Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 13 de 13
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Curr Opin Psychol ; 35: 65-70, 2020 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32361404

RESUMEN

We argue that dramatic social change arising from collective action does not represent the worst-case scenario for group members. Specifically, we introduce the concept of collective inertia: a societal state where group members face a macro system devoid of clearly articulated collective goals and values coupled with dysfunctional social and normative structures. Our analysis emphasizes the dynamic process of social change, involving a shift from one societal state to another. Collective inertia is the one societal state that may have devastating long-term consequences for groups.


Asunto(s)
Cambio Social , Humanos
2.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 119(3): 600-632, 2020 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31566394

RESUMEN

Groups experience collective autonomy restriction whenever they perceive that other groups attempt to limit the freedom of their group to determine and express its own identity. We argue that collective autonomy restriction motivates groups (both structurally advantaged and disadvantaged) to improve their power position within the social hierarchy. Four studies spanning real-world (Studies 1 and 2) and lab-based (Studies 3 and 4) intergroup contexts supported these ideas. In Study 1 (N = 311), Black Americans' (a relatively disadvantaged group) experience of collective autonomy restriction was associated with greater support for collective action, and less system justification. In Study 2, we replicated these findings with another sample of Black Americans (N = 292). We also found that collective autonomy restriction was positively associated with White Americans' (a relatively advantaged group, N = 294) support for collective action and ideologies that bolster White's dominant position. In Study 3 (N = 387, 97 groups), groups that were susceptible to being controlled by a high-power group (i.e., were of low structural power) desired group power more when their collective autonomy was restricted (vs. supported). In Study 4 (N = 803, 257 groups) experiencing collective autonomy restriction (vs. support) increased low-power group members' support of collective action, decreased system justification, and evoked hostile emotions, both when groups were and were not materially exploited (by being tasked with more than their fair share of work). Across studies, we differentiate collective autonomy restriction from structural group power, other forms of injustice, group agency, and group identification. These findings indicate that collective autonomy restriction uniquely motivates collective behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Conducta Cooperativa , Procesos de Grupo , Motivación , Autonomía Personal , Identificación Social , Adulto , Negro o Afroamericano , Femenino , Jerarquia Social , Humanos , Masculino , Población Blanca
3.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 116(1): 141-165, 2019 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29323507

RESUMEN

Four studies assessed the potentially detrimental effects that restrictions to collective autonomy (i.e., a group's freedom to determine and practice its own identity) may have for the personal autonomy and psychological well-being of group members. In Study 1, using 3 distinct samples (NSample1a = 123, NSample1b = 129, NSample1c = 370), correlational and cross-cultural evidence indicates that perceived restrictions to the collective autonomy of one's group is directly associated with reduced personal autonomy, and indirectly associated with diminished well-being through personal autonomy. In Study 2 (N = 411), a longitudinal assessment of group members over 3 time-points during a 4-month period found that group members who perceived greater collective autonomy restriction also experienced reduced personal autonomy, and in turn, reduced psychological well-being over time. In Study 3 (N = 255), group members described a time during which their ingroup had (or did not have) its collective autonomy unduly restricted by other groups. Participants who were primed to think that their group lacked collective autonomy reported reduced feelings of personal autonomy, and reduced psychological well-being (compared with those primed to think their group had collective autonomy). In Study 4 (N = 389), collective autonomy was manipulated within the context of an intensive laboratory simulation. Collective autonomy-restricted group members experienced less personal autonomy than those who did not have their collective autonomy restricted. Together these findings suggest that restrictions to a group's collective autonomy may have detrimental consequences for the personal autonomy and psychological well-being of group members. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Depresivo/psicología , Procesos de Grupo , Autonomía Personal , Satisfacción Personal , Autoimagen , Identificación Social , Adulto , Comparación Transcultural , Estudios Transversales , Trastorno Depresivo/epidemiología , Femenino , Humanos , India/epidemiología , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Quebec/epidemiología , Estados Unidos/epidemiología , Adulto Joven
4.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 44(12): 1725-1738, 2018 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29877130

RESUMEN

Because of stigma and underrepresentation, many ethnic minority students may find it difficult to align their ethnicities with their ideal selves. However, these difficulties and their potential consequences have been empirically neglected. To inform this gap in the literature, we propose that the novel concept of ethnic/ideal self-discrepancies (i.e., perceived mismatches between who a person aspires to be and this person's conception of their ethnic self) is associated with the academic outcomes of ethnic minority students. As hypothesized, large ethnic/ideal self-discrepancies predict high academic disengagement, according to cross-sectional data from Study 1 ( n = 147) and Study 2 ( n = 105), as well as high academic disengagement 2 months later according to half-longitudinal data from Study 2 ( n = 78). In Study 3 ( n = 99), ethnic minority students experimentally induced to perceive high ethnic/ideal self-discrepancies reported significantly higher academic disengagement than ethnic minority students in a low discrepancy condition.


Asunto(s)
Rendimiento Académico/psicología , Etnicidad/psicología , Autoimagen , Identificación Social , Estudiantes/psicología , Rendimiento Académico/etnología , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudios Transversales , Etnicidad/estadística & datos numéricos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Autoeficacia , Percepción Social , Estigma Social , Adulto Joven
5.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 40(5): 617-32, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24501046

RESUMEN

The role of reference group norms in self-regulation was examined from the perspective of transgressions. Results from four studies suggest that following the transgression of a reference group's norms, individuals who strongly identify with their group report more intense feelings of guilt, an emotion reflecting an inference that "bad" behaviors are perceived as the cause of the transgression. Conversely, weakly identified individuals reported more intense feelings of shame, an emotion reflecting an inference that "bad" characteristics of the person are perceived as the cause of the transgression. The studies also explored the differential relevance of the reference groups when assessing transgressive behaviors, the counterfactual thoughts individuals have about possible causes for the transgressions, and the motivational outcomes of guilt and shame using behavioral data. Results of the studies offer insights into self-regulation, maintenance of group norms, and offer implications for alcohol consumption interventions, such as social marketing campaigns.


Asunto(s)
Consumo Excesivo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/psicología , Vergüenza , Identificación Social , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
6.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 105(6): 978-95, 2013 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23937644

RESUMEN

In this research, we investigated how group power influences the way members of groups in asymmetrical conflict approach intergroup negotiations. Drawing on theories of negotiations and of intergroup power, we predicted that group power would interact with features of the proposed negotiating agenda to influence willingness to come to the table. Based on the negotiation literature, we focused on 2 types of sequential negotiation agendas: 1 beginning with the discussion of consequential issues before less consequential issues (consequential first) and 1 leaving the discussion of consequential issues until after less consequential issues are discussed (consequential later). Because they are motivated to advance changes to their disadvantaged status quo, we expected low-power group members to favor consequential first over consequential later invitations to negotiate. High-power group members, motivated to protect their advantage, were expected to show the reverse preference. Converging evidence from 5 experiments involving real-world and experimental groups supported these predictions. Across studies, participants received an invitation to negotiate from the other group involving either a consequential first or consequential later agenda. Low-power group members preferred consequential first invitations because these implied less stalling of change to the status quo, and high-power group members preferred consequential later invitations because these invitations seemed to pose less threat to their position. Theoretical and practical implications for negotiations research and conflict resolution are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Conflicto Psicológico , Negociación/psicología , Poder Psicológico , Conducta Social , Adulto , Árabes/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Israel , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
7.
Br J Soc Psychol ; 52(4): 703-25, 2013 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23013238

RESUMEN

Dramatic social change involves profound transformations that impact an entire group moving forward. Such is the reality for race relations in South Africa. Research has found that most people report a trajectory of group-based relative deprivation that appears to parallel actual historical events. However, a significant subset of respondents reported a trajectory in which the perceived status of their group remained stable despite dramatic social change. The first goal of our research was to assess whether both the historically 'assumed' and 'stable' group trajectories arise consistently among South Africans (N = 2,989). The second and more important goal was to identify the factors that might account for this dichotomy in perceived trajectory building on both traditional and recent advances in relative deprivation theory as well as on social identity theory. We hypothesized that higher levels of in-group identification would be associated with the historically assumed group trajectory. Results supported this hypothesis. The third goal was to link the different group trajectories with important psychological outcomes such as personal well-being, group self-esteem, and interracial attitudes.


Asunto(s)
Carencia Psicosocial , Cambio Social , Identificación Social , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Autoimagen , Sudáfrica
8.
Br J Soc Psychol ; 50(4): 726-46, 2011 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22122029

RESUMEN

Responding to calls to contextualize social psychological variables in history, the present research examines the relationship between collective relative deprivation and collective esteem using a historical perspective. We hypothesized that collective relative deprivation perceived to be experienced during an important low-point in a group's history serves to define the group's current collective identity, which is in turn associated with collective esteem. In Study 1, cultural narrative interviews were conducted with Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers in order to identify key historical chapters for these groups and to examine the extent to which historical low-points were identity-defining features of their narratives. In Study 2, using the information obtained from these narratives, collective relative deprivation was explored across group members' perceived histories and related to current in-group entitativity and collective esteem. The relationship between collective relative deprivation thought to be experienced by one's group during a historical low-point and collective esteem was positive for both Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers and was mediated by in-group entitativity. Collective relative deprivation perceived to be experienced during a historical low-point serves to define one's collective identity, which is in turn associated with greater collective esteem.


Asunto(s)
Etnicidad/historia , Carencia Psicosocial , Autoimagen , Identificación Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Historia del Siglo XVIII , Historia del Siglo XIX , Humanos , Entrevistas como Asunto , Masculino , Quebec , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
9.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 36(7): 883-97, 2010 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20519575

RESUMEN

Knowing oneself and experiencing oneself as clearly defined has been linked to positive self-esteem and psychological well-being; however, this association has been tested only at the level of personal identity. The authors propose that a clear cultural identity provides the individual with a clear prototype with which to engage the processes necessary to construct a clear personal identity and, by extension, to achieve self-esteem and well-being. For samples of undergraduate students, Anglophone Quebecers, Francophone Québécois, Chinese North Americans, and Aboriginal Canadians, cultural identity clarity was positively related to self-concept clarity, self-esteem, and markers of subjective well-being. The relationship between cultural identity clarity and both self-esteem and well-being was consistently mediated by self-concept clarity. Interventions designed to clarify cultural identity might have psychological benefits for individuals facing cultural identity challenges.


Asunto(s)
Autoimagen , Identificación Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Canadá , China/etnología , Cultura , Femenino , Humanos , Indígenas Norteamericanos/psicología , Masculino , Salud Mental , Persona de Mediana Edad , Inventario de Personalidad , Pruebas Psicológicas , Quebec , Adulto Joven
10.
Transcult Psychiatry ; 47(1): 93-111, 2010 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20511254

RESUMEN

Collective trauma, be it through colonization (e.g., Aboriginal Peoples), slavery (e.g., African Americans) or war, has a dramatic impact on the psychological well-being of each and every individual member of the collective. Thus, interventions are often conceptualized and delivered at the individual level with a view to minimizing the psychological disequilibrium of each individual. In contrast, we propose a theory of self that emphasizes the primacy of cultural identity for psychological well-being. We present a series of studies that illustrate the importance of cultural identity clarity for personal identity and for psychological well-being. Our theoretical model proposes that interventions aimed at clarifying cultural identity may play a constructive role in the promotion of the well-being of group members exposed to collective trauma.


Asunto(s)
Conflicto Psicológico , Diversidad Cultural , Etnicidad/psicología , Autoimagen , Identificación Social , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/etnología , Aculturación , Adaptación Psicológica , Canadá , China/etnología , Humanos , Calidad de Vida/psicología , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/psicología , Terrorismo/psicología
11.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 91(6): 1032-44, 2006 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17144763

RESUMEN

It has long been established that there is a linear and positive relationship between relative deprivation and prejudice. However, a recent experiment suggests that the converse of relative deprivation, relative gratification, may also be associated with prejudice (S. Guimond & M. Dambrun, 2002). Specifically, the evidence suggests that the usual test for a linear relationship between relative deprivation-gratification and prejudice might conceal the existence of a bilinear relationship. This function, labeled the V-curve hypothesis, predicts that both relative deprivation and relative gratification are associated with higher levels of prejudice. This hypothesis was tested with a representative sample of South Africans (N=1,600). Results provide strong support for the V-curve hypothesis. Furthermore, strength of ethnic identification emerged as a partial mediator for the effect of relative gratification on prejudice.


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Población Negra/psicología , Emigración e Inmigración , Satisfacción Personal , Prejuicio , Carencia Psicosocial , Opinión Pública , Identificación Social , Población Blanca/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Recolección de Datos , Femenino , Hostilidad , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Percepción Social , Factores Socioeconómicos , Sudáfrica
12.
J Psychol ; 136(4): 371-82, 2002 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12230140

RESUMEN

The personal/group discrimination discrepancy involves disadvantaged group members rating discrimination directed at their group considerably higher than ratings of discrimination aimed at themselves personally as members of that group. This robust phenomenon has been found in samples of women, African Americans, and aboriginal people. In the present study, the authors used a sample of Inuit from a remote Arctic community to confirm the perceived discrepancy. However, ratings for perceived group discrimination were surprisingly low. The authors argue that geographical isolation may have led Inuit to be unaware of the impact of discrimination on their lives. In support of this argument, findings showed that group discrimination ratings were higher for Inuit who did have contact with mainstream Canadian culture. Implications for the traditional contact hypothesis are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Inuk/psicología , Grupos Minoritarios/psicología , Prejuicio , Aislamiento Social , Percepción Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Canadá , Empleo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Teoría Psicológica
13.
Anesthesiology ; 96(1): 189-95, 2002 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11753020

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: General anesthetics reduce neuronal death caused by focal cerebral ischemia in rodents and by in vitro ischemia in cultured neurons and brain slices. However, in intact animals, the protective effect may enhance neuronal survival for only several days after an ischemic injury, possibly because anesthetics prevent acute but not delayed cell death. To further understand the mechanisms and limitations of volatile anesthetic neuroprotection, the authors developed a rat hippocampal slice culture model of cerebral ischemia that permits assessment of death and survival of neurons for at least 2 weeks after simulated ischemia. METHODS: Survival of CA1, CA3, and dentate gyrus neurons in cultured hippocampal slices (organotypic slice culture) was examined 2-14 days after 45 min of combined oxygen-glucose deprivation at 37 degrees C (OGD). Delayed cell death was serially measured in each slice by quantifying the binding of propidium iodide to DNA with fluorescence microscopy. RESULTS: Neuronal death was greatest in the CA1 region, with maximal death occurring 3-5 days after OGD. In CA1, cell death was 80 +/- 18% (mean +/- SD) 3 days after OGD and was 80-100% after 1 week. Death of 70 +/- 16% of CA3 neurons and 48 +/- 28% of dentate gyrus neurons occurred by the third day after OGD. Both isoflurane (1%) and the N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist MK-801 (10 microm) reduced cell death to levels similar to controls (no OGD) for 14 days after the injury. Isoflurane also reduced cell death in CA1 and CA3 caused by application of 100 but not 500 microm glutamate. Cellular viability (calcein fluorescence) and morphology were preserved in isoflurane-protected neurons. CONCLUSIONS: In an in vitro model of simulated ischemia, 1% isoflurane is of similar potency to 10 microm MK-801 in preventing delayed cell death. Modulation of glutamate excitotoxicity may contribute to the protective mechanism.


Asunto(s)
Anestésicos por Inhalación/farmacología , Isquemia Encefálica/tratamiento farmacológico , Isoflurano/farmacología , Fármacos Neuroprotectores/farmacología , Animales , Isquemia Encefálica/patología , Muerte Celular/efectos de los fármacos , Supervivencia Celular/efectos de los fármacos , Maleato de Dizocilpina/farmacología , Ácido Glutámico/toxicidad , Técnicas In Vitro , Ratas , Ratas Sprague-Dawley
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...