Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 21
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 28(8): 757-768, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39111289

RESUMEN

People regularly encounter various types of conflict. Here, we ask if, and, if so, how, different types of conflict, from lab-based Stroop conflicts to everyday-life self-control or moral conflicts, are related to one other. We present a framework that assumes that action-goal representations are hierarchically organized, ranging from concrete actions to abstract goals. The framework's key assumption is that conflicts involving more abstract goals (e.g., self-control/moral conflict) are embedded in a more complex action space; thus, to resolve such conflicts, people need to consider more associated goals and actions. We discuss how differences in complexity impact conflict resolution mechanisms and the costs/benefits of resolving conflicts. Altogether, we offer a new way to conceptualize and analyze conflict regulation across different domains.


Asunto(s)
Conflicto Psicológico , Humanos , Autocontrol , Objetivos , Principios Morales , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología
2.
J Pers ; 2024 Mar 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38450535

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: We tested the hypothesis that complex behaviors are commonly supported by self-regulation strategies, even when those behaviors are supported by strong instigation habits. BACKGROUND: Goal-directed and habit-mediated processes arise from separable systems that have been suggested to seldomly interact. RESULTS: Self-regulation strategy use was lower for habitually instigated simple behaviors compared to nonhabitually instigated simple behaviors. However, participants' use of self-regulation strategies increased with the increasing complexity of behaviors, even when complex behaviors were habitually instigated. The difference in the extent of strategy use between habitually and nonhabitually instigated actions was absent when behavioral complexity was particularly high. CONCLUSION: These results point to a qualitative distinction-while simple behaviors may progress in a relatively automatic and unthinking manner, complex behaviors receive frequent support from self-regulation strategies, even if they are instigated habitually.

3.
Nat Commun ; 13(1): 3201, 2022 06 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35680874

RESUMEN

Self-regulation has been studied across levels of analysis; however, little attention has been paid to the extent to which self-report, neural, and behavioral indices predict goal pursuit in real-life. We use a mixed-method approach (N = 201) to triangulate evidence among established measures of different aspects of self-regulation to predict both the process of goal pursuit using experience sampling, as well as longer-term goal progress at 1, 3, and 6-month follow-ups. While self-reported trait self-control predicts goal attainment months later, we observe a null relationship between longitudinal goal attainment and ERPs associated with performance-monitoring and reactivity to positive/rewarding stimuli. Despite evidence that these ERPs are reliable and trait-like, and despite theorizing that suggests otherwise, our findings suggest that these ERPs are not meaningfully associated with everyday goal attainment. These findings challenge the ecological validity of brain measures thought to assess aspects of self-regulation.


Asunto(s)
Objetivos , Autocontrol , Atención , Encéfalo , Potenciales Evocados , Humanos
4.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 162: 112-120, 2021 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33529643

RESUMEN

Recent years have witnessed calls for increased rigour and credibility in the cognitive and behavioural sciences, including psychophysiology. Many procedures exist to increase rigour, and among the most important is the need to increase statistical power. Achieving sufficient statistical power, however, is a considerable challenge for resource intensive methodologies, particularly for between-subjects designs. Meta-analysis is one potential solution; yet, the validity of such quantitative review is limited by potential bias in both the primary literature and in meta-analysis itself. Here, we provide a non-technical overview and evaluation of open science methods that could be adopted to increase the transparency of novel meta-analyses. We also contrast post hoc statistical procedures that can be used to correct for publication bias in the primary literature. We suggest that traditional meta-analyses, as applied in ERP research, are exploratory in nature, providing a range of plausible effect sizes without necessarily having the ability to confirm (or disconfirm) existing hypotheses. To complement traditional approaches, we detail how prospective meta-analyses, combined with multisite collaboration, could be used to conduct statistically powerful, confirmatory ERP research.


Asunto(s)
Metaanálisis como Asunto , Psicofisiología , Sesgo , Humanos , Estudios Prospectivos , Sesgo de Publicación
5.
J Pers ; 89(4): 634-651, 2021 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33128774

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: What strategies do people use to resist desires in their day-to-day life? How effective are these strategies? Do people use different strategies for different desires? This study addresses these questions using experience sampling to examine strategy use in daily life. METHOD: Participants (N = 197, Mage  = 20.4, 63% female) reported on their use of six specific strategies (situation modification, distraction, reminding self of goals, promise to give in later, reminder of why it is bad, willpower) to resist desires (4,462 desires reported over a week). RESULTS: Participants reported using at least one strategy 89% of the time, and more than one strategy 25% of the time. Goal reminders and promises to give in later were more likely to be used for stronger desires. People also preferred different strategies for different types of desires (e.g., eating vs. leisure vs. work, etc.). CONCLUSION: In contrast to recent theoretical predictions, we find that many strategies, including inhibition, are similarly effective and that using multiple strategies is especially effective.


Asunto(s)
Motivación , Autocontrol , Adulto , Evaluación Ecológica Momentánea , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Prevalencia , Adulto Joven
6.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 155: 87-98, 2020 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32479773

RESUMEN

Many clinical neuroscience investigations have suggested that trait anxiety is associated with increased neural reactivity to mistakes in the form of an event-related potential called the error-related negativity (ERN). Several recent meta-analyses indicated that the anxiety-ERN association was of a small-to-medium effect size, however, these prior investigations did not comprehensively adjust effect sizes for publication bias. Here, in an updated meta-analysis (k = 58, N = 3819), we found support for an uncorrected effect size of r = -0.19, and applied a range of methods to test for and correct publication bias (trim-and-fill, PET, PEESE, Peters' test, three-parameter selection model). The majority of bias-correction methods suggested that the correlation between anxiety and the ERN is non-zero, but smaller than the uncorrected effect size (average adjusted effect size: r = -0.12, range: r = -0.05 to -0.18). Moderation analyses also revealed more robust effects for clinical anxiety and anxious samples characterised by worry, however, it should be noted that these larger effects were also associated with elevated indicators of publication bias relative to the overall analysis. Mixed anxiety and sub-clinical anxiety were not associated with the amplitude of the ERN. Our results suggest that the anxiety-ERN relationship survives multiple corrections for publication bias, albeit not among all sub-types and populations of anxiety. Nevertheless, only 50% of the studies included in our analysis reported significant results, indicating that future research exploring the anxiety-ERN relationship would benefit from increased statistical power.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Ansiedad , Trastornos de Ansiedad , Humanos , Sesgo de Publicación
7.
Psychol Sci ; 31(5): 531-547, 2020 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32315259

RESUMEN

People feel tired or depleted after exerting mental effort. But even preregistered studies often fail to find effects of exerting effort on behavioral performance in the laboratory or elucidate the underlying psychology. We tested a new paradigm in four preregistered within-subjects studies (N = 686). An initial high-demand task reliably elicited very strong effort phenomenology compared with a low-demand task. Afterward, participants completed a Stroop task. We used drift-diffusion modeling to obtain the boundary (response caution) and drift-rate (information-processing speed) parameters. Bayesian analyses indicated that the high-demand manipulation reduced boundary but not drift rate. Increased effort sensations further predicted reduced boundary. However, our demand manipulation did not affect subsequent inhibition, as assessed with traditional Stroop behavioral measures and additional diffusion-model analyses for conflict tasks. Thus, effort exertion reduced response caution rather than inhibitory control, suggesting that after exerting effort, people disengage and become uninterested in exerting further effort.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Ego , Fatiga , Autocontrol , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Atención , Teorema de Bayes , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Test de Stroop , Adulto Joven
8.
Cortex ; 109: 124-140, 2018 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30316113

RESUMEN

A growing body of work in social and affective neuroscience suggests that emotion plays an instrumental role in error monitoring processes, rather than only a moderating one. High-powered replications of studies that support this idea, however, are lacking. Here, we attempted a preregistered replication of our own study that had provided evidence for the functional role of emotions in error monitoring: that a neural signal of error monitoring-the error-related negativity-is reduced when participants undergo a misattribution of arousal procedure (Inzlicht & Al-Khindi, 2012). Like a previous replication attempt (Rodilla et al, 2016), our misattribution procedure failed to reduce the amplitude of the ERN. However, it also failed its manipulation check to reduce state anxiety, limiting the conclusions we can draw. Nonetheless, these findings are consistent with the view that our original study may have been a false positive. We discuss these findings in the context of the replication crisis in psychology and of work on the emotional properties of the ERN.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Humanos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
9.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 147(7): 1066-1077, 2018 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29565607

RESUMEN

Touch is central to mammalian communication, socialization, and wellbeing. Despite this prominence, interpersonal touch is relatively understudied. In this preregistered investigation, we assessed the influence of interpersonal touch on the subjective, neural, and behavioral correlates of cognitive control. Forty-five romantic couples were recruited (N = 90; dating >6 months), and one partner performed an inhibitory control task while electroencephalography was recorded to assess neural performance monitoring. Interpersonal touch was provided by the second partner and was manipulated between experimental blocks. A within-subject repeated-measures design was used to maximize statistical power, with our sample size providing 80% power for even small effect sizes (ds > .25). Results indicated that participants were not only happier when receiving touch, but also showed increased neural processing of mistakes. Further exploratory cognitive modeling using indirect effects tests and drift diffusion models of decision making revealed that touch was indirectly associated with both improved inhibitory control and increased rates of evidence accumulation (drift rate) through its influence on neural monitoring. Thus, beyond regulating emotion and stress, interpersonal touch appears to enhance the neurocognitive processes underling flexible goal-directed behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Parejas Sexuales/psicología , Tacto/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Encéfalo/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Emociones/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Adulto Joven
10.
Dermatol Online J ; 24(11)2018 Nov 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30695974

RESUMEN

Tattoos present a diagnostic challenge for dermatologists. Various reactions to tattoo have been identified in the literature ranging from allergic, to infectious, to neoplastic. Of the neoplastic cases identified, it is unclear whether the tattoo ink was directly causative, or if the cases were merely coincidence, as the number of cutaneous malignancies has also been on the rise. We present a novel case of two desmoplastic intradermal Spitz nevi arising within red tattoo ink.


Asunto(s)
Nevo de Células Epitelioides y Fusiformes/diagnóstico , Nevo Intradérmico/diagnóstico , Neoplasias Cutáneas/diagnóstico , Tatuaje , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Tinta , Nevo de Células Epitelioides y Fusiformes/patología , Nevo de Células Epitelioides y Fusiformes/cirugía , Nevo Intradérmico/patología , Nevo Intradérmico/cirugía , Neoplasias Cutáneas/patología , Neoplasias Cutáneas/cirugía
11.
Neuroimage ; 172: 838-852, 2018 05 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29107773

RESUMEN

Many everyday choices are based on personal, subjective preferences. When choosing between two options, we often feel conflicted, especially when trading off costs and benefits occurring at different times (e.g., saving for later versus spending now). Although previous work has investigated the neurophysiological basis of conflict during inhibitory control tasks, less is known about subjective conflict resulting from competing subjective preferences. In this pre-registered study, we investigated subjective conflict during intertemporal choice, whereby participants chose between smaller immediate versus larger delayed rewards (e.g., $15 today vs. $22 in 30 days). We used economic modeling to parametrically vary eleven different levels of conflict, and recorded EEG data and pupil dilation. Midfrontal theta power, derived from EEG, correlated with pupil responses, and our results suggest that these signals track different gradations of subjective conflict. Unexpectedly, both signals were also maximally enhanced when decisions were surprisingly easy. Therefore, these signals may track events requiring increased attention and adaptive shifts in behavioral responses, with subjective conflict being only one type of such event. Our results suggest that the neural systems underlying midfrontal theta and pupil responses interact when weighing costs and benefits during intertemporal choice. Thus, understanding these interactions might elucidate how individuals resolve self-control conflicts.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Conflicto Psicológico , Pupila/fisiología , Adolescente , Dilatación , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
12.
Psychophysiology ; 54(10): 1559-1573, 2017 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28621433

RESUMEN

Electromyographic activity over the corrugator supercilii (cEMG), the primary facial muscle involved in negative emotions, is increased during the commission of errors on speeded reaction-time tasks. In the present paper, data from two previously published studies were reanalyzed to investigate the reliability and stability of error-related, correct-related, and difference cEMG across increasing numbers of trials. For a modified go/no-go and a flanker task, we found that error-related cEMG was highly stable and reliable in 14 trials, and correct-related cEMG between 56 and 82 trials, respectively. Given the typical number of trials used in studies of cognitive control, these findings suggest that many investigations of error monitoring are already sufficient to obtain acceptable error- and correct-related cEMG signals. Error-related cEMG activity is relatively easy to measure and, as such, it shows great promise for future research investigating the cognitive and affective mechanisms of error monitoring.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva , Músculos Faciales/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Electromiografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Desempeño Psicomotor , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Adulto Joven
13.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 119: 31-40, 2017 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28088350

RESUMEN

The detection of conflict between incompatible impulses, thoughts, and actions is a ubiquitous source of motivation across theories of goal-directed action. In this overview, we explore the hypothesis that conflict is emotive, integrating perspectives from affective science and cognitive neuroscience. Initially, we review evidence suggesting that the mental and biological processes that monitor for information processing conflict-particularly those generated by the anterior midcingulate cortex-track the affective significance of conflict and use this signal to motivate increased control. In this sense, variation in control resembles a form of affect regulation in which control implementation counteracts the aversive experience of conflict. We also highlight emerging evidence proposing that states and dispositions associated with acceptance facilitate control by tuning individuals to the emotive nature of conflict, before proposing avenues for future research, including investigating the role of affect in reinforcement learning and decision making.


Asunto(s)
Conflicto Psicológico , Emociones/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Giro del Cíngulo/fisiología , Motivación/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Humanos
14.
Psychophysiology ; 53(2): 159-70, 2016 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26470645

RESUMEN

Emerging research in social and affective neuroscience has implicated a role for affect and motivation in performance monitoring and cognitive control. No study, however, has investigated whether facial electromyography (EMG) over the corrugator supercilii-a measure associated with negative affect and the exertion of effort-is related to neural performance monitoring. Here, we explored these potential relationships by simultaneously measuring the error-related negativity, error positivity (Pe), and facial EMG over the corrugator supercilii muscle during a punished, inhibitory control task. We found evidence for increased facial EMG activity over the corrugator immediately following error responses, and this activity was related to the Pe for both between- and within-subject analyses. These results are consistent with the idea that early, avoidance-motivated processes are associated with performance monitoring, and that such processes may also be related to orienting toward errors, the emergence of error awareness, or both.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Conflicto Psicológico , Músculos Faciales/fisiología , Adolescente , Afecto/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Electromiografía , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
15.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 16(1): 93-105, 2016 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26350627

RESUMEN

Mindfulness has been associated with enhanced performance monitoring; however, little is known about the processes driving this apparent neurocognitive benefit. Here, we tested whether focusing present-moment awareness toward the nonjudgmental experience of emotion facilitates rapid neural responses to negative performance outcomes (i.e., mistakes). In particular, we compared whether directing present-moment awareness toward emotions or thoughts would enhance the neurophysiological correlates of performance monitoring: the error-related negativity (ERN) and the error positivity (Pe). Participants were randomly assigned to either a thought-focused or an emotion-focused group, and first they completed a preinduction go/no-go task. Subsequently, the groups followed inductions that promoted mindful attention toward either thoughts or emotions, before completing a final postinduction go/no-go session. The results indicated that emotion-focused participants demonstrated higher neural sensitivity to errors in the time course of the ERN, whereas focusing on thoughts had no effect on performance monitoring. In contrast, neither induction procedure altered the amplitude of the later Pe component. Although our manipulations also induced changes in behavior, the ERN effects remained significant after controlling for performance. Thus, our results suggest that mindfulness meditation boosts early neural performance monitoring (ERN amplitude), specifically through meditation's influence on affective processing.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Concienciación/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Meditación/psicología , Adulto Joven
16.
PLoS One ; 10(12): e0143312, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26633830

RESUMEN

It has recently been suggested that gratitude can benefit self-regulation by reducing impulsivity during economic decision making. We tested if comparable benefits of gratitude are observed for neural performance monitoring and conflict-driven self-control. In a pre-post design, 61 participants were randomly assigned to either a gratitude or happiness condition, and then performed a pre-induction flanker task. Subsequently, participants recalled an autobiographical event where they had felt grateful or happy, followed by a post-induction flanker task. Despite closely following existing protocols, participants in the gratitude condition did not report elevated gratefulness compared to the happy group. In regard to self-control, we found no association between gratitude--operationalized by experimental condition or as a continuous predictor--and any control metric, including flanker interference, post-error adjustments, or neural monitoring (the error-related negativity, ERN). Thus, while gratitude might increase economic patience, such benefits may not generalize to conflict-driven control processes.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Conflicto Psicológico , Emociones/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Adolescente , Impulso (Psicología) , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Felicidad , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria Episódica , Adulto Joven
17.
Psychophysiology ; 52(9): 1205-17, 2015 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26041054

RESUMEN

Cognitive control is accompanied by observable negative affect. But how is this negative affect experienced subjectively, and are these feelings related to variation in cognitive control? To address these questions, 42 participants performed a punished inhibitory control task while periodically reporting their subjective experience. We found that within-subject variation in subjective experience predicted control implementation, but not neural monitoring (i.e., the error-related negativity, ERN). Specifically, anxiety and frustration predicted increased and decreased response caution, respectively, while hopelessness accompanied reduced inhibitory control, and subjective effort coincided with the increased ability to inhibit prepotent responses. Clarifying the nature of these phenomenological results, the effects of frustration, effort, and hopelessness-but not anxiety-were statistically independent from the punishment manipulation. Conversely, while the ERN was increased by punishment, the lack of association between this component and phenomenology suggests that early monitoring signals might precede the development of control-related subjective experience. Our results indicate that the types of feelings experienced during cognitively demanding tasks are related to different aspects of controlled performance, critically suggesting that the relationship between emotion and cognitive control extends beyond the dimension of valence.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Adolescente , Encéfalo/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
18.
Emotion ; 14(6): 1014-1026, 2014 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25286068

RESUMEN

Traditional models of cognitive control have explained performance monitoring as a "cold" cognitive process, devoid of emotion. In contrast to this dominant view, a growing body of clinical and experimental research indicates that cognitive control and its neural substrates, in particular the error-related negativity (ERN), are moderated by affective and motivational factors, reflecting the aversive experience of response conflict and errors. To add to this growing line of research, here we use the classic emotion regulation paradigm-a manipulation that promotes the cognitive reappraisal of emotion during task performance-to test the extent to which affective variation in the ERN is subject to emotion reappraisal, and also to explore how emotional regulation of the ERN might influence behavioral performance. In a within-subjects design, 41 university students completed 3 identical rounds of a go/no-go task while electroencephalography was recorded. Reappraisal instructions were manipulated so that participants either down-regulated or up-regulated emotional involvement, or completed the task normally, without engaging any reappraisal strategy (control). Results showed attenuated ERN amplitudes when participants down-regulated their emotional experience. In addition, a mediation analysis revealed that the association between reappraisal style and attenuated ERN was mediated by changes in reported emotion ratings. An indirect effects model also revealed that down-regulation predicted sensitivity of error-monitoring processes (difference ERN), which, in turn, predicted poorer task performance. Taken together, these results suggest that the ERN appears to have a strong affective component that is associated with indices of cognitive control and behavioral monitoring.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Regulación hacia Abajo , Emociones/fisiología , Adolescente , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adulto Joven
19.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 67(5): 884-98, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24070406

RESUMEN

The current research investigated differences in reactive and proactive cognitive control as a function of depressive symptomatology. Three participant groups with varying symptom levels (Beck Depression Inventory-II, BDI-II score) completed both the classic and an emotional-face Stroop task separately under speed and accuracy instructions. All groups made equivalent speed-accuracy trade-offs independent of task, suggesting that proactive adjustments are unaffected by depressive symptoms. Additionally, groups made equivalent reactive control adjustments (Stroop effects, congruency sequence effects) in the classic Stroop task, suggesting that these reactive control adjustments are spared across a wide range of BDI-II scorers. In contrast, the high BDI-II group displayed a selective impairment in the resolution of conflict in the emotional-face Stroop task. Thus, while proactive control and many aspects of reactive control were unaffected by the level of depressive symptoms, specific impairments occurred when current task demands required the trial-to-trial regulation of emotional processing.


Asunto(s)
Depresión/fisiopatología , Emociones , Cara , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Ajuste Social , Test de Stroop , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Adulto Joven
20.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 19(6): 1210-6, 2012 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22987148

RESUMEN

People tend to slow down after mistakes. This posterror slowing (PES) has commonly been explained by a change to a more conservative response threshold to avoid future errors. Alternatively, the attention-orienting account posits that all infrequent, surprising events (including errors) elicit an orienting response followed by a time-consuming process of task reorientation, explaining PES without increased response caution. In the present study, we employed both behavioral and electrophysiological measures to compare the predictions of these accounts using a flanker paradigm in which accurate or false external response feedback was provided. Participants demonstrated typical posterror adjustments, responding more slowly and accurately in posterror than in postcorrect trials. This finding provides initial evidence suggesting that posterror adjustments are motivated by the avoidance of subsequent mistakes. Most importantly, PES and an event-related potential relating to the attentional processing of feedback, the feedback-related P300 (f-P300), were modulated by feedback type. More specifically, the f-P300 was larger after false than after accurate feedback, suggesting that participants oriented their attention toward (i.e., were surprised by) inaccurate feedback signals. Interestingly, false feedback differentially modulated reaction times: Participants were slower after correct responses when feedback falsely informed of an error rather than confirmed the correct response. In contrast, faster responses were made after errors when feedback falsely indicated correct rather than incorrect performance. When these patterns of results are regarded together, they are best explained by theories of cognitive control in which posterror adjustments in choice reaction time tasks are assumed to reflect control processes leading to more conservative performance after error signals.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Retroalimentación Psicológica/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA