Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 25
Filtrar
1.
Behav Res Methods ; 54(3): 1530-1540, 2022 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34751923

RESUMEN

The stop-signal paradigm has become ubiquitous in investigations of inhibitory control. Tasks inspired by the paradigm, referred to as stop-signal tasks, require participants to make responses on go trials and to inhibit those responses when presented with a stop-signal on stop trials. Currently, the most popular version of the stop-signal task is the 'choice-reaction' variant, where participants make choice responses, but must inhibit those responses when presented with a stop-signal. An alternative to the choice-reaction variant of the stop-signal task is the 'anticipated response inhibition' task. In anticipated response inhibition tasks, participants are required to make a planned response that coincides with a predictably timed event (such as lifting a finger from a computer key to stop a filling bar at a predefined target). Anticipated response inhibition tasks have some advantages over the more traditional choice-reaction stop-signal tasks and are becoming increasingly popular. However, currently, there are no openly available versions of the anticipated response inhibition task, limiting potential uptake. Here, we present an open-source, free, and ready-to-use version of the anticipated response inhibition task, which we refer to as the OSARI (the Open-Source Anticipated Response Inhibition) task.


Asunto(s)
Inhibición Psicológica , Desempeño Psicomotor , Humanos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
2.
Neuropsychopharmacology ; 45(13): 2170-2179, 2020 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32919405

RESUMEN

The cognitive enhancing effects of methylphenidate are well established, but the mechanisms remain unclear. We recently demonstrated that methylphenidate boosts cognitive motivation by enhancing the weight on the benefits of a cognitive task in a manner that depended on striatal dopamine. Here, we considered the complementary hypothesis that methylphenidate might also act by changing the weight on the opportunity cost of a cognitive task, that is, the cost of foregoing alternative opportunity. To this end, 50 healthy participants (25 women) completed a novel cognitive effort-discounting task that required choices between task and leisure. They were tested on methylphenidate, placebo, as well as the selective D2-receptor agent sulpiride, the latter to strengthen inference about dopamine receptor selectivity of methylphenidate's effects. Furthermore, they also underwent an [18F]DOPA PET scan to quantify striatal dopamine synthesis capacity. Methylphenidate boosted choices of cognitive effort over leisure across the group, and this effect was greatest in participants with more striatal dopamine synthesis capacity. The effects of sulpiride did not reach significance. This study strengthens the motivational account of methylphenidate's effects on cognition, and suggests that methylphenidate reduces the cost of mental labor by increasing striatal dopamine.


Asunto(s)
Dopamina , Metilfenidato , Cuerpo Estriado , Inhibidores de Captación de Dopamina , Femenino , Humanos , Actividades Recreativas
3.
iScience ; 23(1): 100777, 2020 Jan 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31958755

RESUMEN

We investigated whether a task requiring concurrent perceptual decision-making and response control can be performed concurrently, whether evidence accumulation and response control are accomplished by the same neurons, and whether perceptual decision-making and countermanding can be unified computationally. Based on neural recordings in a prefrontal area of macaque monkeys, we present behavioral, neural, and computational results demonstrating that perceptual decision-making of varying difficulty can be countermanded efficiently, that single prefrontal neurons instantiate both evidence accumulation and response control, and that an interactive race between stochastic GO evidence accumulators for each alternative and a distinct STOP accumulator fits countermanding choice behavior and replicates neural trajectories. Thus, perceptual decision-making and response control, previously regarded as distinct mechanisms, are actually aspects of a common neuro-computational mechanism supporting flexible behavior.

4.
Elife ; 82019 04 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31033438

RESUMEN

Response inhibition is essential for navigating everyday life. Its derailment is considered integral to numerous neurological and psychiatric disorders, and more generally, to a wide range of behavioral and health problems. Response-inhibition efficiency furthermore correlates with treatment outcome in some of these conditions. The stop-signal task is an essential tool to determine how quickly response inhibition is implemented. Despite its apparent simplicity, there are many features (ranging from task design to data analysis) that vary across studies in ways that can easily compromise the validity of the obtained results. Our goal is to facilitate a more accurate use of the stop-signal task. To this end, we provide 12 easy-to-implement consensus recommendations and point out the problems that can arise when they are not followed. Furthermore, we provide user-friendly open-source resources intended to inform statistical-power considerations, facilitate the correct implementation of the task, and assist in proper data analysis.


Asunto(s)
Consenso , Conducta Impulsiva/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Animales , Toma de Decisiones , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Humanos , Modelos Animales , Modelos Psicológicos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción
5.
eNeuro ; 5(2)2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30094335

RESUMEN

The aging brain is characterized by altered dopamine signaling. The amino acid tyrosine, a catecholamine precursor, is known to improve cognitive performance in young adults, especially during high environmental demands. Tyrosine administration might also affect catecholamine transmission in the aging brain, thereby improving cognitive functioning. In healthy older adults, impairments have been demonstrated in two forms of response inhibition: reactive inhibition (outright stopping) and proactive inhibition (anticipatory response slowing) under high information load. However, no study has directly compared the effects of a catecholamine precursor on reactive and load-dependent proactive inhibition. In this study we explored the effects of tyrosine on reactive and proactive response inhibition and signal in dopaminergically innervated fronto-striatal regions. Depending on age, tyrosine might lead to beneficial or detrimental neurocognitive effects. We aimed to address these hypotheses in 24 healthy older human adults (aged 61-72 years) using fMRI in a double blind, counterbalanced, placebo-controlled, within-subject design. Across the group, tyrosine did not alter reactive or proactive inhibition behaviorally but did increase fronto-parietal proactive inhibition-related activation. When taking age into account, tyrosine affected proactive inhibition both behaviorally and neurally. Specifically, increasing age was associated with a greater detrimental effect of tyrosine compared with placebo on proactive slowing. Moreover, with increasing age, tyrosine decreased fronto-striatal and parietal proactive signal, which correlated positively with tyrosine's effects on proactive slowing. Concluding, tyrosine negatively affected proactive response slowing and associated fronto-striatal activation in an age-dependent manner, highlighting the importance of catecholamines, perhaps particularly dopamine, for proactive response inhibition in older adults.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/efectos de los fármacos , Función Ejecutiva/efectos de los fármacos , Lóbulo Parietal/efectos de los fármacos , Corteza Prefrontal/efectos de los fármacos , Inhibición Proactiva , Putamen/efectos de los fármacos , Inhibición Reactiva , Tirosina/farmacología , Anciano , Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Método Doble Ciego , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/efectos de los fármacos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Lóbulo Parietal/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Prefrontal/diagnóstico por imagen , Desempeño Psicomotor/efectos de los fármacos , Putamen/diagnóstico por imagen , Tirosina/administración & dosificación , Tirosina/efectos adversos
6.
Psychol Med ; 48(15): 2515-2521, 2018 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29415788

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Attenuated inhibitory control is one of the most robust findings in the neuropsychology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). However, it is unclear whether this represents a deficit in outright stopping (reactive inhibition), whether it relates to a deficit in anticipatory response slowing (proactive inhibition), or both. In addition, children with other development disorders, such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD), often have symptoms of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity similar to children with ADHD. These may relate to similar underlying changes in inhibitory processing. METHODS: In this study, we used a modified stop-signal task to dissociate reactive and proactive inhibition. We included not only children with ADHD, but also children primarily diagnosed with an ASD and high parent-rated levels of ADHD symptoms. RESULTS: We replicated the well-documented finding of attenuated reactive inhibition in children with ADHD. In addition, we found a similar deficit in children with ASD and a similar level of ADHD symptoms. In contrast, we found no evidence for deficits in proactive inhibition in either clinical group. CONCLUSIONS: These findings re-emphasize the role of reactive inhibition in children with ADHD and ADHD symptoms. Moreover, our findings stress the importance of a trans-diagnostic approach to the relationship between behavior and neuropsychology.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Inhibición Proactiva , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Inhibición Reactiva , Niño , Humanos , Masculino
7.
Eur J Neurosci ; 45(12): 1512-1523, 2017 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28449195

RESUMEN

Response inhibition is an important executive process studied by clinical and experimental psychologists, neurophysiologists and cognitive neuroscientists alike. Stop-signal paradigms are popular because they are grounded in a theory that provides methods to estimate the latency of an unobservable process: the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT). Critically, SSRT estimates can be biased by skew of the response time distribution and gradual slowing over the course of the experiment. Here, we present a series of experiments that directly compare three common stop-signal paradigms that differ in the distribution of response times. The results show that the widely used choice response (CR) and simple response (SR) time versions of the stop-signal paradigm are particularly susceptible to skew of the response time distribution and response slowing, and that using the anticipated response (AR) paradigm based on the Slater-Hammel task offers a viable alternative to obtain more reliable SSRT estimates.


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica , Conducta de Elección , Inhibición Neural , Adulto , Anciano , Encéfalo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Encéfalo/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
Neurobiol Aging ; 46: 96-106, 2016 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27460154

RESUMEN

Two distinct forms of response inhibition may underlie observed deficits in response inhibition in aging. We assessed whether age-related neurocognitive impairments in response inhibition reflect deficient reactive inhibition (outright stopping) or also deficient proactive inhibition (anticipatory response slowing), which might be particularly evident with high information load. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging in young (n = 25, age range 18-32) and older adults (n = 23, 61-74) with a stop-signal task. Relative to young adults, older adults exhibited impaired reactive inhibition (i.e., longer stop-signal reaction time) and increased blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal for successful versus unsuccessful inhibition in the left frontal cortex and cerebellum. Furthermore, older adults also exhibited impaired proactive slowing, but only as a function of information load. This load-dependent behavioral deficit was accompanied by a failure to increase blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal under high information load in lateral frontal cortex, presupplementary motor area and striatum. Our findings suggest that inhibitory deficits in older adults are caused both by reduced stopping abilities and by diminished preparation capacity during information overload.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Inhibición Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anticipación Psicológica , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición , Femenino , Neuroimagen Funcional , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Consumo de Oxígeno/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
9.
Eur J Neurosci ; 41(8): 1086-94, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25832122

RESUMEN

The subjective belief of what will happen plays an important role across many cognitive domains, including response inhibition. However, tasks that study inhibition do not distinguish between the processing of objective contextual cues indicating stop-signal probability and the subjective expectation that a stop-signal will or will not occur. Here we investigated the effects of stop-signal probability and the expectation of a stop-signal on proactive inhibition. Twenty participants performed a modified stop-signal anticipation task while being scanned with functional magnetic resonance imaging. At the beginning of each trial, the stop-signal probability was indicated by a cue (0% or > 0%), and participants had to indicate whether they expected a stop-signal to occur (yes/no/don't know). Participants slowed down responding on trials with a > 0% stop-signal probability, but this proactive response slowing was even greater when they expected a stop-signal to occur. Analyses were performed in brain regions previously associated with proactive inhibition. Activation in the striatum, supplementary motor area and left dorsal premotor cortex during the cue period was increased when participants expected a stop-signal to occur. In contrast, activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus and right inferior parietal cortex activity during the stimulus-response period was related to the processing of contextual cues signalling objective stop-signal probability, regardless of expectation. These data show that proactive inhibition depends on both the processing of objective contextual task information and the subjective expectation of stop-signals.


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Inhibición Psicológica , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Probabilidad , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto Joven
10.
Neuroimage Clin ; 7: 132-41, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25610775

RESUMEN

ADHD is characterized by increased intra-individual variability in response times during the performance of cognitive tasks. However, little is known about developmental changes in intra-individual variability, and how these changes relate to cognitive performance. Twenty subjects with ADHD aged 7-24 years and 20 age-matched, typically developing controls participated in an fMRI-scan while they performed a go-no-go task. We fit an ex-Gaussian distribution on the response distribution to objectively separate extremely slow responses, related to lapses of attention, from variability on fast responses. We assessed developmental changes in these intra-individual variability measures, and investigated their relation to no-go performance. Results show that the ex-Gaussian measures were better predictors of no-go performance than traditional measures of reaction time. Furthermore, we found between-group differences in the change in ex-Gaussian parameters with age, and their relation to task performance: subjects with ADHD showed age-related decreases in their variability on fast responses (sigma), but not in lapses of attention (tau), whereas control subjects showed a decrease in both measures of variability. For control subjects, but not subjects with ADHD, this age-related reduction in variability was predictive of task performance. This group difference was reflected in neural activation: for typically developing subjects, the age-related decrease in intra-individual variability on fast responses (sigma) predicted activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus (dACG), whereas for subjects with ADHD, activity in this region was related to improved no-go performance with age, but not to intra-individual variability. These data show that using more sophisticated measures of intra-individual variability allows the capturing of the dynamics of task performance and associated neural changes not permitted by more traditional measures.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Individualidad , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Joven
11.
Neuroimage ; 103: 65-74, 2014 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25224995

RESUMEN

Response inhibition involves proactive and reactive modes. Proactive inhibition is goal-directed, triggered by warning cues, and serves to restrain actions. Reactive inhibition is stimulus-driven, triggered by salient stop-signals, and used to stop actions completely. Functional MRI studies have identified brain regions that activate during proactive and reactive inhibition. It remains unclear how these brain regions operate in functional networks, and whether proactive and reactive inhibition depend on common networks, unique networks, or a combination. To address this we analyzed a large fMRI dataset (N=65) of a stop-signal task designed to measure proactive and reactive inhibition, using independent component analysis (ICA). We found 1) three frontal networks that were associated with both proactive and reactive inhibition, 2) one network in the superior parietal lobe, which also included dorsal premotor cortex and left putamen, that was specifically associated with proactive inhibition, and 3) two right-lateralized frontal and fronto-parietal networks, including the right inferior frontal gyrus and temporoparietal junction as well as a bilateral fronto-temporal network that were uniquely associated with reactive inhibition. Overlap between networks was observed in dorsolateral prefrontal and parietal cortices. Taken together, we offer a new perspective on the neural underpinnings of inhibitory control, by showing that proactive inhibition and reactive inhibition are supported by a group of common and unique networks that appear to integrate and interact in frontoparietal areas.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Inhibición Reactiva , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador/métodos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino
12.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(7): 2848-53, 2014 Feb 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24550315

RESUMEN

Decision-making is explained by psychologists through stochastic accumulator models and by neurophysiologists through the activity of neurons believed to instantiate these models. We investigated an overlooked scaling problem: How does a response time (RT) that can be explained by a single model accumulator arise from numerous, redundant accumulator neurons, each of which individually appears to explain the variability of RT? We explored this scaling problem by developing a unique ensemble model of RT, called e pluribus unum, which embodies the well-known dictum "out of many, one." We used the e pluribus unum model to analyze the RTs produced by ensembles of redundant, idiosyncratic stochastic accumulators under various termination mechanisms and accumulation rate correlations in computer simulations of ensembles of varying size. We found that predicted RT distributions are largely invariant to ensemble size if the accumulators share at least modestly correlated accumulation rates and RT is not governed by the most extreme accumulators. Under these regimes the termination times of individual accumulators was predictive of ensemble RT. We also found that the threshold measured on individual accumulators, corresponding to the firing rate of neurons measured at RT, can be invariant with RT but is equivalent to the specified model threshold only when the rate correlation is very high.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Psicológicos , Neuronas/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Biología Computacional , Simulación por Computador , Humanos , Método de Montecarlo , Neurofisiología , Procesos Estocásticos
13.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 35(9): 4415-27, 2014 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24532023

RESUMEN

During adolescence, functional and structural changes in the brain facilitate the transition from childhood to adulthood. Because the cortex and the striatum mature at different rates, temporary imbalances in the frontostriatal network occur. Here, we investigate the development of the subcortical and cortical components of the frontostriatal network from early adolescence to early adulthood in 60 subjects in a cross-sectional design, using functional MRI and a stop-signal task measuring two forms of inhibitory control: reactive inhibition (outright stopping) and proactive inhibition (anticipation of stopping). During development, reactive inhibition improved: older subjects were faster in reactive inhibition. In the brain, this was paralleled by an increase in motor cortex suppression. The level of proactive inhibition increased, with older subjects slowing down responding more than younger subjects when anticipating a stop-signal. Activation increased in the right striatum, right ventral and dorsal inferior frontal gyrus, and supplementary motor area. Moreover, functional connectivity during proactive inhibition increased between striatum and frontal regions with age. In conclusion, we demonstrate that developmental improvements in proactive inhibition are paralleled by increases in activation and functional connectivity of the frontostriatal network. These data serve as a stepping stone to investigate abnormal development of the frontostriatal network in disorders such as schizophrenia and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.


Asunto(s)
Cuerpo Estriado/crecimiento & desarrollo , Cuerpo Estriado/fisiología , Lóbulo Frontal/crecimiento & desarrollo , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Inhibición Proactiva , Adolescente , Desarrollo del Adolescente/fisiología , Adulto , Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Niño , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Vías Nerviosas/crecimiento & desarrollo , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto Joven
14.
Schizophr Res ; 150(2-3): 555-62, 2013 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24051015

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Impaired working memory (WM) is a hallmark of schizophrenia. In addition to classical WM regions such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the striatum, dysfunctions in the default-mode network (DMN) contribute to these WM deficits. Unaffected siblings of patients also show WM impairments. However, the nature of the functional deficits underlying these impairments is unclear, mainly because of impaired performance confounding neuroimaging results. METHODS: Here, we investigated WM and DMN activity in 23 unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients and 24 healthy volunteers using fMRI and a Sternberg WM task. WM load was determined prior to scanning to ensure 90% accuracy for all subjects. RESULTS: Siblings showed hyperactivation during the encoding phase of WM in the right medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) which is the anterior part of the DMN. No differences were found during the maintenance phase. During the retrieval phase, siblings showed hyperactivation in WM regions: DLPFC, inferior parietal cortex and the striatum. Siblings who showed hyperactivity in the MPFC during encoding showed DLPFC and striatum hyperactivation during retrieval. CONCLUSIONS: Our finding of hyperactivation in WM and DMN areas indicates that siblings fail to adequately inhibit DMN activity during demanding cognitive tasks and subsequently hyperactivate WM areas. This failure may reflect dopamine hyperactivity in the striatum which prevents adequate DMN suppression needed for effective WM. This study provides support for the notion that aberrant WM and DMN activation patterns may represent candidate endophenotypes for schizophrenia.


Asunto(s)
Cuerpo Estriado/patología , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/patología , Esquizofrenia/complicaciones , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Vías Nerviosas/patología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Hermanos , Adulto Joven
16.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 25(2): 157-74, 2013 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23066733

RESUMEN

Stopping an action requires suppression of the primary motor cortex (M1). Inhibitory control over M1 relies on a network including the right inferior frontal cortex (rIFC) and the supplementary motor complex (SMC), but how these regions interact to exert inhibitory control over M1 is unknown. Specifically, the hierarchical position of the rIFC and SMC with respect to each other, the routes by which these regions control M1, and the causal involvement of these regions in proactive and reactive inhibition remain unclear. We used off-line repetitive TMS to perturb neural activity in the rIFC and SMC followed by fMRI to examine effects on activation in the networks involved in proactive and reactive inhibition, as assessed with a modified stop-signal task. We found repetitive TMS effects on reactive inhibition only. rIFC and SMC stimulation shortened the stop-signal RT (SSRT) and a shorter SSRT was associated with increased M1 deactivation. Furthermore, rIFC and SMC stimulation increased right striatal activation, implicating frontostriatal pathways in reactive inhibition. Finally, rIFC stimulation altered SMC activation, but SMC stimulation did not alter rIFC activation, indicating that rIFC lies upstream from SMC. These findings extend our knowledge about the functional organization of inhibitory control, an important component of executive functioning, showing that rIFC exerts reactive control over M1 via SMC and right striatum.


Asunto(s)
Cuerpo Estriado/citología , Cuerpo Estriado/fisiología , Corteza Motora/citología , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Vías Eferentes/citología , Vías Eferentes/fisiología , Femenino , Lóbulo Frontal/citología , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Adulto Joven
17.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 34(9): 2015-24, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22359406

RESUMEN

The ability to stop a prepared response (reactive inhibition) appears to depend on the degree to which stopping is expected (proactive inhibition). Functional MRI studies have shown that activation during proactive and reactive inhibition overlaps, suggesting that the whole neural network for reactive inhibition becomes already activated in anticipation of stopping. However, these studies measured proactive inhibition as the effect of stop-signal probability on activation during go trials. Therefore, activation could reflect expectation of a stop-signal (evoked by the stop-signal probability cue), but also violation of this expectation because stop-signals do not occur on go trials. We addressed this problem, using a stop-signal task in which the stop-signal probability cue and the go-signal were separated in time. Hence, we could separate activation during the cue, reflecting expectation of the stop-signal, from activation during the go-signal, reflecting expectation of the stop-signal or violation of that expectation. During the cue, the striatum, the supplementary motor complex (SMC), and the midbrain activated. During the go-signal, the right inferior parietal cortex (IPC) and the right inferior frontal cortex (IFC) activated. These findings suggest that the neural network previously associated with proactive inhibition can be subdivided into two components. One component, including the striatum, the SMC, and the midbrain, activated during the cue, implicating this network in proactive inhibition. Another component, consisting of the right IPC and the right IFC, activated during the go-signal. Rather than being involved in proactive inhibition, this network appears to be involved in processes associated with violation of expectations.


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Joven
18.
J Neurosci ; 32(31): 10449-50, 2012 Aug 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22855793
19.
J Alzheimers Dis ; 30(2): 355-65, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22414571

RESUMEN

Hippocampal pathology is central to Alzheimer's disease (AD) and other forms of dementia such as frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD). Autopsy studies have shown that certain hippocampal subfields are more vulnerable than others to AD and FTLD pathology, in particular the subiculum and cornu ammonis 1 (CA1). We conducted shape analysis of hippocampi segmented from structural T1 MRI images on clinically diagnosed dementia patients and controls. The subjects included 19 AD and 35 FTLD patients [13 frontotemporal dementia (FTD), 13 semantic dementia (SD), and 9 progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA)] and 21 controls. Compared to controls, SD displayed severe atrophy of the whole left hippocampus. PNFA and FTD also displayed atrophy on the left side, restricted to the hippocampal head in FTD. Finally, AD displayed most atrophy in left hippocampal body with relative sparing of the hippocampal head. Consistent with neuropathological studies, most atrophic deformation was found in CA1 and subiculum areas in FTLD and AD.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/patología , Degeneración Lobar Frontotemporal/patología , Hipocampo/patología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Adulto , Anciano , Atrofia/patología , Región CA1 Hipocampal/patología , Región CA2 Hipocampal/patología , Región CA3 Hipocampal/patología , Demencia/patología , Giro Dentado/patología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Afasia Progresiva Primaria no Fluente/patología
20.
PLoS One ; 7(1): e29517, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22235303

RESUMEN

Almost all cortical areas are connected to the subcortical basal ganglia (BG) through parallel recurrent inhibitory and excitatory loops, exerting volitional control over automatic behavior. As this model is largely based on non-human primate research, we used high resolution functional MRI and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) to investigate the functional and structural organization of the human (pre)frontal cortico-basal network controlling eye movements. Participants performed saccades in darkness, pro- and antisaccades and observed stimuli during fixation. We observed several bilateral functional subdivisions along the precentral sulcus around the human frontal eye fields (FEF): a medial and lateral zone activating for saccades in darkness, a more fronto-medial zone preferentially active for ipsilateral antisaccades, and a large anterior strip along the precentral sulcus activating for visual stimulus presentation during fixation. The supplementary eye fields (SEF) were identified along the medial wall containing all aforementioned functions. In the striatum, the BG area receiving almost all cortical input, all saccade related activation was observed in the putamen, previously considered a skeletomotor striatal subdivision. Activation elicited by the cue instructing pro or antisaccade trials was clearest in the medial FEF and right putamen. DTI fiber tracking revealed that the subdivisions of the human FEF complex are mainly connected to the putamen, in agreement with the fMRI findings. The present findings demonstrate that the human FEF has functional subdivisions somewhat comparable to non-human primates. However, the connections to and activation in the human striatum preferentially involve the putamen, not the caudate nucleus as is reported for monkeys. This could imply that fronto-striatal projections for the oculomotor system are fundamentally different between humans and monkeys. Alternatively, there could be a bias in published reports of monkey studies favoring the caudate nucleus over the putamen in the search for oculomotor functions.


Asunto(s)
Ganglios Basales/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Imagen de Difusión Tensora/métodos , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Volición/fisiología , Adulto , Ganglios Basales/anatomía & histología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Neostriado/anatomía & histología , Neostriado/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/anatomía & histología , Corteza Prefrontal/anatomía & histología , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...