity, correlation to standardized in-training exam (ITE) as a measure of criterion validity, and reclassification of entrustment as a measure of consequential validity. RESULTS: Both retrospective and real-time assessments were highly reliable (all intra-class correlations >0.86). Both increased with a year of postgraduate training. Real-time entrustment ratings were significantly correlated with standardized ITE scores; retrospective ratings were not. Real-time ratings explained 37% (2019) and 46% (2020) of variance in examination scores vs. 21% (2019) and 7% (2020) for retrospective ratings. Direct observation resulted in a different level of entrustment compared with retrospective ratings in 44% of cases (p = <0.001). CONCLUSIONS: Ratings based on direct observation made unique contributions to entrustment decisions.


CONTEXTE: La décision de confier une activité peut être rétrospective (basée sur les expériences antérieures avec un apprenant) ou en temps réel (basée sur l'observation directe). Nous avons étudié les évaluations de niveaux de confiance fondées sur des interactions antérieures des candidats par les évaluateurs et celles fondées sur l'observation directe systématique, dans le cadre d'un examen clinique objectif structuré (ECOS). MÉTHODES: Seize évaluateurs du corps professoral ont fourni 287 évaluations rétrospectives et en temps réel du niveau de confiance faites lors des stations d'ECOS en 2019 et 2020 concernant 16 stagiaires en cardiologie. La fiabilité et la validité de ces évaluations ont été analysées en comparant les corrélations entre les stations comme mesure de la fiabilité, les différences entre les années d'études postdoctorales comme indice de la validité de construit, la corrélation avec l'examen normalisé en cours de formation (ITE) comme mesure de la validité de critère, et le reclassement des évaluations de la confiance comme mesure de la validité corrélative. RÉSULTATS: Les évaluations rétrospectives et en temps réel étaient toutes les deux très fiables (toutes les corrélations intra-classes >0,86). Les deux augmentaient avec le niveau de formation postdoctorale. Les évaluations de la confiance en temps réel étaient significativement corrélées aux scores de l'examen normalisé en cours de formation; les évaluations rétrospectives ne l'étaient pas. Les évaluations en temps réel expliquaient 37 % (2019) et 46 % (2020) de la variance des notes d'examen, contre 21 % (2019) et 7 % (2020) pour les évaluations rétrospectives. L'observation directe a permis de reclasser 44 % des évaluations rétrospectives de la confiance (p=<0,001 dans les deux cas). CONCLUSION: Les évaluations basées sur l'observation directe contribuent de façon importante à la décision de confier une activité.

12.
Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract ; 26(3): 811-825, 2021 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33423154

RESUMEN

Rapidly assessing how ill a patient is based on their immediate presentation-colloquially termed 'eyeballing' in practice-serves a vital role in acute care settings. Yet surprisingly little is known about how this diagnostic skill is learned or how it should be taught. Some authors have pointed to a dual-process model, suggesting that assessments of illness severity are driven by two distinct types of processing: an intuitive, fast, pattern recognition-like process (Type 1) that depends on many prior patient encounters and outcomes being stored in memory; and a deliberate, slow, analytic process (Type 2) characterized by additional data gathering, data scrutiny, or recollection of rules. But prior studies have supported a dual-process model for the assessment of illness severity only insofar as experienced clinicians chiefly displayed what was presumed to be Type 1 processing. Here we further explored a dual-process model by examining whether less experienced clinicians displayed both types of processing when assessing illness severity across a series of cases. Consistent with the model, a dissociation between Type 1 and Type 2 processing was observed through resident reports of deliberation, response times, and three eye tracking metrics associated with diagnostic expertise. We conclude by discussing potential implications for the training of this enigmatic diagnostic skill.


Asunto(s)
Tecnología de Seguimiento Ocular , Aprendizaje , Cuidados Críticos , Humanos
13.
ATS Sch ; 2(4): 620-631, 2021 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35083465

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Trainees in acute care specialties often grapple with the decision to perform an invasive procedure in a rapidly decompensating patient, for whom the benefits and risks are inherently uncertain. The difference between trainees who know when to act and when to seek supervision and those who do not is often linked to individual trainee psychological and cultural perceptions of uncertainty. But how much comfort with uncertainty relates to the situational context rather than the trainee traits is underexplored. OBJECTIVE: The objective of this study was to explore trainee actions around decompensating patients and assess the degree to which invasive intervention and supervision seeking depend on situational certainty or individual trait-based perceptions of uncertainty. METHODS: A total of 41 internal medicine residents completed a survey to measure anxiety related to uncertainty using the Physicians' Reactions to Uncertainty (PRU) tool and to measure uncertainty avoidance using the Values Survey Module (VSM) before responding to 14 written emergency situations. Half of the scenarios contain sufficient diagnostic certainty to warrant aggressive intervention, and half lack sufficient diagnostic clarity to offset the risk of intervention. Mixed multivariable modeling was used to identify the relationship between planned invasive intervention, situational uncertainty, and trait-based perceptions of uncertainty measured in the PRU and VSM. RESULTS: Trainees' first actions were appropriate in 60% of cases. Multivariable modeling suggested that situational certainty was more predictive of upfront intervention (odds ratio [OR], 30.5; P < 0.0001) than trait-based PRU (OR, 1.22; P = 0.05) and VSM (OR, 1.73; P < 0.0001). Similarly, situational certainty was more predictive of reduced supervision seeking (OR, 0.20; P < 0.0001) than trait-based PRU (OR, 2.03; P < 0.001) and VSM (P = not significant). CONCLUSIONS: Situation-specific certainty was more strongly correlated with invasive intervention in cases of decompensated patients than individual trainee traits. Focusing on trainee contextual understanding of procedural risk-benefit ratios in decompensating patients holds more promise for improving trainee actions and supervision seeking than tackling their perceptions around uncertainty.

14.
Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract ; 25(5): 1191-1201, 2020 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33247316

RESUMEN

The paper reviews 50 years of research in health sciences education and identifies several recurring controversies-formative versus summative assessment, high and low fidelity simulation, expertise as knowledge versus skills, and the impact of teaching versus curriculum. I then look at the role these may play in the current situation where COVID has necessitated rapid change to distance learning. I then posit an essential role for research in teaching and learning, using multiple methods from qualitative to neuropsychological to better understand the dimensions of effective teaching. The ultimate goal is to operationalize these findings in creation of distance learning modules.


Asunto(s)
Empleos en Salud/educación , Investigación/organización & administración , Competencia Clínica , Curriculum , Evaluación Educacional , Conocimientos, Actitudes y Práctica en Salud , Humanos , Entrenamiento Simulado
15.
Anat Sci Educ ; 13(3): 401-412, 2020 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31665563

RESUMEN

Anatomy education has been revolutionized through digital media, resulting in major advances in realism, portability, scalability, and user satisfaction. However, while such approaches may well be more portable, realistic, or satisfying than traditional photographic presentations, it is less clear that they have any superiority in terms of student learning. In this study, it was hypothesized that virtual and mixed reality presentations of pelvic anatomy will have an advantage over two-dimensional (2D) presentations and perform approximately equal to physical models and that this advantage over 2D presentations will be reduced when stereopsis is decreased by covering the non-dominant eye. Groups of 20 undergraduate students learned pelvic anatomy under seven conditions: physical model with and without stereo vision, mixed reality with and without stereo vision, virtual reality with and without stereo vision, and key views on a computer monitor. All were tested with a cadaveric pelvis and a 15-item, short-answer recognition test. Compared to the key views, the physical model had a 70% increase in accuracy in structure identification; the virtual reality a 25% increase, and the mixed reality a non-significant 2.5% change. Blocking stereopsis reduced performance on the physical model by 15%, on virtual reality by 60%, but by only 2.5% on the mixed reality technology. The data show that virtual and mixed reality technologies tested are inferior to physical models and that true stereopsis is critical in learning anatomy.


Asunto(s)
Anatomía/educación , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Estudiantes/psicología , Realidad Virtual , Adolescente , Evaluación Educacional/estadística & datos numéricos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Anatómicos , Huesos Pélvicos/anatomía & histología , Estudiantes/estadística & datos numéricos , Interfaz Usuario-Computador , Adulto Joven
16.
JAMA Netw Open ; 2(12): e1918023, 2019 12 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31860107

RESUMEN

Importance: Clinicians use probability estimates to make a diagnosis. Teaching students to make more accurate probability estimates could improve the diagnostic process and, ultimately, the quality of medical care. Objective: To test whether novice clinicians can be taught to make more accurate bayesian revisions of diagnostic probabilities using teaching methods that apply either explicit conceptual instruction or repeated examples. Design, Setting, and Participants: A randomized clinical trial of 2 methods for teaching bayesian updating and diagnostic reasoning was performed. A web-based platform was used for consent, randomization, intervention, and testing of the effect of the intervention. Participants included 61 medical students at McMaster University and Eastern Virginia Medical School recruited from May 1 to September 30, 2018. Interventions: Students were randomized to (1) receive explicit conceptual instruction regarding diagnostic testing and bayesian revision (concept group), (2) exposure to repeated examples of cases with feedback regarding posttest probability (experience group), or (3) a control condition with no conceptual instruction or repeated examples. Main Outcomes and Measures: Students in all 3 groups were tested on their ability to update the probability of a diagnosis based on either negative or positive test results. Their probability revisions were compared with posttest probability revisions that were calculated using the Bayes rule and known test sensitivity and specificity. Results: Of the 61 participants, 22 were assigned to the concept group, 20 to the experience group, and 19 to the control group. Approximate age was 25 years. Two participants were first-year; 37, second-year; 12, third-year; and 10, fourth-year students. Mean (SE) probability estimates of students in the concept group were statistically significantly closer to calculated bayesian probability than the other 2 groups (concept, 0.4%; [0.7%]; experience, 3.5% [0.7%]; control, 4.3% [0.7%]; P < .001). Although statistically significant, the differences between groups were relatively modest, and students in all groups performed better than expected, based on prior reports in the literature. Conclusions and Relevance: The study showed a modest advantage for students who received theoretical instruction on bayesian concepts. All participants' probability estimates were, on average, close to the bayesian calculation. These findings have implications for how to teach diagnostic reasoning to novice clinicians. Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT04130607.


Asunto(s)
Competencia Clínica , Toma de Decisiones Clínicas/métodos , Educación de Pregrado en Medicina/métodos , Estudiantes de Medicina/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto , Teorema de Bayes , Curriculum , Evaluación Educacional , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Anamnesis/métodos , Probabilidad
18.
Acad Med ; 94(8): 1066, 2019 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31361637
19.
Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract ; 24(3): 441-442, 2019 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30915640

RESUMEN

Due to an unfortunate turn of events, Fig. 3 was omitted from the original publication.

20.
Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract ; 24(3): 427-440, 2019 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30694452

RESUMEN

There is an ongoing debate regarding the cause of diagnostic errors. One view is that errors result from unconscious application of cognitive heuristics; the alternative is that errors are a consequence of knowledge deficits. The objective of this study was to compare the effectiveness of checklists that (a) identify and address cognitive biases or (b) promote knowledge retrieval, as a means to reduce errors in ECG interpretation. Novice postgraduate year (PGY) 1 emergency medicine and internal medicine residents (n = 40) and experienced cardiology fellows (PGY 4-6) (n = 21) were randomly allocated to three conditions: a debiasing checklist, a content (knowledge) checklist, or control (no checklist) to be used while interpreting 20 ECGs. Half of the ECGs were deliberately engineered to predispose to bias. Diagnostic performance under either checklist intervention was not significantly better than the control. As expected, more errors occurred when cases were designed to induce bias (F = 96.9, p < 0.0001). There was no significant interaction between the instructional condition and level of learner. Checklists attempting to help learners identify cognitive bias or mobilize domain-specific knowledge did not have an overall effect in reducing diagnostic errors in ECG interpretation, although they may help novices. Even when cognitive biases are deliberately inserted in cases, cognitive debiasing checklists did not improve participants' performance.


Asunto(s)
Cardiología/educación , Lista de Verificación , Errores Diagnósticos/prevención & control , Educación de Postgrado en Medicina/métodos , Electrocardiografía , Medicina de Emergencia/educación , Medicina Interna/educación , Sesgo , Canadá , Competencia Clínica , Cognición , Toma de Decisiones , Errores Diagnósticos/psicología , Humanos , Países Bajos , Estados Unidos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...