Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
1.
Pediatr Transplant ; 22(3): e13154, 2018 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29388290

RESUMEN

Many pediatric centers utilize a variety of protocols including preemptive plasmapheresis to prevent the recurrence of FSGS post-transplant. But the effectiveness of this expensive, time-consuming process of plasmapheresis in the prevention of FSGS recurrence is still unclear. We retrospectively reviewed all pediatric cases of FSGS in our center that received a kidney transplant and compared the transplant and patient outcomes of those transplanted after 2006 who received pretransplant plasmapheresis to those prior to 2006 who did not. Of the 57 children with FSGS, 31 and 26 were transplanted before and after 2006, respectively. The cohorts differed significantly in keeping with the center immunosuppression protocol changes, and prior to 2006, the recipients were significantly younger. All children with FSGS transplanted after 2006 underwent three and one sessions of 1.0 plasma volume/exchange plasmapheresis with fresh frozen plasma replacement prior to the transplant in living and deceased donors, respectively, in addition to five sessions of every other day post-transplant pheresis. The incidence (27% vs 26%, P = 1.0) and time to recurrence of FSGS in the kidney allograft (P = .22) were not significantly different in patients that did and did not undergo prophylactic plasmapheresis. We need to re-evaluate the role of preemptive plasmapheresis in the prevention of FSGS recurrence in a prospective multicenter study.


Asunto(s)
Glomeruloesclerosis Focal y Segmentaria/terapia , Trasplante de Riñón , Atención Perioperativa/métodos , Plasmaféresis/métodos , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Recurrencia , Estudios Retrospectivos , Resultado del Tratamiento
2.
J Prev Alzheimers Dis ; 4(4): 236-241, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29181488

RESUMEN

Tau neurofibrillary tangles are found in the brains of patients suffering from Alzheimer's disease and other tauopathies. The progressive spreading of tau pathology from one brain region to the next is believed to be caused by extracellular transsynaptic transmission of misfolded tau between neurons. Preclinical studies have shown that antibodies against tau can prevent this transfer of misfolded tau between cells. Thus, antibodies against tau have the potential to stop or slow the progression of tau pathology observed in human tauopathies. To test this hypothesis, a humanized anti-tau antibody (ABBV-8E12) was developed and a phase 1 clinical trial of this antibody has been completed. The double-blind, placebo-controlled phase 1 study tested single doses of ABBV-8E12 ranging from 2.5 to 50 mg/kg in 30 patients with progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP). ABBV-8E12 was found to have an acceptable safety profile with no clinically concerning trends in the number or severity of adverse events between the placebo and dosed groups. Pharmacokinetic modelling showed that the antibody has a plasma half-life and cerebrospinal fluid:plasma ratio consistent with other humanized antibodies, and there were no signs of immunogenicity against ABBV-8E12. Based on the acceptable safety and tolerability profile of single doses of ABBV-8E12, AbbVie is currently enrolling patients into two phase 2 clinical trials to assess efficacy and safety of multiple doses of ABBV-8E12 in patients with early Alzheimer's disease or PSP.


Asunto(s)
Anticuerpos Monoclonales Humanizados/uso terapéutico , Factores Inmunológicos/uso terapéutico , Tauopatías/terapia , Anticuerpos Monoclonales Humanizados/efectos adversos , Anticuerpos Monoclonales Humanizados/farmacocinética , Método Doble Ciego , Evaluación Preclínica de Medicamentos , Humanos , Factores Inmunológicos/efectos adversos , Factores Inmunológicos/farmacocinética , Inmunoterapia , Modelos Biológicos , Tauopatías/sangre , Tauopatías/líquido cefalorraquídeo
3.
Pediatr Transplant ; 21(5)2017 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28557148

RESUMEN

We previously demonstrated that detectable BKV replication in donor urine pretransplant was significantly associated with post-transplant recipient BKV viremia. In this 4-year prospective study, we assessed whether recipient BKV replication pretransplant was associated with post-transplant viremia/BKV nephropathy. We studied 220 primary adult and pediatric organ transplant recipients for 490 person-years and 2100 clinical visits. BKV viruria was detectable in 28 (16%), 26 adults and two children; and viremia in none pretransplant. Post-transplant viruria occurred in all recipients with pretransplant BKV viruria, significantly more than in recipients without pretransplant viruria on univariate (P<.005) and multivariate analysis including type of organ transplanted and immunosuppression type (P .008). Time to post-transplant viruria was significantly shorter in recipients with pretransplant viruria (P .01). By univariate and multivariate analysis, BKV viruria in recipients pretransplant did not impact post-transplant BKV viremia (P=.97 and .97, respectively) even when stratified by type of organ transplant (kidney P=.6; liver P=.5). The peak serum and urine BKV PCR post-transplant were not significantly different in patients with pretransplant BKV viruria and no one developed BK nephropathy. In conclusion, recipient BKV viruria prior to transplant predicts post-transplant viruria but not viremia or BKV nephropathy.


Asunto(s)
Virus BK/aislamiento & purificación , Enfermedades Renales/virología , Trasplante de Órganos , Infecciones por Polyomavirus/virología , Complicaciones Posoperatorias/virología , Infecciones Tumorales por Virus/virología , Viremia/virología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Biomarcadores/metabolismo , Niño , Preescolar , ADN Viral/sangre , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Lactante , Enfermedades Renales/diagnóstico , Enfermedades Renales/metabolismo , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Infecciones por Polyomavirus/diagnóstico , Infecciones por Polyomavirus/metabolismo , Complicaciones Posoperatorias/diagnóstico , Complicaciones Posoperatorias/metabolismo , Periodo Preoperatorio , Estudios Prospectivos , Factores de Riesgo , Infecciones Tumorales por Virus/diagnóstico , Infecciones Tumorales por Virus/metabolismo , Viremia/diagnóstico , Viremia/metabolismo , Esparcimiento de Virus , Adulto Joven
4.
Am J Transplant ; 17(6): 1670-1673, 2017 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28133953

RESUMEN

In small children with end-stage renal disease, an adult-sized kidney transplant is the best option. However, in the face of a completely thrombosed inferior vena cava (IVC), such transplants can be challenging, given the difficulty of achieving adequate renal venous outflow and the risk of graft thrombosis. Using a new technique to anastomose the renal vein to the right hepatic vein/IVC junction, we successfully implanted an adult-sized graft in two small children (9.8 and 14 kg) who had end-stage renal disease and a completely thrombosed IVC. After mobilizing the right lobe of the liver and obtaining total vascular occlusion of the liver, we used a Fogarty catheter to dilate the retrohepatic IVC. In the right hepatic vein, we made a venotomy and extended it inferiorly onto the retrohepatic IVC. To that venotomy, we anastomosed the donor left renal vein, using continuous 7-0 Prolene sutures. Both patients attained excellent renal allograft function: One had a serum creatinine level of 0.30 mg/dL at 6 mo after transplant, and the other had a level of 0.29 mg/dL at 1 year. In these two small children with completely thrombosed IVC, our technique for transplanting an adult-sized kidney provided adequate venous outflow.


Asunto(s)
Trasplante de Riñón , Vena Cava Inferior/cirugía , Trombosis de la Vena/cirugía , Adulto , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Recién Nacido , Donadores Vivos , Masculino , Pronóstico
5.
Osteoarthritis Cartilage ; 20(5): 405-412, 2012 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22326896

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: The distribution and function of lymphatic vessels in normal and diseased human knees are understood incompletely. This study aimed to investigate whether lymphatic density is associated with clinical, histological or radiographic parameters in osteoarthritis (OA). METHODS: Sections of synovium from 60 knees from patients with OA were compared with 60 post mortem control knees (from 37 individuals). Lymphatic vessels were identified using immunohistochemistry for podoplanin, and quantified as lymphatic vessel density (LVD) and lymphatic endothelial cell (LEC) fractional area. Effusion status was determined by clinical examination, radiographs were scored for OA changes, and inflammation grading used haematoxylin and eosin stained sections of synovium. RESULTS: Lymphatic vessels were present in synovia from both disease groups, but were not identified in subchondral bone. Synovial lymphatic densities were independent of radiological severity and age. Synovia from patients with OA displayed lower LVD (z=-3.4, P=0.001) and lower LEC fractional areas (z=-4.5, P<0.0005) than non-arthritic controls. In patients with OA, low LVD was associated with clinically detectable effusion (z=-2.2, P=0.027), but not with histological evidence of synovitis. The negative associations between lymphatics and OA/effusion appeared to be independent of other measured confounders. CONCLUSION: Lymphatic vessels are present in lower densities in OA synovia. Abnormalities of synovial fluid drainage may confound the value of effusion as a clinical sign of synovitis in OA.


Asunto(s)
Articulación de la Rodilla/patología , Vasos Linfáticos/patología , Osteoartritis de la Rodilla/patología , Anciano , Artroplastia de Reemplazo de Rodilla , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Edema/etiología , Edema/patología , Endotelio Linfático/patología , Femenino , Humanos , Artropatías/patología , Articulación de la Rodilla/irrigación sanguínea , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Osteoartritis de la Rodilla/complicaciones , Osteoartritis de la Rodilla/diagnóstico por imagen , Osteoartritis de la Rodilla/cirugía , Radiografía , Membrana Sinovial/irrigación sanguínea , Membrana Sinovial/patología
7.
J Neurotrauma ; 18(10): 1107-19, 2001 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11686496

RESUMEN

Spinal cord injury leads to many forms of autonomic dysfunction including autonomic dysreflexia, a condition involving recurrent episodes of paroxysmal hypertension and associated bradycardia. This hypertension may reach intensities that are life-threatening. We investigated autonomic dysreflexia and the sprouting of central processes of primary afferent neurons (a potential mechanism for autonomic dysreflexia) in a clinically-relevant calibrated clip-compression model of spinal cord injury in the rat. Autonomic dysreflexia was induced by colon distension in the conscious rats 2 weeks after severe (50-g) clip compression injury of the spinal cord at the 4th thoracic segment. The central arbor of small-diameter primary afferent fibers in laminae III-VII of the spinal cord dorsal horn was also assessed at 2 weeks after cord injury by quantitative morphometry, using calcitonin gene-related peptide as a marker. In response to colon distension, arterial pressure increased by 41 +/- 3 mmHg from a resting value of 109 +/- 4 mmHg, and heart rate decreased by 124 +/- 13 beats/min from a value of 515 +/- 16 beats/min (n = 7). Minimal locomotor function was recovered by these rats: by 2 weeks after injury they attained scores of only 3.1 +/- 1.3 on the Basso, Beattie and Bresnahan scale. Histopathology of the clip-compression lesion site in the cord consisted of extensive central necrosis extending several segments rostral and caudal to the lesion. Quantitative measures of the small-diameter afferent arbors revealed significant increases in area ranging from 20-27% in thoracolumbar segments caudal to the injury (n = 5) in comparison to sham-injured rats (n = 6). A second study was done to assess the impact of severity of injury on the relationship between the size of the primary afferent arbors and autonomic dysreflexia. At 2 weeks after milder (20-g) clip injury at T4, rats exhibited responses to colon distension that were not those associated with autonomic dysreflexia (n = 5). Arterial pressure increased by only 16 +/- 3 mmHg and heart rate tended to increase (+19 +/- 12 beats/min). These rats attained a locomotor score of 7.1 +/- 0.4 by 2 weeks. The lesions at the injury site also contained necrosis and mild cavitation within the gray matter. No change in the small-diameter afferent arbor was detected at 2 weeks after the 20-g clip injury at T4 (n = 6 rats). These findings suggest that after severe but not mild clip compression injury of the spinal cord, sprouting of the afferent component of the spinal reflex are contributes to the development of autonomic dysreflexia. Neither dysreflexia, nor changes in the afferent arbor size occurred after mild cord injury. This clinically relevant clip compression cord injury model, studied more frequently for locomotor function, is excellent for investigating mechanisms for the development of autonomic dysreflexia and strategies for its prevention.


Asunto(s)
Disreflexia Autónoma/fisiopatología , Neuronas Aferentes/patología , Traumatismos de la Médula Espinal/patología , Traumatismos de la Médula Espinal/fisiopatología , Animales , Disreflexia Autónoma/etiología , Presión Sanguínea/fisiología , Péptido Relacionado con Gen de Calcitonina/metabolismo , Colorantes , Ciclo Estral/fisiología , Femenino , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Inmunohistoquímica , Masculino , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Fibras Nerviosas/patología , Fibras Nerviosas Mielínicas/patología , Células del Asta Posterior/patología , Ratas , Médula Espinal/patología , Traumatismos de la Médula Espinal/complicaciones
9.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 17(9): 1525-34, 2000 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10975362

RESUMEN

We measured the detectability of moving signal dots in dynamic noise to determine whether local motion signals are preferentially combined along an axis parallel to the direction of motion. Observers were asked to detect a signal composed of three dots moving in a linear trajectory among dynamic noise dots. The signal dots were collinear and equally spaced in a configuration that was either parallel to or perpendicular to their trajectory. The probability of detecting the signal was measured as a function of noise density, over a range of signal dot spacings from 0.5 degrees to 5.0 degrees. At any given noise density, the signal in the parallel configuration was more detectable than that in the perpendicular configuration. Our four observers could tolerate 1.5-2.5 times more noise in the parallel configuration. This improvement is not due merely to temporal summation between consecutive dots in the parallel trajectory. Temporal summation functions measured on our observers indicate that the benefit from spatial coincidence of the dots lasts for no more than 50 ms, whereas the increased detectability of the parallel configuration is observed up to the largest temporal separations tested (210 ms). These results demonstrate that dots arranged parallel to the signal trajectory are more easily detected than those arranged perpendicularly. Moreover, this enhancement points to the existence of visual mechanisms that preferentially organize motion information parallel to the direction of motion.


Asunto(s)
Artefactos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Probabilidad , Factores de Tiempo
10.
Vision Res ; 40(10-12): 1227-68, 2000.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10788638

RESUMEN

Most theories of visual search emphasize issues of limited versus unlimited capacity and serial versus parallel processing. In the present article, we suggest a broader framework based on two principles, one empirical and one theoretical. The empirical principle is to focus on conditions at the intersection of visual search and the simple detection and discrimination paradigms of spatial vision. Such simple search conditions avoid artifacts and phenomena specific to more complex stimuli and tasks. The theoretical principle is to focus on the distinction between high and low threshold theory. While high threshold theory is largely discredited for simple detection and discrimination, it persists in the search literature. Furthermore, a low threshold theory such as signal detection theory can account for some of the phenomena attributed to limited capacity or serial processing. In the body of this article, we compare the predictions of high threshold theory and three versions of signal detection theory to the observed effects of manipulating set size, discriminability, number of targets, response bias, external noise, and distractor heterogeneity. For almost all cases, the results are inconsistent with high threshold theory and are consistent with all three versions of signal detection theory. In the Discussion, these simple theories are generalized to a larger domain that includes search asymmetry, multidimensional judgements including conjunction search, response time, search with multiple eye fixations and more general stimulus conditions. We conclude that low threshold theories can account for simple visual search without invoking mechanisms such as limited capacity or serial processing.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Psicológicos , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Humanos , Psicometría , Psicofísica , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Detección de Señal Psicológica/fisiología
11.
Vision Res ; 39(1): 19-30, 1999 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10211392

RESUMEN

Previous work has shown that a single dot moving in a consistent direction is easily detected among noise dots in Brownian motion (Watamaniuk et al., Vis Res 1995;35:65-77). In this study we calculated the predictions of a commonly-used psychophysical motion model for a motion trajectory in noise. This model assumes local motion energy detectors optimally tuned to the signal, followed by a decision stage that implements the maximum rule. We first show that local motion detectors do indeed explain the detectability of brief trajectories (100 ms) that fall within a single unit, but that they severely underestimate the detectability of extended trajectories that span multiple units. For instance, a 200 ms trajectory is approximately three times more detectable than two isolated 100 ms trajectories presented together within an equivalent temporal interval. This result suggests a nonlinear interaction among local motion units. This interaction is not restricted to linear trajectories because circular trajectories with curvatures larger than 1 degree are almost as detectable as linear trajectories. Our data are consistent with a flexible network that feeds forward excitation among units tuned to similar directions of motion.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Humanos , Matemática , Modelos Biológicos , Psicofísica
12.
Vision Res ; 37(4): 397-406, 1997 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9156171

RESUMEN

We address a surprising result in a previous study of speed discrimination with multiple moving gratings: discrimination thresholds decreased when the number of stimuli was increased, but remained unchanged when the area of a single stimulus was increased [Verghese & Stone (1995). Vision Research, 35, 2811-2823]. In this study, we manipulated the spatial- and phase relationship between multiple grating patches to determine their effect on speed discrimination thresholds. In a fusion experiment, we merged multiple stimulus patches, in stages, into a single patch. Thresholds increased as the patches were brought closer and their phase relationship was adjusted to be consistent with a single patch. Thresholds increased further still as these patches were fused into a single patch. In a fission experiment, we divided a single large patch into multiple patches by superimposing a cross with luminance equal to that of the background. Thresholds decreased as the large patch was divided into quadrants and decreased further as the quadrants were maximally separated. However, when the cross luminance was darker than the background, it was perceived as an occluder and thresholds, on average, were unchanged from that for the single large patch. A control experiment shows that the observed trend in discrimination thresholds is not due to the differences in perceived speed of the stimuli. These results suggest that the parsing of the visual image into entities affects the combination of speed information across space, and that each discrete entity effectively provides a single independent estimate of speed.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Humanos , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología
13.
Nature ; 381(6578): 161-3, 1996 May 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8610014

RESUMEN

Little is known about how or where the visual system parses the visual scene into objects or surfaces. However, it is generally assumed that the segmentation and grouping of pieces of the image into discrete entities is due to 'later' processing stages, after the 'early' processing of the visual image by local mechanisms selective for attributes such as colour, orientation, depth, and motion. Speed perception is also thought to be mediated by early mechanisms tuned for speed. Here we show that manipulating the way in which an image is parsed changes the way in which local speed information is processed. Manipulations that cause multiple stimuli to appear as parts of a single patch degrade speed discrimination, whereas manipulations that perceptually divide a single large stimulus into parts improve discrimination. These results indicate that processes as early as speed perception may be constrained by the parsing of the visual image into discrete entities.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Humanos , Modelos Neurológicos , Umbral Sensorial
14.
Vision Res ; 35(20): 2811-23, 1995 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8533322

RESUMEN

We used speed discrimination tasks to measure the ability of observers to combine speed information from multiple stimuli distributed across space. We compared speed discrimination thresholds in a classical discrimination paradigm to those in an uncertainty/search paradigm. Thresholds were measured using a temporal two-interval forced-choice design. In the discrimination paradigm, the n gratings in each interval all moved at the same speed and observers were asked to choose the interval with the faster gratings. Discrimination thresholds for this paradigm decreased as the number of gratings increased. This decrease was not due to increasing the effective stimulus area as a control experiment that increased the area of a single grating did not show a similar improvement in thresholds. Adding independent speed noise to each of the n gratings caused thresholds to decrease at a rate similar to the original no-noise case, consistent with observers combining an independent sample of speed from each grating in both the added- and no-noise cases. In the search paradigm, observers were asked to choose the interval in which one of the n gratings moved faster. Thresholds in this case increased with the number of gratings, behavior traditionally attributed to an input bottleneck. However, results from the discrimination paradigm showed that the increase was not due to observers' inability to process these gratings. We have also shown that the opposite trends of the data in the two paradigms can be predicted by a decision theory model that combines independent samples of speed information across space. This demonstrates that models typically used in classical detection and discrimination paradigms are also applicable to search paradigms. As our model does not distinguish between samples in space and time, it predicts that discrimination performance should be the same regardless of whether the gratings are presented in two spatial intervals or two temporal intervals. Our last experiment largely confirmed this prediction.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Teoría de las Decisiones , Umbral Diferencial/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Psicofísica , Factores de Tiempo
15.
Vision Res ; 34(18): 2453-67, 1994 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7975284

RESUMEN

We measured the probability of detecting the target in a visual search task, as a function of the following parameters: the discriminability of the target from the distractors, the duration of the display, and the number of elements in the display. We examined the relation between these parameters at criterion performance (80% correct) to determine if the parameters traded off according to the predictions of a limited capacity model. For the three dimensions that we studied, orientation, color, and spatial frequency, the observed relationship between the parameters deviates significantly from a limited capacity model. The data relating discriminability to display duration are better than predicted over the entire range of orientation and color differences that we examined, and are consistent with the prediction for only a limited range of spatial frequency differences--from 12 to 23%. The relation between discriminability and number varies considerably across the three dimensions and is better than the limited capacity prediction for two of the three dimensions that we studied. Orientation discrimination shows a strong number effect, color discrimination shows almost no effect, and spatial frequency discrimination shows an intermediate effect. The different trading relationships in each dimension are more consistent with early filtering in that dimension, than with a common limited capacity stage. Our results indicate that higher-level processes that group elements together also play a strong role. Our experiments provide little support for limited capacity mechanisms over the range of stimulus differences that we examined in three different dimensions.


Asunto(s)
Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Modelos Psicológicos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción , Rotación , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
16.
Vision Res ; 34(7): 955-62, 1994 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8160407

RESUMEN

Observers were asked to locate a target in a brief, two-scale display. Accuracy of locating the target was measured as a function of the ratio between the two scales. At each scale ratio, the probability of locating the target as a function of the number of elements is well fit by the idea that the observer accurately monitors only a "critical" number of elements. The dependence of critical number on scale ratio is well accounted for by a model that assumes that the observer's decision is based on an evenly spaced array of samples. The sample spacing is under attentional control, but is always uniform.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adulto , Atención , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción
17.
Anesth Analg ; 78(3): 482-5, 1994 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8109764

RESUMEN

We compared the hemodynamic response to laryngoscopy and intubation, as well as emergence and recovery times, when propofol or thiopental were used for rapid intravenous induction of anesthesia in 59 infants undergoing repair of inguinal hernia. An intravenous catheter was inserted under N2O analgesia and atropine 0.01 mg/kg was administered to all patients. Subsequent induction with propofol (3 mg/kg), thiopental (5 mg/kg), or halothane (2%) was followed with succinylcholine (2 mg/kg) and tracheal intubation. Ventilation was manually assisted during surgery, and tracheas were extubated when patients were completely awake. Infants who received propofol showed less hypertensive response to intubation than those who received thiopental or halothane. In the 1- to 6-mo age group, emergence (extubation) time was significantly longer for infants who received thiopental (10.2 +/- 1.4 min) than for those who received propofol or halothane (5.5 +/- 2.5 and 6.2 +/- 1.3 min, respectively). Infants who received thiopental induction had a higher incidence of perioperative airway complications than all others. There was no significant difference in the recovery and discharge times among the three groups. We conclude that when rapid intravenous induction is required for infants, propofol is more effective than thiopental in obtunding the hypertensive response to intubation, and in young infants (1-6 mo) it results in more prompt emergence after short surgical procedures.


Asunto(s)
Anestesia Intravenosa , Hernia Inguinal/cirugía , Propofol , Tiopental , Presión Sanguínea/fisiología , Procedimientos Quirúrgicos Electivos , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Factores de Tiempo
18.
Stroke ; 24(1): 126-31, 1993 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8418536

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: A retrospective study of five patients with the clinical or magnetic resonance-based diagnosis of carotid dissection was done. Clinical data, imaging studies, treatment, and outcome were reviewed. The potential applicability of three-dimensional time-of-flight magnetic resonance angiography in these settings was evaluated. SUMMARY OF REPORT: This modality reliably showed vascular abnormalities and focal, segmental, or aneurysmal dilatation when correlated with conventional angiograms in three patients. Turbulence and magnetic susceptibility at the acute turn of the carotid in the petrous canal led to a false-positive diagnosis on magnetic resonance angiography in one patient (in whom subtle fibromuscular hyperplasia was found with conventional angiography but missed with magnetic resonance angiography). CONCLUSIONS: When combined with appropriate clinical signs, magnetic resonance imaging and magnetic resonance angiography can reliably establish the diagnosis of carotid dissection. Pitfalls of magnetic resonance angiography are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Angiografía/métodos , Enfermedades de las Arterias Carótidas/diagnóstico , Adulto , Estudios de Evaluación como Asunto , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estudios Retrospectivos
19.
Vision Res ; 32(5): 983-95, 1992 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1604866

RESUMEN

Is visual attention mediated by a general-purpose processor with a small data capacity? Such an attentive processor could perform a wide range of transformations upon a small amount of image data. We suggest that this limited capacity corresponds to a fixed amount of information, measured in bits. We measure how much information an observer's attention can handle by measuring how much we can restrict display information without impairing the observer's performance. The attentive visual tasks we study are the detection of a stationary dot in a field of moving dots, and the detection of a static square in a field of flashing squares. Performance of these tasks is perfect up to a critical number of elements (the span of attention) and then falls as the number of elements increases beyond this critical number. The display information required for unimpaired performance in each of these tasks is low; the results indicate that visual attention processes only 30 to 60 bits of display information.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Juicio , Matemática , Modelos Neurológicos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Agudeza Visual/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA