Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 240
Filtrar
1.
Biol Psychol ; 137: 1-11, 2018 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29913202

RESUMEN

Sexual dimorphism has been proposed as one of the facial traits to have evolved through sexual selection and to affect attractiveness perception. Even with numerous studies documenting its effect on attractiveness and mate choice, the neurophysiological correlates of the perception of sexual dimorphism are not yet fully understood. In the present study, event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded during visualisation of faces that had been previously transformed in shape to appear more masculine or more feminine. The participants' task consisted of judging the attractiveness of half of the total number of faces, and performing a sex discrimination task on the other half. Both early and late potentials were modulated by the sex of faces, whereas the effect of the sexually dimorphic transform was mainly visible in the P2 (positive deflection around 200 ms after stimulus onset), EPN (early posterior negativity) and LPP (late positive potential) components. There was an effect of sexual dimorphism on P2 and EPN amplitudes when female participants visualised male faces, which may indicate that masculinity is particularly attended to when viewing opposite sex members. Also, ERP results seem to support the idea of sex differences in social categorisation decisions regarding faces, although differences were not evident on behavioural results. In general, these findings contribute to a better understanding of how humans perceive sexually dimorphic characteristics in other individuals' faces and how they affect attractiveness judgements.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados , Reconocimiento Facial , Caracteres Sexuales , Conducta Sexual , Adolescente , Adulto , Cara , Femenino , Feminidad , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Matrimonio , Masculinidad , Percepción , Adulto Joven
2.
Int J Lab Hematol ; 37(5): 577-82, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26372049

RESUMEN

Automated high performance liquid chromatography and Capillary electrophoresis are used to quantitate the proportion of Hemoglobin A2 (HbA2 ) in blood samples order to enable screening and diagnosis of carriers of ß-thalassemia. Since there is only a very small difference in HbA2 levels between people who are carriers and people who are not carriers such analyses need to be both precise and accurate. This paper examines the different parameters of such equipment and discusses how they should be assessed.


Asunto(s)
Automatización de Laboratorios , Cromatografía Líquida de Alta Presión , Electroforesis Capilar , Hemoglobina A2/química , Cromatografía Líquida de Alta Presión/instrumentación , Cromatografía Líquida de Alta Presión/métodos , Cromatografía Líquida de Alta Presión/normas , Electroforesis Capilar/instrumentación , Electroforesis Capilar/métodos , Electroforesis Capilar/normas , Hemoglobina A2/genética , Humanos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Talasemia beta/sangre , Talasemia beta/diagnóstico , Talasemia beta/genética
3.
Neuroimage ; 84: 367-75, 2014 Jan 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24012546

RESUMEN

Empathy involves experiencing emotion vicariously, and understanding the reasons for those emotions. It may be served partly by a motor simulation function, and therefore share a neural basis with imitation (as opposed to mimicry), as both involve sensorimotor representations of intentions based on perceptions of others' actions. We recently showed a correlation between imitation accuracy and Empathy Quotient (EQ) using a facial imitation task and hypothesised that this relationship would be mediated by the human mirror neuron system. During functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), 20 adults observed novel 'blends' of facial emotional expressions. According to instruction, they either imitated (i.e. matched) the expressions or executed alternative, pre-prescribed mismatched actions as control. Outside the scanner we replicated the association between imitation accuracy and EQ. During fMRI, activity was greater during mismatch compared to imitation, particularly in the bilateral insula. Activity during imitation correlated with EQ in somatosensory cortex, intraparietal sulcus and premotor cortex. Imitation accuracy correlated with activity in insula and areas serving motor control. Overlapping voxels for the accuracy and EQ correlations occurred in premotor cortex. We suggest that both empathy and facial imitation rely on formation of action plans (or a simulation of others' intentions) in the premotor cortex, in connection with representations of emotional expressions based in the somatosensory cortex. In addition, the insula may play a key role in the social regulation of facial expression.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Empatía/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología , Neuronas Espejo/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Adulto Joven
4.
Biol Lett ; 9(6): 20130633, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24307526

RESUMEN

Recently, the importance of skin colour for facial attractiveness has been recognized. In particular, dietary carotenoid-induced skin colour has been proposed as a signal of health and therefore attractiveness. While perceptual results are highly consistent, it is currently not clear whether carotenoid skin colour is preferred because it poses a cue to current health condition in humans or whether it is simply seen as a more aesthetically pleasing colour, independently of skin-specific signalling properties. Here, we tested this question by comparing attractiveness ratings of faces to corresponding ratings of meaningless scrambled face images matching the colours and contrasts found in the face. We produced sets of face and non-face stimuli with either healthy (high-carotenoid coloration) or unhealthy (low-carotenoid coloration) colour and asked participants for attractiveness ratings. Results showed that, while for faces increased carotenoid coloration significantly improved attractiveness, there was no equivalent effect on perception of scrambled images. These findings are consistent with a specific signalling system of current condition through skin coloration in humans and indicate that preferences are not caused by sensory biases in observers.


Asunto(s)
Belleza , Carotenoides/química , Cara/fisiología , Pigmentación de la Piel , Piel/anatomía & histología , Piel/metabolismo , Adolescente , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Prejuicio , Distribución Aleatoria , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Adulto Joven
5.
Front Psychol ; 4: 358, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23801979

RESUMEN

Adaptation to facial characteristics, such as gender and viewpoint, has been shown to both bias our perception of faces and improve facial discrimination. In this study, we examined whether adapting to two levels of face trustworthiness improved sensitivity around the adapted level. Facial trustworthiness was manipulated by morphing between trustworthy and untrustworthy prototypes, each generated by morphing eight trustworthy and eight untrustworthy faces, respectively. In the first experiment, just-noticeable differences (JNDs) were calculated for an untrustworthy face after participants adapted to an untrustworthy face, a trustworthy face, or did not adapt. In the second experiment, the three conditions were identical, except that JNDs were calculated for a trustworthy face. In the third experiment we examined whether adapting to an untrustworthy male face improved discrimination to an untrustworthy female face. In all experiments, participants completed a two-interval forced-choice (2-IFC) adaptive staircase procedure, in which they judged which face was more untrustworthy. JNDs were derived from a psychometric function fitted to the data. Adaptation improved sensitivity to faces conveying the same level of trustworthiness when compared to no adaptation. When adapting to and discriminating around a different level of face trustworthiness there was no improvement in sensitivity and JNDs were equivalent to those in the no adaptation condition. The improvement in sensitivity was found to occur even when adapting to a face with different gender and identity. These results suggest that adaptation to facial trustworthiness can selectively enhance mechanisms underlying the coding of facial trustworthiness to improve perceptual sensitivity. These findings have implications for the role of our visual experience in the decisions we make about the trustworthiness of other individuals.

6.
J Hosp Infect ; 84(2): 97-102, 2013 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23518458

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: New Department of Health (England) Choice Framework for Local Policies and Procedures guidance (CFPP 0101) still states that ninhydrin can be used to check for efficient protein removal from surgical instruments processed in sterile services departments (SSDs). AIM: With the potential transfer of variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (vCJD) via surgical procedures it is necessary to re-evaluate recommended methods for protein detection. METHODS: This paper reports studies on the sensitivity and applicability of ninhydrin for detecting proteins in laboratories and SSDs. The efficiency of protein removal by swabbing was also evaluated. FINDINGS: Ninhydrin showed poor sensitivity toward proteins. Limits of detection for bovine serum albumin (BSA) in solution were 205 µg/mL compared with arginine 6 µg/mL. A commercial kit could detect neither rat brain homogenate nor BSA at <1000 µg protein pipetted directly into the vials. Swabbing with water-wetted rayon swabs was inefficient at removing protein (50 µg) from instruments (N = 6) with 32 ± 4% BSA and 61 ± 5% fibrinogen remaining bound. Swabs dipped in 0.5% detergent (Triton X-100) solution had slightly better removal efficiency with 20 ± 3% BSA and 24 ± 2.8% fibrinogen remaining. CONCLUSIONS: Ninhydrin kits, currently used in SSDs, are ineffective at detecting residual proteins due not only to the insensitivity of ninhydrin towards proteins but also to the poor desorption of adhered proteins by swabbing. Overall ninhydrin, either as a laboratory reagent or as supplied in protein detection kits, does not provide sensitive detection of proteins and generates high numbers of false negatives when used in decontamination practices.


Asunto(s)
Descontaminación/métodos , Descontaminación/normas , Indicadores y Reactivos/metabolismo , Ninhidrina/metabolismo , Proteínas/análisis , Control de Calidad , Animales , Bovinos , Inglaterra , Humanos , Ratas , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Instrumentos Quirúrgicos
7.
Biol Lett ; 9(3): 20130050, 2013 Jun 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23536442

RESUMEN

Women in the UK prefer the faces of men with low levels of the stress hormone cortisol, and the relationship is moderated by the sex hormone testosterone. In a Latvian sample, however, women's preferences were not affected by cortisol, and the interaction with testosterone differed from that of the UK. To further explore cross-cultural variation in preferences for facial cues to sex- and stress-hormones, we tested the preferences of women from 13 countries for facial composites constructed to differ in combinations of the hormones. We found significant relationships between a measure of societal development (the United Nations human development index 2011) and preferences for cues to testosterone in the face, and the interaction between preferences for cues to testosterone and cortisol. We also found a significant relationship between preferences for cues to testosterone and a societal-level measure of parasite stress. We conclude that societal-level ecological factors influence the relative value of traits revealed by combinations of sex- and stress-hormones.


Asunto(s)
Comparación Transcultural , Cara , Hidrocortisona/sangre , Factores Sexuales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
Psychol Sci ; 23(7): 718-22, 2012 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22623509

RESUMEN

Male facial width-to-height ratio appears to correlate with antisocial tendencies, such as aggression, exploitation, cheating, and deception. We present evidence that male facial width-to-height ratio is also associated with a stereotypically male prosocial tendency: to increase cooperation with other in-group members during intergroup competition. We found that men who had wider faces, compared with men who had narrower faces, showed more self-sacrificing cooperation to help their group members when there was competition with another group. We propose that this finding makes sense given the evolutionary functions of social helpfulness and aggression.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Competitiva/fisiología , Conducta Cooperativa , Cara/anatomía & histología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculinidad , Conducta Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Procesos de Grupo , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Psicológicas , Juegos de Video/psicología , Adulto Joven
9.
Med Sci Law ; 52(1): 6-11, 2012 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22041123

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Studies on the ageing of bruises have been reported on Caucasians or individuals of fair ethnicity. This study focuses on bruise changes in dark-skinned individuals using tristimulus colorimetry for forensic analysis in such individuals. METHODS: Eighteen subjects of South Indian or Sri-Lankan ethnicity were recruited. Subjects were bruised using a vacuum pump and then daily colour measurements were taken of the bruise using a tristimulus colorimeter. The L*a*b* readings were recorded of a control area and of the bruise until it disappeared. Two Caucasians were used for comparison. RESULTS: This study showed that, using colorimetry, bruises on dark-skinned individuals can be measured and analysed even if the bruises are unclear visually. As the bruise is beneath the skin, the colour difference ΔL*, Δa* and Δb* were calculated. All values showed a trend, indicating that the L*a*b* measuring technique is a reliable method to analyse bruises on dark-skinned individuals. Comparisons of Asian subjects and Caucasian subjects were performed. The largest difference was seen in the b* value. Statistical analysis showed that ΔL* colour difference was the most consistent (95% CI -4.05 to -2.49) showing a significant difference between days 1-4 and 5-8. CONCLUSION: Objective assessment of bruises on dark-skinned individuals using the L*a*b* method of measuring gave reproducible results. Furthermore, the study showed that the yellowing of a bruise cannot be seen or measured with a tristimulus colorimeter on dark-skinned individuals due to the pigmentation of the skin. With further studies and more subjects, the age of bruises could potentially be assessed for use in forensic analysis.


Asunto(s)
Colorimetría/métodos , Contusiones/patología , Pigmentación de la Piel , Adolescente , Adulto , Pueblo Asiatico , Femenino , Patologia Forense , Humanos , India , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Espectrofotometría , Sri Lanka , Población Blanca , Adulto Joven
10.
Med Sci Law ; 51(3): 170-6, 2011 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21905574

RESUMEN

AIMS: The aims of this study were (A) to investigate whether the number of years of forensic experience affected the accuracy with which forensic experts (FEs) were able to age bruises and (B) to identify the properties and colours of a bruise that were utilized by FEs in their assessment of bruise age. The study then investigated the possibility of using a more objective technique. It was decided to use readily available digital photography and software to objectively assess changes in bruise colouration and to investigate if this can be used to age bruises. METHODS: Twenty-three FEs were shown 25 photographs of bruises of varying but known ages and asked to estimate the ages. In part two of the study, bruises were inflicted on volunteers using a vacuum pump and photographs taken of the bruise daily from infliction to resolution. The images were analysed using Adobe Photoshop. Red, green and blue (RGB) values were recorded for each bruise and analyses carried out comparing the values over time between subjects and within subjects. RESULTS: This study both enhanced and supported a previous conclusion that visual assessment of photographs is an unreliable method for ageing bruises. Additionally, it found that the degree of forensic experience had no effect on accuracy. It also identified that colour (particularly yellow, red and purple) and intensity of colour were the most commonly used properties of a bruise in the assessment of its age. The RGB method proved to be a reliable technique with which to measure bruise colour, but its validity in the assessment of bruise age was poor. CONCLUSIONS: Visual assessment of bruises is unreliable and the accuracy of ageing was not improved by the degree of forensic experience. The RGB method gave highly reproducible results, but did not accurately assess bruise age. However, results within subjects suggested that there may be individual variation in haemoglobin metabolism.


Asunto(s)
Contusiones/patología , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Patologia Forense , Humanos , Variaciones Dependientes del Observador , Fotograbar , Factores de Tiempo
12.
Horm Behav ; 60(3): 269-74, 2011 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21672543

RESUMEN

The stress-linked version of the immunocompetence handicap hypothesis has been proposed to account for inconsistencies in relationships between testosterone and immune response. The model has received some support from studies demonstrating roles of stress hormones in relationships between testosterone, immune function and secondary sexual ornamentation. Such work, however, has relied on artificial elevation of testosterone so may not reflect relationships in natural populations. We created human male facial stimuli on the basis of naturally co-occurring levels of salivary testosterone and the stress hormone cortisol. In Study 1 we tested female preferences for male faces with cues to combinations of the hormones across the menstrual cycle, and in Study 2 we tested perceptions of health and dominance in a novel set of facial stimuli. Females preferred cues to low cortisol, a preference that was strongest during the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle. The effects of cortisol on attractiveness and perceived health and dominance were contingent upon level of testosterone: the effects of the stress hormone were reduced when testosterone was high. We propose explanations for our results, including low cortisol as a cue to a heritable component of health, attractiveness as a predictor of low social-evaluative threat (and, therefore, low baseline cortisol) and testosterone as a proxy of male ability to cope efficiently with stressors.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Cara , Hidrocortisona/fisiología , Inmunocompetencia , Conducta Sexual , Estrés Psicológico/inmunología , Testosterona/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Ciclo Menstrual , Adulto Joven
14.
Br Dent J ; 210(9): E14, 2011 May 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21372833

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To assess residual protein on dental instruments cleaned in general dental practice by manual, manual plus ultrasonic and automated washer disinfector (AWD) processes. DESIGN AND SETTING: Instruments submitted by 30 dental surgeries in the South West of England. SUBJECTS (MATERIALS) AND METHODS: Instruments analysed were matrix bands, associated retaining clips, diamond and stainless steel burs, extraction forceps and hand scalers. Each instrument was visually assessed under magnification for residual debris. Residual protein was extracted by immersion in detergent and sonication. A collection of used but uncleaned instruments of each type (n = 177) was also analysed for adherent protein using ophthalaldehyde/N-acetylcysteine reagent. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: Residual protein levels allowed comparisons to be made on the effectiveness of different cleaning processes. RESULTS: One thousand, three hundred and four instruments were analysed. Observational data demonstrated several shortcomings in cleaning chemistries and operation of the AWD. For uncleaned instruments, median residual protein levels ranged from 0.4 µg (stainless steel burs) to 462 µg (extraction forceps). Following manual washing, median protein levels ranged from 0.3-78 µg; for manual plus ultrasonic washing, levels ranged from 9-39 µg and AWD levels ranged from 0.3-27 µg. Manual washing combined with ultrasonic cleaning was significantly less effective than the other two processes (p <0.008). AWDs reduced the variability in the cleaning process. No correlation was found between visual scoring and residual protein determination. CONCLUSION(S): There was a wide variation in residual protein levels both within and between different methods and instruments and this underlines the complexity of this process.


Asunto(s)
Infección Hospitalaria/prevención & control , Descontaminación/métodos , Instrumentos Dentales , Contaminación de Equipos/prevención & control , Control de Infección Dental/métodos , Descontaminación/instrumentación , Equipo Reutilizado , Humanos , Control de Infección Dental/instrumentación , Proteínas/análisis , Estadísticas no Paramétricas , Esterilización/instrumentación , Esterilización/métodos , Ultrasonido
15.
Proc Biol Sci ; 278(1706): 774-80, 2011 Mar 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20843854

RESUMEN

The stress-linked immunocompetence handicap hypothesis (SL-ICHH) of sexual selection incorporates a role of the stress hormone corticosterone (C; cortisol in humans) in relationships between testosterone (T), immunity and secondary sexual trait expression. In support of this, C has been shown to mediate and moderate relationships between T and immune response and to be inversely related to attractiveness in some avian species. We predicted that female preferences for cues to T in human male faces would be contingent upon co-occurring cortisol levels. In study 1, we tested relationships between T and cortisol and attractiveness, masculinity and health ratings of raw male faces. We found cortisol to be inversely related to attractiveness. In study 2, we tested female preferences for male faces that were parametrically manipulated on the basis of cues to naturally co-occurring levels of T and cortisol across the menstrual cycle. Women preferred cues to low cortisol in general and in the fertile phase of the cycle, and there was an interaction between T and cortisol in general and in the non-fertile phase. Results were consistent with the SL-ICHH but not the original immunocompetence handicap model: females expressed preferences for cues to cortisol but not for cues to T, except in interaction with the stress hormone. Results inform the SL-ICHH by demonstrating female preferences for low cortisol and the nature of its interaction with T in humans, as well as indicating the traits that may be signalled by different combinations of the hormones including immune response, current health and resource acquisition characteristics.


Asunto(s)
Cara/fisiología , Inmunocompetencia/fisiología , Estrés Fisiológico/fisiología , Adolescente , Corticosterona , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Testosterona , Adulto Joven
16.
J Dent Res ; 89(12): 1389-94, 2010 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20940360

RESUMEN

The aim of this study was to investigate the involvement of autoimmune reactions to native and post-translationally modified extracellular matrix components in the pathogenesis of periodontitis. Sera from individuals with aggressive periodontitis (AgP, n = 25), chronic periodontitis (CP, n = 14), and gingivitis (G, n = 18) were tested for the presence of autoantibodies against: (a) native collagen type I (CI) and collagen type III (CIII); (b) CI and CIII post-translationally modified by reactive oxygen species (ROS) of the type present during inflammation; and (c) citrullinated filaggrin-derived peptides (CCP). Autoantibodies to native and ROS-modified CI and CIII as well as autoantibodies to CCP were observed exclusively in patients with AgP and not in those with CP or G. In conclusion, autoimmune reactions to native and post-translationally modified self-antigens may play a role specifically in the pathogenesis of AgP.


Asunto(s)
Periodontitis Agresiva/inmunología , Autoinmunidad/inmunología , Adulto , Periodontitis Agresiva/sangre , Autoanticuerpos/sangre , Autoantígenos/inmunología , Periodontitis Crónica/sangre , Periodontitis Crónica/inmunología , Citrulina/inmunología , Colágeno Tipo I/inmunología , Colágeno Tipo III/inmunología , Electroforesis en Gel de Poliacrilamida , Matriz Extracelular/inmunología , Femenino , Proteínas Filagrina , Fluorescencia , Gingivitis/sangre , Gingivitis/inmunología , Humanos , Radical Hidroxilo/farmacología , Ácido Hipocloroso/farmacología , Imagenología Tridimensional , Proteínas de Filamentos Intermediarios/inmunología , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Nitratos/farmacología , Oxidantes/farmacología , Fosfoproteínas/inmunología , Precursores de Proteínas/inmunología , Procesamiento Proteico-Postraduccional/inmunología , Especies Reactivas de Oxígeno/inmunología , Adulto Joven
17.
Psychol Sci ; 21(3): 349-54, 2010 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20424067

RESUMEN

Decisions about whom to trust are biased by stable facial traits such as attractiveness, similarity to kin, and perceived trustworthiness. Research addressing the validity of facial trustworthiness or its basis in facial features is scarce, and the results have been inconsistent. We measured male trustworthiness operationally in trust games in which participants had options to collaborate for mutual financial gain or to exploit for greater personal gain. We also measured facial (bizygomatic) width (scaled for face height) because this is a sexually dimorphic, testosterone-linked trait predictive of male aggression. We found that men with greater facial width were more likely to exploit the trust of others and that other players were less likely to trust male counterparts with wide rather than narrow faces (independent of their attractiveness). Moreover, manipulating this facial-width ratio with computer graphics controlled attributions of trustworthiness, particularly for subordinate female evaluators.


Asunto(s)
Cefalometría , Conducta Cooperativa , Señales (Psicología) , Cara/anatomía & histología , Identidad de Género , Confianza , Adolescente , Adulto , Gráficos por Computador , Decepción , Toma de Decisiones , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Juicio , Masculino , Distorsión de la Percepción , Predominio Social , Adulto Joven
18.
J Forensic Leg Med ; 17(3): 143-9, 2010 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20211455

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Forensic experts are frequently asked to comment on the age of injuries and interpretation may have significant medico-legal consequences. The purpose of this study is to determine the accuracy with which forensic experts can visually age a bruise from photographs produced under standard conditions. METHODS: Bruises were produced on the upper arms of 11 subjects by a suction pump. Sequential photographs were taken daily until they were no longer visible to the naked eye. Fifteen forensic experts who did not know the age of the bruises were asked to estimate their ages and also to place them in chronological order. RESULTS: Hundred and thirty-two images of 25 bruises were produced, ranging from 0 to 209h in age. There was considerable inter- and intra-observer variability in accuracy (median difference between the estimated age and the real age=26.0h (95% C.I. 24.0-31.0h). There was greatest accuracy for bruises photographed between 0 and 12h. No significant differences were seen between male and female observers (p=0.553) and performance was also unaffected by the age of the observer (p=0.160). Of the bruise images placed in chronological order >80% of the observers made between 0 and 2 errors. CONCLUSION: Bruise age estimates by forensic experts, from photographs, are unreliable. However, it appears that the vast majority of observers are better able to place bruises from the same subjects in their chronological order.


Asunto(s)
Contusiones/patología , Fotograbar , Femenino , Patologia Forense , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Masculino , Variaciones Dependientes del Observador , Factores de Tiempo
19.
J Chromatogr B Analyt Technol Biomed Life Sci ; 877(31): 3946-52, 2009 Dec 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19889587

RESUMEN

Some of the major serum proteins that are also found in follicular fluid, including transferrin, alpha-macroglobulin and albumin, are thought to play a role in oocyte maturation. This study set out to identify proteins in human follicular fluid by capillary zone electrophoresis and to investigate their relationship to follicular/oocyte maturity and fertility outcome. 176 individual follicular fluid samples, from 30 women undertaking in vitro fertilization/intracytoplasmic sperm injection (IVF/ICSI), were run using an optimized capillary zone electrophoresis method that gave a good separation of sixteen peaks in most samples. Nine of the peaks were identified and quantified but seven remain unknown and require further proteomic identification. Of the identified protein peaks, levels of each were corrected for follicular volume and total content calculated. No significant difference in protein levels was found with regard to oocyte recovery and fertilization. Protein concentrations tended to decrease as the follicular sphere increased whilst total content in follicular fluid increased in proportion to size. This is consistent with simple transudation across a sphere surface area which does not increase in proportion to the follicular fluid. This is not true of the concentration and content pattern of other proteins/biomolecules which are produced by follicular cells locally. In conclusion, neither concentration nor absolute levels of nine major proteins identified in follicular fluids correlated with oocyte presence and fertility outcome. Future work to remove more concentrated proteins (e.g. albumin) would enhance separation of smaller peaks and identification of the unknown molecules.


Asunto(s)
Proteínas del Huevo/análisis , Electroforesis Capilar/métodos , Líquido Folicular/química , Adulto , Proteínas Sanguíneas/análisis , Femenino , Fertilización In Vitro , Humanos , Oocitos/citología , Oocitos/metabolismo , Inducción de la Ovulación , Inyecciones de Esperma Intracitoplasmáticas
20.
Psychol Sci ; 20(2): 149-54, 2009 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19175530

RESUMEN

We used computer image manipulation to develop a test of perception of subtle gradations in cuteness between infant faces. We found that young women (19-26 years old) were more sensitive to differences in infant cuteness than were men (19-26 and 53-60 years old). Women aged 45 to 51 years performed at the level of the young women, whereas cuteness sensitivity in women aged 53 to 60 years was not different from that of men (19-26 and 53-60 years old). Because average age at menopause is 51 years in Britain, these findings suggest the possible involvement of reproductive hormones in cuteness sensitivity. Therefore, we compared cuteness discrimination in pre- and postmenopausal women matched for age and in women taking and not taking oral contraceptives (progestogen and estrogen). Premenopausal women and young women taking oral contraceptives (which raise hormone levels artificially) were more sensitive to variations of cuteness than their respective comparison groups. We suggest that cuteness sensitivity is modulated by female reproductive hormones.


Asunto(s)
Belleza , Anticonceptivos Hormonales Orales , Cara , Progesterona/administración & dosificación , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Persona de Mediana Edad , Posmenopausia , Premenopausia , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...