Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 44
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Neuropsychologia ; 179: 108462, 2023 01 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36563998

RESUMEN

The effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) for improving attention and working memory have been generally mixed and small, potentially due to variability between studies with montages, stimulus parameters and outcome measures. The tDCS montage is an important parameter which determines the degree and intensity of stimulation in targeted brain regions. This study aimed to examine the effects of using three different montages for modulating attention and working memory performance: Bi-frontal, Broad-frontal and Broad-parietal. Ninety-three healthy adults participated in a counterbalanced cross-over study. Participants received both active and sham tDCS with either the Bi-frontal, Broad-frontal or Broad-parietal montage during performance of both a 1- and 2-back task. TDCS montage moderated 2-back working memory reaction time performance, though not accuracy, with faster reaction times observed for active compared to sham tDCS with the Broad-frontal montage only (F (1,90) = 5.26, p = .024, η2 = 0.06). TDCS montage did not significantly moderate performance on the 1-back task. The cognitive effects of tDCS varied according to montage, task, and outcome measure. TDCS administered with the cathode placed extracephalically in a Broad-frontal montage may be beneficial for improving working memory.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Transcraneal de Corriente Directa , Adulto , Humanos , Atención/fisiología , Estudios Cruzados , Electrodos , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología
2.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 176: 149-163, 2022 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35288262

RESUMEN

Auditory stimulus intensity of innocuous tones is generally thought to have a direct effect on the amplitude of ERP components, but these effects have rarely been explored across a wide component range, or in multiple paradigms. Here we investigate component sensitivity to stimulus intensity differences in two studies. Study 1 (N = 36) employed a between-participants paradigm in which repeated trains of standard stimuli were presented as 50 or 80 dB SPL 1000 Hz tones. Study 2 (N = 18) used a within-participant presentation of alternating 60 and 80 dB SPL 1000 Hz tones. Electrode caps with 19 channels (referred to linked ears) generated ERPs covering the first 600 ms of each participant's EEG responses; these were submitted to separate temporal PCAs in each study. A similar series of components was obtained in each study: P1, N1a, N1b, N1c, P2, P3a, P3b, nP3, SW1, and SW2; an N2 was found in Study 2 only. Loud tones in Study 1 produced greater amplitudes in all components except SW1. In Study 2, Loud cf. Soft tones produced smaller P1 and nP3, larger N1 components, P2, and P3a, with no effect on N2, P3b, SW1 or SW2. These results indicate similar sequential processes underlying sensory processing in one- and two-stimulus paradigms, with the later stimulus intensity effects varying with paradigm.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Humanos
3.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 31(5): 814-836, 2021 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32114899

RESUMEN

Non-invasive transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has been reported to facilitate working memory in normal adults. There is some evidence in people with Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) but overall evidence is mixed. This study aimed to address shortcomings of prior study designs in TBI to examine whether a single dose of tDCS would lead to benefits in working memory. Thirty people with severe, chronic TBI were administered a single session of either anodal tDCS (2 mA for 20 min) or sham tDCS (2 mA for 30 s), in a counterbalanced order, over the left parietal cortex while performing 1-back and 2-back working memory tasks. Skin conductance levels were examined as a measure of task activated arousal, a possible functional analogue of cortical excitability. We found that tDCS led to no improvements in accuracy on the working memory tasks. A slight increase in variability and reaction time with tDCS was related to decreased task activated arousal. Overall, this study yielded no evidence that a single session of tDCS can facilitate working memory for people with TBI.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo , Estimulación Transcraneal de Corriente Directa , Adulto , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/complicaciones , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/terapia , Cognición , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Corteza Prefrontal
4.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 19(5): 1218-1230, 2019 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31187442

RESUMEN

The reflexive orienting response triggered by nonpredictive gaze cues is thought to be driven by a dedicated social neural network responsible for directing attention toward socially salient information. However, atypical processing of eye gaze using concomitant perceptual features has been proposed to underlie attentional orienting in groups with impairments in social cognition. Here, we examined the neurophysiological indices of visuospatial attention during a spatial cueing task, considering individual variability in social cognition in typically developing individuals, and the relative salience of social gaze and perceptual motion cues. We found enhanced neural activation to incongruent cues, wherein modulation of the N2b serves as a marker of the allocation of attention in the spatial domain. Our findings suggest the social gaze cue is less salient for those with greater autistic traits. An attentional bias toward perceptual motion cues correlated with greater social anxiety and alexithymia, and thus may reflect reduced sensitivity to social stimuli. These results provide evidence for likely neurophysiological mechanisms underlying gaze cueing and offer insight into the use of qualitatively different cognitive mechanisms used to access social information. Such paradigms provide potential insight into normative orienting responses reported in atypical groups and would benefit investigations of gaze following abilities in clinical populations.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Fijación Ocular , Conducta Social , Procesamiento Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor , Adulto Joven
5.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 19(1): 197-210, 2019 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30488224

RESUMEN

Early theories of emotion processing propose an interplay between autonomic function and cognitive appraisal of emotions. Patients with frontotemporal dementia show profound social cognition deficits and atrophy in regions implicated in autonomic emotional responses (insula, amygdala, prefrontal cortex), yet objective measures of facial expressiveness and physiological arousal have been relatively unexplored. We investigated psychophysiological responses (surface facial electromyography (EMG); skin conductance level (SCL)) to emotional stimuli in 25 behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) patients, 14 semantic dementia (SD) patients, and 24 healthy older controls, while viewing emotionally positive, neutral, or negative video clips. Voxel-based morphometry was conducted to identify neural correlates of responses. Unlike controls, patients with bvFTD did not show differential facial EMG responses according to emotion condition, whereas SD patients showed increased zygomaticus responses to both positive and neutral videos. Controls showed greater arousal (SCL) when viewing positive and negative videos; however, both bvFTD and SD groups showed no change in SCL across conditions. Regardless of group membership, right insula damage was associated with dampened zygomaticus responses to positive film stimuli. Change in arousal (SCL) was associated with lower integrity of the caudate, amygdala, and temporal pole. Our results demonstrate that while bvFTD patients show an overall dampening of responses, SD patients appear to show incongruous facial emotional expressions. Abnormal responding is related to cortical and subcortical brain atrophy. These results identify potential mechanisms for the abnormal social behaviour in bvFTD and SD and demonstrate that psychophysiological responses are an important mechanism underpinning normal socioemotional functioning.


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Demencia Frontotemporal/fisiopatología , Conducta Social , Anciano , Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiopatología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Cara , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
6.
Brain Imaging Behav ; 13(3): 819-829, 2019 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29948905

RESUMEN

Adults with severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) often suffer poor social cognition. Social cognition is complex, requiring verbal, non-verbal, auditory, visual and affective input and integration. While damage to focal temporal and frontal areas has been implicated in disorders of social cognition after TBI, the role of white matter pathology has not been examined. In this study 17 adults with chronic, severe TBI and 17 control participants underwent structural MRI scans and Diffusion Tensor Imaging. The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT) was used to assess their ability to understand emotional states, thoughts, intentions and conversational meaning in everyday exchanges. Track-based spatial statistics were used to perform voxelwise analysis of Fractional Anisotropy (FA) and Mean Diffusivity (MD) of white matter tracts associated with poor social cognitive performance. FA suggested a wide range of tracts were implicated in poor TASIT performance including tracts known to mediate, auditory localisation (planum temporale) communication between nonverbal and verbal processes in general (corpus callosum) and in memory in particular (fornix) as well as tracts and structures associated with semantics and verbal recall (left temporal lobe and hippocampus), multimodal processing and integration (thalamus, external capsule, cerebellum) and with social cognition (orbitofrontal cortex, frontopolar cortex, right temporal lobe). Even when controlling for non-social cognition, the corpus callosum, fornix, bilateral thalamus, right external capsule and right temporal lobe remained significant contributors to social cognitive performance. This study highlights the importance of loss of white matter connectivity in producing complex social information processing deficits after TBI.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/psicología , Cognición/fisiología , Sustancia Blanca/patología , Adulto , Anciano , Encéfalo/patología , Lesiones Encefálicas/psicología , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/patología , Conectoma , Imagen de Difusión por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Imagen de Difusión Tensora/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Conducta Social , Sustancia Blanca/fisiología
7.
Soc Neurosci ; 13(4): 471-479, 2018 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28712330

RESUMEN

The corpus callosum (CC) is vulnerable to severe traumatic brain injury (TBI). Social cognition requires integration of non-verbal and verbal information in order to understand social behaviour and may be compromised if the CC is damaged. 17 adults with severe, chronic TBI and 17 control participants underwent structural MRI and Diffusion Tensor Imaging. A region of interest analysis examined fractional anisotropy (FA) and mean diffusivity (MD) across regions of the CC. Performance on The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): part 1 (emotion recognition) and parts 2 and 3 (social inference), was examined in relation to FA and MD. Across participants, higher genu FA values were related to higher TASIT part 3 scores. Increased splenium FA was associated with better performance for TASIT parts 1-3. There was no association between DTI values and TASIT in the controls alone. In the TBI group, FA of the genu and splenium was correlated with TASIT part 3. The pattern of performance was similar when controlling for non-social cognitive ability. In conclusion, social information is complex and multi-modal requiring inter-hemispheric connection. People with TBI, regardless of focal grey matter injury, may lose social cognitive ability due to trauma related changes to the corpus callosum.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/psicología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/diagnóstico por imagen , Trastornos del Conocimiento/etiología , Cuerpo Calloso/diagnóstico por imagen , Conducta Social , Adulto , Anciano , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/complicaciones , Enfermedad Crónica , Cognición , Imagen de Difusión Tensora , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Sustancia Blanca/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
8.
Neuroimage Clin ; 12: 607-614, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27709066

RESUMEN

Severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) often leads to deficits in physiological arousal and empathy, which are thought to be linked. This study examined whether injury-related brain volume loss in key limbic system structures is associated with these deficits. Twenty-four adults with TBI and 24 matched Controls underwent MRI scans to establish grey matter volumes in the amygdala, thalamus, and hippocampus. EEG and skin conductance levels were recorded to index basal physiological arousal. Self-report emotional empathy levels were also assessed. The TBI group had reduced brain volumes, topographic alpha differences, and lower emotional empathy compared to Controls. Regional brain volumes were differentially correlated to arousal and self-report empathy. Importantly, lower volume in pertinent brain structures correlated with lower empathy, for participants with and without TBI. Overall we provide new insights into empathic processes after TBI and their relationship to brain volume loss.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/patología , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Empatía/fisiología , Sustancia Gris/patología , Sistema Límbico/patología , Adulto , Amígdala del Cerebelo/diagnóstico por imagen , Amígdala del Cerebelo/patología , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Sustancia Gris/diagnóstico por imagen , Hipocampo/diagnóstico por imagen , Hipocampo/patología , Humanos , Sistema Límbico/diagnóstico por imagen , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tálamo/diagnóstico por imagen , Tálamo/patología , Adulto Joven
9.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 22(3): 303-13, 2016 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26754292

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: The current study aimed to determine whether reversal learning impairments and feedback-related negativity (FRN), reflecting reward prediction error signals generated by negative feedback during the reversal learning tasks, were associated with social disinhibition in a group of participants with traumatic brain injury (TBI). METHODS: Number of reversal errors on a social and a non-social reversal learning task and FRN were examined for 21 participants with TBI and 21 control participants matched for age. Participants with TBI were also divided into low and high disinhibition groups based on rated videotaped interviews. RESULTS: Participants with TBI made more reversal errors and produced smaller amplitude FRNs than controls. Furthermore, participants with TBI high on social disinhibition made more reversal errors on the social reversal learning task than did those low on social disinhibition. FRN amplitude was not related to disinhibition. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that impairment in the ability to update behavior when social reinforcement contingencies change plays a role in social disinhibition after TBI. Furthermore, the social reversal learning task used in this study may be a useful neuropsychological tool for detecting susceptibility to acquired social disinhibition following TBI. Finally, that the FRN amplitude was not associated with social disinhibition suggests that reward prediction error signals are not critical for behavioral adaptation in the social domain.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/complicaciones , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/psicología , Inhibición Psicológica , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje/etiología , Aprendizaje Inverso/fisiología , Conducta Social , Adulto , Anciano , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Retroalimentación Psicológica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
10.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 26(1): 103-25, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25627984

RESUMEN

Heart rate variability (HRV) may provide an index of capacity for social functioning and may be remediated by HRV biofeedback. Given reductions in HRV are found following traumatic brain injury (TBI), the present study aimed to determine whether lower HRV in TBI is associated with social function, and whether HRV biofeedback might be a useful remediation technique in this population. Resting state HRV and measures of social and emotional processing were collected in 30 individuals with severe TBI (3-34 years post-injury) and 30 controls. This was followed by a single session of HRV biofeedback. HRV was positively associated with social cognition and empathy, and negatively associated with alexithymia for the TBI group. Both TBI and control groups showed significantly increased HRV on both time-domain (i.e., SDNN, rMSSD) and frequency-domain measures (LF, HF, LF:HF ratio) during biofeedback compared to baseline. These results suggest that decreased HRV is linked to social and emotional function following severe TBI, and may be a novel target for therapy using HRV biofeedback techniques.


Asunto(s)
Biorretroalimentación Psicológica/métodos , Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Lesiones Encefálicas/rehabilitación , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Trastornos del Humor/etiología , Trastorno de la Conducta Social/etiología , Adulto , Anciano , Análisis de Varianza , Concienciación , Lesiones Encefálicas/psicología , Enfermedad Crónica , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Adulto Joven
11.
Psychophysiology ; 52(2): 238-48, 2015 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25098203

RESUMEN

In reinforcement learning (RL), discriminative stimuli (S) allow agents to anticipate the value of a future outcome, and the response that will produce that outcome. We examined this processing by recording EEG locked to S during RL. Incentive value of outcomes and predictive value of S were manipulated, allowing us to discriminate between outcome-related and response-related activity. S predicting the correct response differed from nonpredictive S in the P2. S paired with high-value outcomes differed from those paired with low-value outcomes in a frontocentral positivity and in the P3b. A slow negativity then distinguished between predictive and nonpredictive S. These results suggest that, first, attention prioritizes detection of informative S. Activation of mental representations of these informative S then retrieves representations of outcomes, which in turn retrieve representations of responses that previously produced those outcomes.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Objetivos , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Refuerzo en Psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivación , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
12.
Clin Neurophysiol ; 126(2): 314-24, 2015 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25018009

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: This study aimed to elucidate relationships between dysregulated emotional arousal after severe traumatic brain injury (TBI), alpha power and skin conductance levels (SCL), and brain atrophy. METHODS: Nineteen adults with severe TBI and 19 age-, education-, and gender-matched controls (all p's>0.05) participated. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan established bilateral insulae and amygdale volumes. Mean EEG alpha power and SCLs were recorded simultaneously across four, 2 min conditions: eyes-closed pre-task baseline, view neutral face, happy face and angry face. RESULTS: Scalp-wide alpha suppression occurred from pre-task baseline to the face-viewing conditions (p<.001), but was diminished in TBI (p=.04). TBI participants exhibited marginally significantly lower SCL (p=.051), and elevated alpha power hemispherically, contrasting with controls' midline dominance (p<.01). Significant atrophy was observed in most structures in TBI participants (p's=.004-0.04). Larger left insula, left amygdala and right amygdala correlated positively with alpha power and alpha suppression, and SCLs; all structures uniquely contributed to variance in arousal. CONCLUSIONS: Findings suggest that alpha power provides a sensitive measure of dysregulated emotional arousal post-TBI. Atrophy in pertinent brain structures may contribute to these disturbances. SIGNIFICANCE: These findings have potential implications for the assessment and remediation of TBI-related arousal deficits, by directing more targeted remediation, and better assessing post-TBI recovery.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo alfa , Nivel de Alerta , Lesiones Encefálicas/diagnóstico , Lesiones Encefálicas/fisiopatología , Emociones , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Adulto , Anciano , Ritmo alfa/fisiología , Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiopatología , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Lesiones Encefálicas/psicología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Corteza Cerebral/fisiopatología , Emociones/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto Joven
13.
Biol Psychol ; 94(2): 397-407, 2013 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23998995

RESUMEN

Emotion processing, including automatic facial mimicry, plays an important role in social reciprocity. Disruptions in these processes have implications for individuals with impaired social functioning, such as autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). Past research has demonstrated that ASDs are impaired in the recognition of briefly presented emotions and display atypical mimicry of emotions presented for protracted duration. Mimicry (electromyography; EMG) of briefly presented emotions was investigated in adults with ASDs. Concurrent measures of skin conductance and cardiac responses were used as markers of orientation and stimulus detection, respectively. A backward masking task was employed whereby the emotional face (happy, angry) was presented for 30 ms followed by a neutral face "mask". An implicit comparison task required rapid gender identification. The ASD group failed to differentiate by valence in their EMG (zygomaticus, corrugator) and demonstrated atypical pre- and post-stimulus arousal. These findings may provide a potential mechanism for marked deficits in social reciprocity.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/fisiopatología , Emociones/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Motores/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Electromiografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos
15.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 89(3): 483-90, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23830881

RESUMEN

The ability to perceive emotions is imperative for successful interpersonal functioning. The present study examined the neural characteristics of emotional prosody perception with an exploratory event-related potential analysis. Participants were 59 healthy individuals who completed a discrimination task presenting 120 semantically neutral word pairs from five prosody conditions (happy/happy, angry/angry, neutral/neutral, angry/happy, happy/angry). The task required participants to determine whether words in the pair were spoken in same or different emotional prosody. Reflective of an initial processing stage, the word 1 N1 component was found to have greatest amplitude in parietal regions of the hemispheres, and was largest for emotional compared to neutral stimuli, indicating detection of emotion features. A second processing stage, represented by word 1 P2, showed similar topographic effects; however, amplitude was largest for happy in the left hemisphere while angry was largest in the right, illustrating differentiation of emotions. At the third processing stage, word 1 N3 amplitude was largest in frontal regions, indicating later cognitive processing occurs in the frontal cortex. N3 was largest for happy, which had lowest accuracy compared to angry and neutral. The present results support Schirmer and Kotz's (2006) model of vocal emotion perception because they elucidated the function and ERP components by reflecting three primary stages of emotional prosody perception, controlling for semantic influence.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Emociones/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Voz , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Modelos Lineales , Masculino , Análisis Multivariante , Tiempo de Reacción , Vocabulario , Adulto Joven
16.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 35(6): 655-68, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23799244

RESUMEN

It has been argued that higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) have specific deficits in advanced but not simple theory of mind (ToM), yet the questionable ecological validity of some tasks reduces the strength of this assumption. The present study employed The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT), which uses video vignettes to assess comprehension of subtle conversational inferences (sarcasm, lies/deception). Given the proposed relationships between advanced ToM and cognitive and affective empathy, these associations were also investigated. As expected, the high-functioning adults with ASDs demonstrated specific deficits in comprehending the beliefs, intentions, and meaning of nonliteral expressions. They also had significantly lower cognitive and affective empathy. Cognitive empathy was related to ToM and group membership whereas affective empathy was only related to group membership.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/diagnóstico , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/psicología , Empatía , Inteligencia , Teoría de la Mente , Adulto , Afecto , Concienciación , Niño , Comprensión , Señales (Psicología) , Cultura , Femenino , Humanos , Intención , Juicio , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Comunicación no Verbal , Semántica , Percepción del Habla , Conducta Verbal , Grabación en Video , Adulto Joven
17.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 89(3): 460-5, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23707301

RESUMEN

Severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) in adults is associated with abnormalities in arousal and emotional responsivity, which are observed physiologically, behaviourally and via self-report measures. While an accurate measure of physiological arousal is debated, Barry et al. (2005, 2007, 2008) have consistently shown an inverse relationship between skin conductance level (SCL), and mean alpha power (alpha) during an eyes-closed resting condition (EC), accompanied by an increase in SCL and corresponding decrease in alpha during eyes-open (EO). Thus, alpha may provide a novel index of autonomic arousal. This study aimed to elucidate the neural and autonomic correlates of arousal disturbances in TBI. Participants were 17 adults with TBI (13 males; mean age 46.50) and 22 matched controls (14 males; mean age 41.25). Mean alpha and SCL were recorded across two 2 minute conditions (EC and EO). Paralleling previous research (e.g., Barry et al., 2007), a significant decrease in alpha was found from EC to EO for the sample overall, but this was significantly reduced in TBI participants. Further, TBI participants showed diminished regional differences compared to controls. Lower SCLs across EC-EO were also found in TBI participants compared to controls. Contrasting expectations, an increase in SCL from EC to EO was not found. This study showed that examining simple alpha changes provides insight into TBI-related arousal disturbances. Importantly, our findings accord with the nature of TBI, which involves global and region-specific damage.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo alfa/fisiología , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Sistema Nervioso Autónomo/fisiología , Lesiones Encefálicas/fisiopatología , Mapeo Encefálico , Emociones/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Respuesta Galvánica de la Piel , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Análisis Multivariante , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Descanso
18.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 89(3): 475-82, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23628291

RESUMEN

Empirical research into behavioural profiles and autonomic responsivity in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) is highly variable and inconsistent. Two preliminary studies of children with ASDs suggest that there may be subgroups of ASDs depending on their resting arousal levels, and that these subgroups show different profiles of autonomic responsivity. The aim of the present study was to determine whether (i) adults with high-functioning ASDs may be separated into subgroups according to variation in resting arousal; and (ii) these ASD arousal subgroups differ in their behavioural profiles for basic emotion recognition, judgements of trustworthiness, and cognitive and affective empathy. Thirty high-functioning adults with ASDs and 34 non-clinical controls participated. Resting arousal was determined as the average skin conductance (SCL) across a 2 min resting period. There was a subgroup of ASD adults with significantly lower resting SCL. These individuals demonstrated poorer emotion recognition, tended to judge faces more negatively, and had atypical relationships between SCL and affective empathy. In contrast, low cognitive empathy was a feature of all ASD adults. These findings have important implications for clinical interventions and future studies investigating autonomic functioning in ASDs.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Sistema Nervioso Autónomo/fisiopatología , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/complicaciones , Trastornos del Conocimiento/etiología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Empatía/fisiología , Femenino , Respuesta Galvánica de la Piel , Humanos , Pruebas de Inteligencia , Juicio , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Psicofísica , Reconocimiento en Psicología
19.
Cortex ; 49(1): 101-6, 2013 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22030261

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Recent neuropsychological studies show substantial cognitive deficits in patients with frontotemporal dementia (FTD). Schizophrenia (SC) overlaps in terms of neurobehavioural symptoms with FTD. Probabilistic association learning, which is thought to assay fronto-striatal function, is well documented to elicit impairment in SC and has not been investigated in FTD to date; this study compared FTD, SC and a healthy comparison group on probabilistic association learning to determine the extent to which FTD patients were similar in performance to SC patients. METHODS: Twenty FTD patients, 24 SC patients and 26 healthy controls were assessed using the probabilistic association learning weather prediction test. FTD patients were also divided into behavioural and language variants for comparison to the healthy group. RESULTS: FTD patients were impaired during probabilistic association learning in comparison to healthy controls. There was no difference in performance between the FTD and SC groups. FTD behavioural variants performed significantly worse than the healthy comparison group, while FTD language variants did not differ from the healthy comparison group. CONCLUSIONS: This study provides the first evidence for impaired probabilistic association learning in FTD which is of an equivalent degree to that seen in SC. These results support recent structural neuroimaging studies showing fronto-striatal abnormalities in FTD and suggest that fronto-striatal dysfunction may contribute to cognitive deficits in a significant proportion of people with FTD.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Demencia Frontotemporal/psicología , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
20.
Physiol Behav ; 109: 14-22, 2013 Jan 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23142408

RESUMEN

Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) demonstrate atypical behavioural responses to affective stimuli, although the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. Investigating automatic responses to these stimuli may help elucidate these mechanisms. 18 high-functioning adults with ASDs and 18 typically developing controls viewed 54 extreme pleasant (erotica), extreme unpleasant (mutilations), and non-social neutral images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS). Two-thirds of images received an acoustic startle probe 3s post-picture onset. Facial electromyography (EMG) activity (orbicularis, zygomaticus, corrugator), skin conductance (SCR) and cardiac responses were recorded. The adults with ASDs demonstrated typical affective startle modulation and automatic facial EMG responses but atypical autonomic (SCRs and cardiac) responses, suggesting a failure to orient to, or a deliberate effort to disconnect from, socially relevant stimuli (erotica, mutilations). These results have implications for neural systems known to underlie affective processes, including the orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala.


Asunto(s)
Sistema Nervioso Autónomo/fisiopatología , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/fisiopatología , Emociones , Potenciales Evocados Motores/fisiología , Cara , Adolescente , Adulto , Preescolar , Electromiografía , Músculos Faciales/fisiopatología , Femenino , Respuesta Galvánica de la Piel/fisiología , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Humanos , Pruebas de Inteligencia , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Psicofísica , Reflejo de Sobresalto/fisiología , Autoevaluación (Psicología) , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA