Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 12 de 12
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Elife ; 122023 08 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37555830

RESUMEN

Human neuroscience has always been pushing the boundary of what is measurable. During the last decade, concerns about statistical power and replicability - in science in general, but also specifically in human neuroscience - have fueled an extensive debate. One important insight from this discourse is the need for larger samples, which naturally increases statistical power. An alternative is to increase the precision of measurements, which is the focus of this review. This option is often overlooked, even though statistical power benefits from increasing precision as much as from increasing sample size. Nonetheless, precision has always been at the heart of good scientific practice in human neuroscience, with researchers relying on lab traditions or rules of thumb to ensure sufficient precision for their studies. In this review, we encourage a more systematic approach to precision. We start by introducing measurement precision and its importance for well-powered studies in human neuroscience. Then, determinants for precision in a range of neuroscientific methods (MRI, M/EEG, EDA, Eye-Tracking, and Endocrinology) are elaborated. We end by discussing how a more systematic evaluation of precision and the application of respective insights can lead to an increase in reproducibility in human neuroscience.


Asunto(s)
Neurociencias , Humanos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Tamaño de la Muestra , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética
2.
Brain Topogr ; 2023 Jul 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37523005

RESUMEN

Social interactions require both the rapid processing of multifaceted socio-affective signals (e.g., eye gaze, facial expressions, gestures) and their integration with evaluations, social knowledge, and expectations. Researchers interested in understanding complex social cognition and behavior face a "black box" problem: What are the underlying mental processes rapidly occurring between perception and action and why are there such vast individual differences? In this review, we promote electroencephalography (EEG) microstates as a powerful tool for both examining socio-affective states (e.g., processing whether someone is in need in a given situation) and identifying the sources of heterogeneity in socio-affective traits (e.g., general willingness to help others). EEG microstates are identified by analyzing scalp field maps (i.e., the distribution of the electrical field on the scalp) over time. This data-driven, reference-independent approach allows for identifying, timing, sequencing, and quantifying the activation of large-scale brain networks relevant to our socio-affective mind. In light of these benefits, EEG microstates should become an indispensable part of the methodological toolkit of laboratories working in the field of social and affective neuroscience.

3.
Clin Psychol Rev ; 98: 102212, 2022 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36371900

RESUMEN

Expectations are a central maintaining mechanism in mental disorders and most psychological treatments aim to directly or indirectly modify clinically relevant expectations. Therefore, it is crucial to examine why patients with mental disorders maintain dysfunctional expectations, even in light of disconfirming evidence, and how expectation-violating situations should be created in treatment settings to optimize treatment outcome and reduce the risk of treatment failures. The different psychological subdisciplines offer various approaches for understanding the underlying mechanisms of expectation development, persistence, and change. Here, we convey recommendations on how to improve psychological treatments by considering these different perspectives. Based on our expectation violation model, we argue that the outcome of expectation violation depends on several characteristics: features of the expectation-violating situation; the dynamics between the magnitude of expectation violation and cognitive immunization processes; dealing with uncertainties during and after expectation change; controlled and automatic attention processes; and the costs of expectation changes. Personality factors further add to predict outcomes and may offer a basis for personalized treatment planning. We conclude with a list of recommendations derived from basic psychology that could contribute to improved treatment outcome and to reduced risks of treatment failures.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Mentales , Motivación , Humanos , Trastornos Mentales/terapia , Atención
4.
Int J Neuropsychopharmacol ; 25(9): 759-773, 2022 09 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35748393

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Hyperconsolidation of aversive associations and poor extinction learning have been hypothesized to be crucial in the acquisition of pathological fear. Previous animal and human research points to the potential role of the catecholaminergic system, particularly noradrenaline and dopamine, in acquiring emotional memories. Here, we investigated in a between-participants design with 3 groups whether the noradrenergic alpha-2 adrenoreceptor antagonist yohimbine and the dopaminergic D2-receptor antagonist sulpiride modulate long-term fear conditioning and extinction in humans. METHODS: Fifty-five healthy male students were recruited. The final sample consisted of n = 51 participants who were explicitly aware of the contingencies between conditioned stimuli (CS) and unconditioned stimuli after fear acquisition. The participants were then randomly assigned to 1 of the 3 groups and received either yohimbine (10 mg, n = 17), sulpiride (200 mg, n = 16), or placebo (n = 18) between fear acquisition and extinction. Recall of conditioned (non-extinguished CS+ vs CS-) and extinguished fear (extinguished CS+ vs CS-) was assessed 1 day later, and a 64-channel electroencephalogram was recorded. RESULTS: The yohimbine group showed increased salivary alpha-amylase activity, confirming a successful manipulation of central noradrenergic release. Elevated fear-conditioned bradycardia and larger differential amplitudes of the N170 and late positive potential components in the event-related brain potential indicated that yohimbine treatment (compared with a placebo and sulpiride) enhanced fear recall during day 2. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that yohimbine potentiates cardiac and central electrophysiological signatures of fear memory consolidation. They thereby elucidate the key role of noradrenaline in strengthening the consolidation of conditioned fear associations, which may be a key mechanism in the etiology of fear-related disorders.


Asunto(s)
Antagonistas de Receptores Adrenérgicos alfa 2/farmacología , Extinción Psicológica , alfa-Amilasas Salivales , Dopamina , Miedo , Humanos , Masculino , Norepinefrina/fisiología , Receptores Adrenérgicos alfa 2/metabolismo , alfa-Amilasas Salivales/farmacología , Sulpirida/farmacología , Yohimbina/farmacología
5.
Front Psychol ; 12: 726432, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34858264

RESUMEN

Expectations are probabilistic beliefs about the future that shape and influence our perception, affect, cognition, and behavior in many contexts. This makes expectations a highly relevant concept across basic and applied psychological disciplines. When expectations are confirmed or violated, individuals can respond by either updating or maintaining their prior expectations in light of the new evidence. Moreover, proactive and reactive behavior can change the probability with which individuals encounter expectation confirmations or violations. The investigation of predictors and mechanisms underlying expectation update and maintenance has been approached from many research perspectives. However, in many instances there has been little exchange between different research fields. To further advance research on expectations and expectation violations, collaborative efforts across different disciplines in psychology, cognitive (neuro)science, and other life sciences are warranted. For fostering and facilitating such efforts, we introduce the ViolEx 2.0 model, a revised framework for interdisciplinary research on cognitive and behavioral mechanisms of expectation update and maintenance in the context of expectation violations. To support different goals and stages in interdisciplinary exchange, the ViolEx 2.0 model features three model levels with varying degrees of specificity in order to address questions about the research synopsis, central concepts, or functional processes and relationships, respectively. The framework can be applied to different research fields and has high potential for guiding collaborative research efforts in expectation research.

6.
J Affect Disord ; 295: 131-138, 2021 12 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34438321

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: To investigate the powerful placebo effects in antidepressant drug trials and their mechanisms, recent pioneering experimental studies showed that expectation manipulation combined with an active placebo attenuated induced sadness. In the present study, we aimed at extending these findings by assessing the psychophysiological response in addition to mere self-report. METHODS: One hundred and thirteen healthy female students were randomly assigned to a drug expectation group (active placebo, positive treatment expectation), placebo expectation group (active placebo, no treatment expectation), or a no-treatment group (no placebo, no treatment expectation). After placebo intake, sadness was induced by self-deprecating statements using the Velten method combined with sad music, including a rumination phase. Sadness was measured using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Expanded Form (PANAS-X). Heart rate and skin conductance were assessed continuously. RESULTS: After mood induction and after rumination, self-reported sadness was significantly lower, and skin conductance level was significantly higher, in the drug expectation group than in the no-treatment group. The mood induction was further accompanied by a heart rate deceleration within all groups. LIMITATIONS: Generalizability is limited by sample selectivity and focusing on sadness as a symptom of depression, exclusively. CONCLUSION: Expectation-induced placebo effects significantly influenced sadness-correlated changes in autonomic arousal, and not only subjectively reported sadness, indicating that placebo effects in the context of affect are not merely due to subjective response bias. The systematic modification of treatment expectation could be utilized in clinical practice to optimize current therapeutic approaches to improve mood regulation.


Asunto(s)
Rociadores Nasales , Tristeza , Afecto , Nivel de Alerta , Sistema Nervioso Autónomo , Femenino , Humanos
7.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33711549

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Emerging human studies demonstrate that theta oscillations in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex are enhanced during fear recall (enhanced fear expression) and reduced during successful extinction recall (reduced fear expression). Although evidence suggests sex differences in fear recall and extinction recall, there are currently no human studies examining the oscillatory foundations of these memory processes separately in men and women. METHODS: Because previous studies suggest that estradiol partially mediates these sex differences, we examined 20 men (low estradiol and low progesterone), 20 women using oral contraceptives (low estradiol and low progesterone), and 20 free-cycling women during midcycle (high estradiol and low progesterone). We used a fear-conditioning procedure, allowing us to separately assess fear recall and extinction recall 24 hours after fear and extinction learning. Skin conductance responses and electroencephalography were recorded during fear recall and extinction recall, and prefrontal oscillations were source localized. RESULTS: We found elevated fear expression during fear recall and impaired extinction recall, as indicated by increased peripheral arousal (skin conductance responses) and fronto-central theta oscillations, source localized in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Importantly, peripheral arousal and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex theta oscillations were stronger in men and women on oral contraceptives than in women from the midcycle group. CONCLUSIONS: Our data show that neural oscillatory and peripheral correlates of heightened fear expression during fear recall and (impaired) extinction recall do not simply differ between sexes but depend on hormonal fluctuations within women.


Asunto(s)
Estradiol , Extinción Psicológica , Recuerdo Mental , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Ritmo Teta , Estradiol/fisiología , Miedo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
Neuroimage ; 226: 117569, 2021 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33221446

RESUMEN

Electrophysiological studies in rodents allow recording neural activity during threats with high temporal and spatial precision. Although fMRI has helped translate insights about the anatomy of underlying brain circuits to humans, the temporal dynamics of neural fear processes remain opaque and require EEG. To date, studies on electrophysiological brain signals in humans have helped to elucidate underlying perceptual and attentional processes, but have widely ignored how fear memory traces evolve over time. The low signal-to-noise ratio of EEG demands aggregations across high numbers of trials, which will wash out transient neurobiological processes that are induced by learning and prone to habituation. Here, our goal was to unravel the plasticity and temporal emergence of EEG responses during fear conditioning. To this end, we developed a new sequential-set fear conditioning paradigm that comprises three successive acquisition and extinction phases, each with a novel CS+/CS- set. Each set consists of two different neutral faces on different background colors which serve as CS+ and CS-, respectively. Thereby, this design provides sufficient trials for EEG analyses while tripling the relative amount of trials that tap into more transient neurobiological processes. Consistent with prior studies on ERP components, data-driven topographic EEG analyses revealed that ERP amplitudes were potentiated during time periods from 33-60 ms, 108-200 ms, and 468-820 ms indicating that fear conditioning prioritizes early sensory processing in the brain, but also facilitates neural responding during later attentional and evaluative stages. Importantly, averaging across the three CS+/CS- sets allowed us to probe the temporal evolution of neural processes: Responses during each of the three time windows gradually increased from early to late fear conditioning, while long-latency (460-730 ms) electrocortical responses diminished throughout fear extinction. Our novel paradigm demonstrates how short-, mid-, and long-latency EEG responses change during fear conditioning and extinction, findings that enlighten the learning curve of neurophysiological responses to threat in humans.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Condicionamiento Clásico/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Miedo , Adolescente , Adulto , Condicionamiento Psicológico , Electroencefalografía , Extinción Psicológica , Femenino , Voluntarios Sanos , Humanos , Masculino , Vías Nerviosas , Plasticidad Neuronal , Adulto Joven
9.
Psychol Sci ; 30(7): 1001-1015, 2019 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31150589

RESUMEN

In classical fear conditioning, neutral conditioned stimuli that have been paired with aversive physical unconditioned stimuli eventually trigger fear responses. Here, we tested whether aversive mental images systematically paired with a conditioned stimulus also cause de novo fear learning in the absence of any external aversive stimulation. In two experiments (N = 45 and N = 41), participants were first trained to produce aversive, neutral, or no imagery in response to three different visual-imagery cues. In a subsequent imagery-based differential-conditioning paradigm, each of the three cues systematically coterminated with one of three different neutral faces. Although the face that was paired with the aversive-imagery cue was never paired with aversive external stimuli or threat-related instructions, participants rated it as more arousing, unpleasant, and threatening and displayed relative fear bradycardia and fear-potentiated startle. These results could be relevant for the development of fear and related disorders without trauma.


Asunto(s)
Condicionamiento Clásico/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Miedo/fisiología , Imaginación/fisiología , Reflejo de Sobresalto/fisiología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
10.
Cereb Cortex ; 29(2): 701-715, 2019 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29373635

RESUMEN

Human functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalography (EEG) studies, as well as animal studies, indicate that the amygdala and frontomedial brain regions are critically involved in conditioned fear and that frontomedial oscillations in the theta range (4-8 Hz) may support communication between these brain regions. However, few studies have used a multimodal approach to probe interactions among these key regions in humans. Here, our goal was to bridge the gap between prior human fMRI, EEG, and animal findings. Using simultaneous EEG-fMRI recordings 24 h after fear conditioning and extinction, conditioned stimuli presented (CS+E, CS-E) and not presented during extinction (CS+N, CS-N) were compared to identify effects specific to extinction versus fear recall. Differential (CS+ vs. CS-) electrodermal, frontomedial theta (EEG) and amygdala responses (fMRI) were reduced for extinguished versus nonextinguished stimuli. Importantly, effects on theta power covaried with effects on amygdala activation. Fear and extinction recall as indicated by theta explained 60% of the variance for the analogous effect in the right amygdala. Our findings show for the first time the interplay of amygdala and frontomedial theta activity during fear and extinction recall in humans and provide insight into neural circuits consistently linked with top-down amygdala modulation in rodents.


Asunto(s)
Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiología , Extinción Psicológica/fisiología , Miedo/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Ritmo Teta/fisiología , Adulto , Amígdala del Cerebelo/diagnóstico por imagen , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Miedo/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Corteza Prefrontal/diagnóstico por imagen , Distribución Aleatoria , Adulto Joven
11.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 155: 7-20, 2018 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29883709

RESUMEN

Individual differences in long-term stability of fear memories are of potential relevance for stable dispositions related to threat processing, such as neuroticism/anxiety and fearfulness. As previous research suggests a prominent role of dopamine for the retention of conditioned and extinguished fear, dopaminergic gene polymorphisms may also relate to individual differences in fear stability. While the COMT Val158Met polymorphism causes individual differences in prefrontal dopamine, its associations with human long-term fear extinction are currently unknown. Here, n = 30/29/28 healthy male Val/Val, Val/Met and Met/Met carriers, respectively, underwent a two-day differential conditioning paradigm with fear acquisition and extinction on Day 1 and a recall test on Day 2 with recordings of EEG and ECG. Fearfulness but not neuroticism/anxiety predicted fear bradycardia (i.e., heart period slowing) during Day 1 fear acquisition while it did not affect extinction or Day 2 fear recall. In contrast, COMT Val158Met significantly modulated Day 2 fear recall as evident in fear bradycardia and Late Positive Potential (LPP) amplitudes while it did not affect Day 1 fear or extinction learning. Furthermore, exploratory analyses revealed that individual differences in fear bradycardia during Day 2 extinction recall depended on Day 1 extinction success. Importantly, this contingency was (a) modulated by COMT Val158Met and (b) significantly reduced in high vs. low neuroticism/anxiety. The present study indicates that (a) individual differences in dopaminergic genotypes may affect the long-term stability of fear memories and (b) fearfulness vs. neuroticism/anxiety might play distinct roles in initial fear reactions vs. long-term stability of fear memories, respectively.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Bradicardia/fisiopatología , Catecol O-Metiltransferasa/genética , Condicionamiento Clásico/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Extinción Psicológica/fisiología , Miedo/fisiología , Neuroticismo/fisiología , Personalidad/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Ansiedad/genética , Electrocardiografía , Electroencefalografía , Respuesta Galvánica de la Piel/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Personalidad/genética , Adulto Joven
12.
Psychophysiology ; 53(9): 1352-65, 2016 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27286734

RESUMEN

Several methods that are promising for studying the neurophysiology of fear conditioning (e.g., EEG, MEG) require a high number of trials to achieve an adequate signal-to-noise ratio. While electric shock and white noise burst are among the most commonly used unconditioned stimuli (US) in conventional fear conditioning studies with few trials, it is unknown whether these stimuli are equally well suited for paradigms with many trials. Here, N = 32 participants underwent a 260-trial differential fear conditioning and extinction paradigm with a 240-trial recall test 24 h later and neutral faces as conditioned stimuli. In a between-subjects design, either white noise bursts (n = 16) or electric shocks (n = 16) served as US, and intensities were determined using the most common procedure for each US (i.e., a fixed 95 dB noise burst and a work-up procedure for electric shocks, respectively). In addition to differing US types, groups also differed in closely linked US-associated characteristics (e.g., calibration methods, stimulus intensities, timing). Subjective ratings (arousal/valence), skin conductance, and evoked heart period changes (i.e., fear bradycardia) indicated more reliable, extinction-resistant, and stable conditioning in the white noise burst versus electric shock group. In fear conditioning experiments where many trials are presented, white noise burst should serve as US.


Asunto(s)
Investigación Conductal/métodos , Condicionamiento Clásico/fisiología , Electrochoque , Extinción Psicológica/fisiología , Miedo/fisiología , Ruido , Proyectos de Investigación , Adulto , Investigación Conductal/normas , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Proyectos de Investigación/normas , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...