Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 70
Filtrar
1.
Appetite ; 176: 106095, 2022 09 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35623506

RESUMEN

Social modeling is a powerful influence on people's food intake: When there is a clear and consistent norm, people eat more when eating with someone who eats a lot and eat less when eating with someone who eats only a little. In three studies, the present research examined how clear versus ambiguous social-normative information influences the modeling of food-related decisions. Using a novel online decision-making paradigm, female participants (total N = 1042) were provided with information about how many cookies previous participants had supposedly selected (no information was provided in the control condition), and then decided how many cookies they would choose for a snack. When there was a clear and consistent norm, the typical social modeling effect was observed. When there was a small number of "dissenters" whose responses conflicted with the norm set by the majority, participants' cookie selection still conformed to the behaviour of the majority (Studies 1 and 2). It was only when the behaviour of the previous participants was highly ambiguous that participants behaved as if they had been given no normative information (Study 3). By demonstrating that, except in extreme cases, people use the available information to discern a social norm that influences their behaviour, these findings highlight the power of social norms related to food-related decisions.


Asunto(s)
Preferencias Alimentarias , Bocadillos , Normas Sociales , Conducta de Elección , Dieta , Conducta Alimentaria , Femenino , Humanos
3.
Appetite ; 155: 104820, 2020 12 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32768601

RESUMEN

Defining and measuring such qualities as restrained eating or dieting may require more than simply administering questionnaires and assuming that we are identifying the population that we wish to study. Different questionnaires may identify different types of restrained eaters, and even deciding what restrained eating consists of is a complicated endeavor. We discuss how to define and measure restrained eating, specifying key attributes, and acknowledging the problems inherent in relying on self-report instruments. We conclude that given the difficulties in defining such constructs as restrained eating, we need to specify more clearly exactly what our research questions are, in order to be sure that we are identifying the populations with the attributes necessary to answer those questions.


Asunto(s)
Ingestión de Alimentos , Conducta Alimentaria , Humanos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
4.
Appetite ; 155: 104811, 2020 12 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32735956

RESUMEN

The Theory of Normal Eating suggests that how much others eat sets an upper limit for how much it is appropriate to eat. This study tested the hypothesis that restrained eaters, who typically eat less than they want to, would be more responsive to a high-intake norm than would unrestrained eaters. Data were combined from 8 experimental studies (total N = 735 female participants; 305 restrained eaters, 430 unrestrained eaters). Each study: (a) included a low-intake norm, a high-intake norm, and a no-norm control condition; (b) measured participants' food intake; and (c) included the Restraint Scale as a measure of dietary restraint. There were no differences between restrained unrestrained eaters in the no-norm control condition or in the magnitude of the inhibition effect (i.e., the difference between the low-intake norm condition and the control condition). There was, however, a restraint difference in the magnitude of the augmentation effect (i.e., the difference between the high-intake norm condition and the control condition). Restrained eaters showed a larger augmentation effect (d = 0.58; 95% CI = 0.29, 0.87) than did unrestrained eaters (d = 0.20; 95% CI = -0.05, 0.45). Social norms provide an upper limit for acceptable food intake, with high-intake norms permitting (but not requiring) individuals to indulge themselves. The fact that restrained eaters were more responsive to the high-intake norm than were unrestrained eaters suggests that the high-intake norm gives restrained eaters permission to indulge when they typically eat less than they want to.


Asunto(s)
Dieta , Inhibición Psicológica , Ingestión de Alimentos , Ingestión de Energía , Conducta Alimentaria , Femenino , Humanos
5.
Eat Behav ; 37: 101390, 2020 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32434120

RESUMEN

"Overeating" is a significant public health concern, but little is known about how lay people conceptualize overeating. This study explored participants' conceptions of overeating. Participants were 175 university students and 296 community members (56% women) who were asked to rate the extent to which several statements reflected the concept of "overeating" (1 = Does not capture the meaning at all; 7 = Captures the meaning perfectly). Results showed that eating outside of hunger was viewed as being most strongly linked to the conceptualization of overeating; mindless eating, eating more than some normative amount, and a lack of restriction were all moderately linked to the conceptualization of overeating; and the quality of the food/eating style was rated as least relevant to the conceptualization of overeating. Participants with a higher BMI, participants who perceived themselves as fat, restrained eaters, and participants with an indication of eating pathology rated all of the constructs as more relevant to the concept of overeating than did their respective counterparts, but their overall pattern of responding was the same. The present research provides some initial insights into people's thoughts on what it means to "overeat," which is important given that simply believing that one has overeaten (regardless of the actual amount consumed) can have adverse psychological and behavioral consequences. These insights provide an avenue for future research to explore whether lay conceptions could be more adaptively reshaped to reduce the negative effects of perceived overeating.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Alimentaria/psicología , Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos/psicología , Hiperfagia/psicología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
6.
Appetite ; 151: 104720, 2020 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32348796

RESUMEN

This study examined the effects of food-based social comparisons on hedonic ratings and consumption of a meal. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three experimental conditions in which they were led to believe that they got a worse meal, a better meal, or the same meal as another participant. They then tasted and rated their own meal. Subsequent liking and ad lib food consumption were measured. Participants who were told that another person got a better meal than they did (upward comparison) liked their meal less than if they were told that another person received either the same meal as they did or a worse meal (downward comparison). Similarly, participants who were in the upward comparison condition ate less food than if they were in the control or downward comparison conditions. Consumption was mediated by liking. The results suggest that being told that someone else is eating a meal that is higher or lower in hedonic value than one's own meal induces hedonic contrast and influences liking and consumption.


Asunto(s)
Preferencias Alimentarias , Comparación Social , Ingestión de Alimentos , Emociones , Humanos , Comidas , Gusto
7.
Front Nutr ; 7: 30, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32266281
8.
Eat Behav ; 34: 101313, 2019 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31330478

RESUMEN

The present study aimed to investigate the effect of the spatial positioning of a healthy food cue in the context of unhealthy food cues on subsequent food choice. Undergraduate women (N = 143) were asked to choose a food from a pictorial-style menu that presented a salad and three unhealthier food options in a horizontal line. The position of the salad was manipulated to be presented either (a) in the middle, (b) at the end, or (c) separated by 5 cm to the right of the line of unhealthier food options. Participants also completed a questionnaire measure of dietary restraint. Participants were significantly more likely to choose the salad when it was presented separately rather than in the middle of the unhealthier food options. This effect was not moderated by dietary restraint. The findings point to the possibility of designing pictorial menus in fast-food outlets in such a way as to subtly nudge people towards making healthier food choices.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Dieta/psicología , Conducta Alimentaria/psicología , Preferencias Alimentarias/psicología , Comunicación Persuasiva , Adolescente , Adulto , Conducta de Elección , Comida Rápida , Femenino , Humanos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
9.
Appetite ; 139: 105-109, 2019 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31051198

RESUMEN

The contemporary food-rich environment has been consistently linked to unhealthy eating. Emerging research suggests that changing the presentation context of unhealthy foods by introducing a subtle nudge in the form of a healthy food cue may promote healthier dietary choices. This study investigated the effect of the timing of a healthy food cue (before or simultaneously with food options) on food choices. Participants (n = 210) were presented with a pictorial café-style menu displaying a healthy food cue (basket of fruit and vegetables) either on the menu cover or inside the menu, or they received a control menu. Participants were asked to make one choice from each of three menu sections (meals, beverages, afters), and then completed a measure of dietary restraint. As predicted, participants chose a greater proportion of healthy food from the healthy-cue-before menu compared to both the healthy-cue-simultaneous and control menus. Furthermore, this effect was stronger for restrained eaters. The findings demonstrate that the timing of presentation of a healthy food cue is critical, and could be used to inform the design of menus in real-world environments, such as cafés and fast-food outlets, to promote healthier dietary choices.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Dieta Saludable/psicología , Etiquetado de Alimentos/métodos , Preferencias Alimentarias/psicología , Factores de Tiempo , Adolescente , Adulto , Conducta de Elección , Comida Rápida , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Restaurantes , Adulto Joven
10.
Appetite ; 125: 380-400, 2018 06 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29471070

RESUMEN

We review the research on the association between personality and dietary restraint as measured by commonly-used self-report assessment instruments (Restraint Scale, TFEQ, DEBQ, and EDE-Q). In order to expand our understanding of the potentially different types of dieters, we examine the different personality profiles that emerge from existing studies of restraint and personality, including associations between restraint and body dissatisfaction and body focus, self-esteem, neuroticism, sensation seeking, impulsivity, perfectionism and narcissism. This research provides some preliminary evidence that different measures of restraint are associated not only with different eating behaviours, but that they capture different personality profiles.


Asunto(s)
Dieta/psicología , Conducta Alimentaria/psicología , Personalidad , Imagen Corporal , Dieta Reductora , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Narcisismo , Neuroticismo , Autoimagen , Autocontrol , Sensación
11.
Appetite ; 116: 511-517, 2017 09 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28564585

RESUMEN

Normative eating cues (portion size, social factors) have a powerful impact on people's food intake, but people often fail to acknowledge the influence of these cues, instead explaining their food intake in terms of internal (hunger) or sensory (taste) cues. This study examined whether the same biases apply when making predictions about how much food a person would eat. Participants (n = 364) read a series of vignettes describing an eating scenario and predicted how much food the target person would eat in each situation. Some scenarios consisted of a single eating cue (hunger, taste, or a normative cue) that would be expected to increase intake (e.g., high hunger) or decrease intake (e.g., a companion who eats very little). Other scenarios combined two cues that were in conflict with one another (e.g., high hunger + a companion who eats very little). In the cue-conflict scenarios involving an inhibitory internal/sensory cue (e.g., low hunger) with an augmenting normative cue (e.g., a companion who eats a lot), participants predicted a low level of food intake, suggesting a bias toward the internal/sensory cue. For scenarios involving an augmenting internal/sensory cue (e.g., high hunger) and an inhibitory normative cue (e.g., a companion who eats very little), participants predicted an intermediate level of food intake, suggesting that they were influenced by both the internal/sensory and normative cue. Overall, predictions about food intake tend to reflect a general bias toward internal/sensory cues, but also include normative cues when those cues are inhibitory. If people are systematically biased toward internal, sensory, and inhibitory cues, then they may underestimate how much food they or other people will eat in many situations, particularly when normative cues promoting eating are present.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Ingestión de Alimentos/psicología , Hambre , Gusto , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Conducta de Elección , Femenino , Preferencias Alimentarias , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tamaño de la Porción , Medio Social , Adulto Joven
12.
J Eat Disord ; 5: 18, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28465828

RESUMEN

People seem to have a basic drive to assess the correctness of their opinions, abilities, and emotions. Without absolute indicators of these qualities, people rely on a comparison of themselves with others. Social comparison theory can be applied to eating behavior. For example, restrained eaters presented with a standard slice of pizza ate more of a subsequent food if they thought that they had gotten a bigger slice of pizza than others (i.e., had broken their diets), whereas unrestrained eaters ate less. Social influences on eating such as modeling and impression formation also rely on comparison of one's own eating to others. Comparing one's food to others' meals generally influences eating, affect, and satisfaction.

13.
Curr Obes Rep ; 6(1): 79-85, 2017 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28205156

RESUMEN

PURPOSE OF REVIEW: The purposes of the present review are to organize the recent literature on the effects of food cues on restrained and unrestrained eaters and to determine current directions in such work. RECENT FINDINGS: Research over the last several years involves both replicating the work showing that restrained eaters respond to attractive food cues by eating more but unrestrained eaters show less responsiveness and extending this work to examine the mechanisms that might underlie this differential responsiveness. Labeling a food as healthy encourages more eating by restrained eaters, while diet-priming cues seem to curtail their consumption even in the face of attractive food cues. Work on cognitive responses indicates that restrained (but not unrestrained) eaters have both attention and memory biases toward food cues. Restrained eaters attend more strongly to food- and diet-related cues than do unrestrained eaters, as evidenced in both their eating behavior and their attention and memory responses to such cues. These effects interact with expectations and manner of presentation of such cues. What remains to be understood is the meaning and mechanism of the attention bias toward food cues in restrained eaters and the implications of such bias for overeating and overweight more broadly speaking.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Conducta Alimentaria/psicología , Sesgo Atencional , Condicionamiento Psicológico , Femenino , Preferencias Alimentarias/psicología , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria , Tamaño de la Porción/psicología
14.
Psychol Health ; 32(4): 483-492, 2017 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28135848

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: We examined whether people's attributions for their eating behaviour differ according to whether they believe they have eaten more, less or about the same as they normally would. DESIGN: Participants were served a small or large portion of pasta for lunch. Afterwards, they were asked to compare how much they ate in the study to how much they normally eat for lunch, resulting in three intake-evaluation categories: 'ate less', 'ate about the same' or 'ate more'. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: How much participants ate; the extent to which they attributed their food intake to an internal cue (i.e. hunger) and an external cue (i.e. the amount of food served). RESULTS: Participants served a large portion ate more than those served a small portion, but the magnitude of the portion-size effect did not vary across intake-evaluation categories. Furthermore, although participants in all groups indicated that their hunger influenced how much they ate, only those in the 'ate more' group indicated that the amount of food available influenced how much they ate. CONCLUSION: People appear to be willing to explain their food intake in terms of an external cue only when they believe that they have eaten more than they normally would.


Asunto(s)
Ingestión de Alimentos/psicología , Hiperfagia/psicología , Autoinforme , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Hambre , Tamaño de la Porción/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto Joven
15.
Health Psychol ; 36(4): 365-369, 2017 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27748612

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Social factors have a powerful influence on people's food intake but people typically fail to acknowledge the influence of such external cues, instead explaining their food intake in terms of factors such as how hungry they are. We examined whether the tendency to explain one's food intake in terms of internal cues (i.e., hunger) rather than external cues (i.e., other people's behavior) would be apparent when those cues are in conflict with one another. METHOD: Female participants (n = 104) took part in a pizza taste test after having been food deprived for 18-hr or after consuming a meal-replacement preload. Half of the participants were also exposed to a social norm that conflicted with their deprivation condition: deprived participants were exposed to a low-intake norm, whereas preloaded participants were exposed to a high-intake norm. After completing the taste test, participants indicated the extent to which their food intake was influence by how hungry they were and how much other people ate. RESULTS: Deprived participants ate less when exposed to a low-intake norm than when no norm was present, but reported that the behaviors of others had no impact on their food intake. In contrast, preloaded participants did not eat significantly more when exposed to a high-intake norm, but reported that the behavior of others made them eat more. CONCLUSIONS: Participants are generally inaccurate in the attributions they make for their food intake, and we suggest that these inaccuracies may be because of motivated misreporting. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Ingestión de Alimentos/psicología , Hambre , Percepción Social , Conducta Alimentaria , Femenino , Humanos , Motivación , Adulto Joven
16.
Appetite ; 109: 93-99, 2017 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27890473

RESUMEN

We conducted a preliminary investigation on the resistance to, and persistence of, social influence regarding the appropriate amount to eat, defined in terms of eating an amount similar to that eaten by a confederate. Participants ate pizza both alone and in the presence of remote confederates presenting either a high or low eating norm. In the portion of the experiment examining resistance to social influence, participants given an initial opportunity to form a personal eating norm by eating alone for one session in the absence of social influence were no more resistant to low eating norms than were those who had no such opportunity; however, those who ate alone for two or three prior sessions did show resistance. For the high eating norm, it took three eating alone sessions to create resistance. In the portion of the experiment examining persistence of social influence, when participants ate alone following a session with norm-setting remote confederates, the effect of the social influence persisted. However, the persistence effect varied by norm and weakened over time. Participants modeled a low eating norm for only one additional session and the size of the effect was markedly weaker. By contrast, the high norm persisted for all of the remaining sessions. Thus, individuals' social influence histories can affect their eating.


Asunto(s)
Ingestión de Alimentos/psicología , Conducta Alimentaria/psicología , Tamaño de la Porción/psicología , Conformidad Social , Normas Sociales , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Adulto Joven
17.
Physiol Behav ; 167: 71-75, 2016 12 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27619172

RESUMEN

Food cue exposure has been shown to trigger overeating in restrained eaters. To explore the difficulties experienced by these individuals in regulating their food intake, recent investigations have sought to determine the impact of exposure to a low calorie food cue, but with mixed success. This study tested the possibility that contextual differences moderate the impact of exposure to such a food cue among restrained eaters. To this end, we compared the effect of exposure to a low calorie food cue either on its own or together with a high calorie food cue. Specifically, we exposed 122 undergraduate women to a low calorie food cue (pictures of grapes), or to a high calorie food cue (pictures of cookies), or both, and examined the effect of such food-cue exposure on intake of either grapes or cookies. Restrained eaters were identified by their scores on the Revised Restraint Scale (Herman & Polivy, 1980). In line with predictions regarding dieting goal activation, restrained eaters ate less of the given food, either grapes or cookies, following exposure to the grapes cue alone than after exposure to the grapes+cookies cue. Thus the context in which a low calorie food cue is presented (alone, or in combination with a high calorie food cue) may play an important role in how much restrained eaters eat. The findings have implications for the regulation of food intake in restrained eaters.


Asunto(s)
Ansia/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Ingestión de Alimentos/fisiología , Ingestión de Alimentos/psicología , Privación de Alimentos , Preferencias Alimentarias , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Escala Visual Analógica , Adulto Joven
18.
Eat Behav ; 23: 24-27, 2016 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27448511

RESUMEN

Norms of appropriateness have been used to account for the influence of a variety of external eating cues (e.g., social factors, portion size) on people's food intake. What is less clear is what, exactly, "appropriate" means. This study explored participants' conceptions of appropriate food intake. Two separate samples were included in this study: 121 university students (73% women) and 107 community members (100% women). Participants were asked to rate the extent to which several statements reflected the concept of "appropriate food intake" or "normal food intake" (1=Does not capture the definition at all; 7=Captures the definition perfectly). These statements included items referring to external eating cues (e.g., "Eating as much as other people", "Eating the entire portion of what you are served") and items referring to internal eating cues (e.g., "Eating an amount that will make you feel satisfied") or nutritional needs (e.g., "Eating a healthy amount"). Repeated measures ANOVAs indicated that participants consistently defined appropriate/normal intake in terms of internal eating cues and nutritional needs. In contrast, despite evidence indicating that perceptions of how much is an appropriate amount to eat are affected by external eating cues, external eating cues were ignored in participants' definition of appropriate/normal intake. The disconnect between how people define appropriate intake (i.e., in terms of internal cues) and what research shows affects norms of appropriateness (i.e., external cues) may reflect people's general unwillingness to acknowledge the influence of external eating cues on their food intake.


Asunto(s)
Ingestión de Alimentos/psicología , Conducta Alimentaria/psicología , Necesidades Nutricionales , Tamaño de la Porción , Adolescente , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Emociones , Ingestión de Energía , Femenino , Alimentos , Humanos , Masculino
19.
Appetite ; 103: 259-264, 2016 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27120095

RESUMEN

Pre-exposure to food cues has often been shown to increase food intake, especially in restrained eaters. This study investigated the role of expectations in the effect of such pre-exposure on food intake. A sample of 88 undergraduate women was exposed to visual food cues (photos of grapes and chocolate-chip cookies). In a 2 × 2 × 2 design, participants were explicitly told to expect that they would be tasting and rating either grapes or chocolate-chip cookies. Participants subsequently completed an ostensible taste test, in which they tasted and rated either grapes or cookies, such that half were given the food that they had been led to expect and the other half were given the other food. Participants' restraint status (restrained versus unrestrained) was based on their scores on the Revised Restraint Scale (Herman & Polivy, 1980). A significant interaction between expected food and restraint status was found. When participants were led to expect that they would be tasting grapes, restrained and unrestrained eaters did not differ in their subsequent consumption (of either grapes or cookies). However, when participants were led to expect that they would be tasting cookies, restrained eaters ate significantly less (of both grapes and cookies) than did unrestrained eaters, even though craving ratings were similarly elevated for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. The findings are consistent with counteractive control theory in that restrained eaters who expected to eat a high caloric food may have been able to activate their dieting goal, thereby limiting their food intake. The findings further point to an important role for expectations in the understanding and regulation of food intake in restrained eaters.


Asunto(s)
Ansia , Ingestión de Alimentos/psicología , Conducta Alimentaria/psicología , Alimentos , Gusto , Adolescente , Adulto , Chocolate , Señales (Psicología) , Ingestión de Energía , Femenino , Humanos , Hambre/fisiología , Estudiantes , Vitis
20.
Physiol Behav ; 156: 177-81, 2016 Mar 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26808493

RESUMEN

Large portion sizes are frequently blamed for the obesity epidemic. In this paper, we examine the culpability of large portion sizes. It is true that portion sizes have increased during the obesity epidemic, but there is as yet little evidence that exposure to large portions produces significant weight gain. Furthermore, some evidence argues against the role of large portions in the obesity epidemic. For instance, the epidemic is largely a matter of weight gain among the upper half of the population weight distribution, whereas large portions appear to have an indiscriminate effect on food intake. Factors other than the size of individual meals may well be responsible for a great deal of the observed population weight gain.


Asunto(s)
Obesidad/etiología , Tamaño de la Porción , Aumento de Peso , Ingestión de Alimentos/fisiología , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Humanos , Obesidad/epidemiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...