Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 14 de 14
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 66(8): 2821-2830, 2023 08 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37418756

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: This study compares the home language environments of children with (a suspicion of) developmental language disorder (DLD) with that of children with typical development (TD). It does so by adopting new technology that automatically provides metrics about children's language environment (Language ENvironment Analysis [LENA]). In addition, relationships between LENA metrics and standardized language tests are explored in the DLD group. METHOD: Ninety-nine 2- to 4-year-old toddlers participated: 59 with (a suspicion of) DLD and 40 with TD. LENA metrics on adult word count, conversational turn count, and child vocalization count were obtained. For all children, data on parental education and multilingualism were available. In the DLD group, data were collected on receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar, and on nonverbal intelligence, using standardized tests. RESULTS: We found lower adult word count, conversational turn count, and child vocalization count in the DLD group, independent of multilingualism but not of parental education. In the DLD group, receptive vocabulary was related to conversational turn count and child vocalization count, but not to adult word count. Expressive vocabulary, receptive grammar, and expressive grammar were not related to LENA metrics. CONCLUSIONS: Toddlers with (a suspicion of) DLD vocalize less at home than children with TD. They also hear fewer adult words and experience fewer conversational turns. Children with DLD's language outcomes are to a limited extent related to language environment at home. Conversational turns and child vocalizations are in this respect more important than adult words, in line with findings for TD populations.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Adulto , Humanos , Preescolar , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Lenguaje , Comunicación , Vocabulario , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Pruebas del Lenguaje
2.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 64(6): 2005-2021, 2021 06 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34019773

RESUMEN

Purpose This study compares online word recognition and prediction in preschoolers with (a suspicion of) a developmental language disorder (DLD) and typically developing (TD) controls. Furthermore, it investigates correlations between these measures and the link between online and off-line language scores in the DLD group. Method Using the visual world paradigm, Dutch children ages 3;6 (years;months) with (a suspicion of) DLD (n = 51) and TD peers (n = 31) listened to utterances such as, "Kijk, een hoed!" (Look, a hat!) in a word recognition task, and sentences such as, "Hé, hij leest gewoon een boek" (literal translation: Hey, he reads just a book) in a word prediction task, while watching a target and distractor picture. Results Both groups demonstrated a significant word recognition effect that looked similar directly after target onset. However, the DLD group looked longer at the target than the TD group and shifted slower from the distractor to target pictures. Within the DLD group, word recognition was linked to off-line expressive language scores. For word prediction, the DLD group showed a smaller effect and slower shifts from verb onset compared to the TD group. Interestingly, within the DLD group, prediction behavior varied considerably, and was linked to receptive and expressive language scores. Finally, slower shifts in word recognition were related to smaller prediction effects. Conclusions While the groups' word recognition abilities looked similar, and only differed in processing speed and dwell time, the DLD group showed atypical verb-based prediction behavior. This may be due to limitations in their processing capacity and/or their linguistic knowledge, in particular of verb argument structure.


Asunto(s)
Tecnología de Seguimiento Ocular , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Niño , Lenguaje Infantil , Humanos , Lenguaje , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Lingüística , Masculino
3.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32824870

RESUMEN

Language problems are a risk factor for externalizing problems, but the developmental path remains unclear. Emotional competence may mediate the relationship, especially when externalizing problems are reactive in nature, such as in Oppositional Deviant Disorder (ODD) and reactive aggression. We examined the development of reactive and proactive externalizing problems in children with (n = 98) and without (n = 156) Developmental Language Disorder (DLD; age: 8-16 years) over 18 months. Relationships with communicative risk factors (structural, pragmatic and emotion communication) and the mediating role of emotional competence (emotion recognition and anger dysregulation) were examined. Multi-level analyses showed that increasing emotion recognition and decreasing anger dysregulation were longitudinally related to decreasing ODD symptoms in both groups, whereas anger dysregulation was related to more reactive aggression in children with DLD alone. Pragmatic and emotion communication problems were related to more reactive externalizing problems, but these relationships were mediated by emotional competence, suggesting that problems in emotional competence explain the communication problems of children with DLD. Therefore, in addition to interventions for communication skills, there is a need to address the emotional competence of children with DLD, as this decreases the risk for reactive externalizing problems.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/epidemiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Ira , Niño , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino
4.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 53(6): 1110-1123, 2018 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30141224

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Depressive symptoms are common in children with developmental language disorder (DLD). However, risk and protective factors contributing to these problems are currently underspecified. AIMS: The current longitudinal study examined the role of emotion-regulation (ER) strategies in the severity of depressive symptoms in children with and without DLD, taking into account the severity of communication problems of children with DLD. METHODS & PROCEDURES: We followed clinically referred children with DLD (n = 114, 49% girls) and without DLD (n = 214, 58% girls) between the ages of 8 and 16 years across an 18-month period. Participants completed self-report questionnaires at three time points. Parents of children with DLD reported on their child's communication problems. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: Multilevel analyses confirmed higher levels of depressive symptoms in youngsters with DLD compared with peers without DLD, with a decrease across time in the DLD group. In both groups, higher levels of approach and increasing avoidant strategies aimed at distraction or trivializing a problem explained lower depressive symptoms, whereas more worry and externalizing strategies contributed to more depressive symptoms. Within the DLD group, semantic language problems were associated with higher depressive symptoms. However, this relation was mediated by the tendency to worry or use externalizing strategies. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: Results suggest that interventions for children with DLD should focus on enhancing their adaptive ER strategies to help them cope with daily stressors just as in the general population.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Psicológica , Depresión/psicología , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/psicología , Adolescente , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Depresión/complicaciones , Depresión/diagnóstico , Femenino , Humanos , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/complicaciones , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Factores de Riesgo , Autoinforme
5.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 61(8): 2028-2044, 2018 08 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29998317

RESUMEN

Purpose: Victimization is a common problem for many children but is exacerbated for children with a developmental language disorder (DLD). However, the severity of communication problems does not explain their victimization rates. In children without DLD, difficulties with emotional competence are a risk factor for victimization and also increase the risk of bullying. In this longitudinal study, we examined the extent to which the level and development of emotional competence (understanding of one's own emotions and levels of anger, sadness, and fear) contributed to the prediction of victimization and bullying in children with and without DLD, over and above the type and severity of communication problems of children with DLD. Method: Clinically referred youngsters (8-16 years old) with (n = 112; 48% girls, 52% boys) and without (n = 233; 58% girls, 42% boys) DLD completed self-reports 3 times over an 18-month period. Parents of children with DLD reported on their children's communication problems. Results: Participants with DLD reported more victimization but comparable levels of bullying behavior compared with peers without DLD. Higher levels of sadness and fear were risk factors for more victimization in both groups. Better understanding of one's own emotions had a larger effect on less victimization in children with DLD, independent of their communication problems. In addition, increased levels of anger and lower levels of understanding of one's own emotions explained more bullying in both groups. Conclusion: Outcomes indicate that secondary difficulties in emotional competence in children with DLD make these children more vulnerable to victimization and warrant specific support and interventions.


Asunto(s)
Acoso Escolar/estadística & datos numéricos , Víctimas de Crimen/estadística & datos numéricos , Emociones , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/psicología , Competencia Mental/psicología , Adolescente , Acoso Escolar/psicología , Niño , Víctimas de Crimen/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Factores de Riesgo
6.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 60(3): 627-639, 2017 03 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28257584

RESUMEN

Purpose: Given the complexity of sentence processing and the specific problems that children with specific language impairment (SLI) experience, we investigated the time course and characteristics of semantic processing at the sentence level in Dutch preschoolers with SLI. Method: We measured N400 responses to semantically congruent and incongruent spoken sentences (e.g., "My father is eating an apple/*blanket") in a group of 37 Dutch preschoolers with SLI and in a group of 25 typically developing (TD) peers. We compared the time course and amplitude of the N400 effect between the two groups. Results: The TD group showed a strong posterior N400 effect in time windows 300-500 ms and 500-800 ms. In contrast, the SLI group demonstrated only a reliable N400 effect in the later time window, 500-800 ms, and did not show a stronger presence at posterior electrodes. Conclusion: The findings suggest that the neuronal processing of semantic information at sentence level is atypical in preschoolers with SLI compared with TD children.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Comprensión/fisiología , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/fisiopatología , Semántica , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Preescolar , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Humanos , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
7.
Front Psychol ; 4: 505, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23986725

RESUMEN

In neurocognitive research on language, the processing principles of the system at hand are usually assumed to be relatively invariant. However, research on attention, memory, decision-making, and social judgment has shown that mood can substantially modulate how the brain processes information. For example, in a bad mood, people typically have a narrower focus of attention and rely less on heuristics. In the face of such pervasive mood effects elsewhere in the brain, it seems unlikely that language processing would remain untouched. In an EEG experiment, we manipulated the mood of participants just before they read texts that confirmed or disconfirmed verb-based expectations about who would be talked about next (e.g., that "David praised Linda because … " would continue about Linda, not David), or that respected or violated a syntactic agreement rule (e.g., "The boys turns"). ERPs showed that mood had little effect on syntactic parsing, but did substantially affect referential anticipation: whereas readers anticipated information about a specific person when they were in a good mood, a bad mood completely abolished such anticipation. A behavioral follow-up experiment suggested that a bad mood did not interfere with verb-based expectations per se, but prevented readers from using that information rapidly enough to predict upcoming reference on the fly, as the sentence unfolds. In all, our results reveal that background mood, a rather unobtrusive affective state, selectively changes a crucial aspect of real-time language processing. This observation fits well with other observed interactions between language processing and affect (emotions, preferences, attitudes, mood), and more generally testifies to the importance of studying "cold" cognitive functions in relation to "hot" aspects of the brain.

8.
Clin Neurophysiol ; 124(7): 1336-45, 2013 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23523114

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Most rhyme awareness assessments do not encompass measures of the global similarity effect (i.e., children who are able to perform simple rhyme judgments get confused when presented with globally similar non-rhyming pairs). The present study examines the neural nature of this effect by studying the N450 rhyme effect. METHODS: Behavioral and electrophysiological responses of Dutch pre-literate kindergartners and literate second graders were recorded while they made rhyme judgments of word pairs in three conditions; phonologically rhyming (e.g., wijn-pijn), overlapping non-rhyming (e.g., pen-pijn) and unrelated non-rhyming pairs (e.g., boom-pijn). RESULTS: Behaviorally, both groups had difficulty judging overlapping but not rhyming and unrelated pairs. The neural data of second graders showed overlapping pairs were processed in a similar fashion as unrelated pairs; both showed a more negative deflection of the N450 component than rhyming items. Kindergartners did not show a typical N450 rhyme effect. However, some other interesting ERP differences were observed, indicating preliterates are sensitive to rhyme at a certain level. SIGNIFICANCE: Rhyme judgments of globally similar items rely on the same process as rhyme judgments of rhyming and unrelated items. Therefore, incorporating a globally similar condition in rhyme assessments may lead to a more in-depth measure of early phonological awareness skills.


Asunto(s)
Concienciación , Mapeo Encefálico , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Fonética , Estimulación Acústica , Factores de Edad , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Preescolar , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Periodicidad , Estimulación Luminosa , Psicolingüística , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Aprendizaje Verbal
9.
Br J Educ Psychol ; 82(Pt 4): 672-89, 2012 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23025398

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Rhyme awareness is one of the earliest forms of phonological awareness to develop and is assessed in many developmental studies by means of a simple rhyme task. The influence of more demanding experimental paradigms on rhyme judgment performance is often neglected. Addressing this issue may also shed light on whether rhyme processing is more global or analytical in nature. AIMS: The aim of the present study was to examine whether lexical status and global similarity relations influenced rhyme judgments in kindergarten children and if so, if there is an interaction between these two factors. SAMPLE: Participants were 41 monolingual Dutch-speaking preliterate kindergartners (average age 6.0 years) who had not yet received any formal reading education. METHOD: To examine the effects of lexical status and phonological similarity processing, the kindergartners were asked to make rhyme judgements on (pseudo) word targets that rhymed, phonologically overlapped or were unrelated to (pseudo) word primes. RESULTS: Both a lexicality effect (pseudo-words were more difficult than words) and a global similarity effect (globally similar non-rhyming items were more difficult to reject than unrelated items) were observed. In addition, whereas in words the global similarity effect was only present in accuracy outcomes, in pseudo-words it was also observed in the response latencies. Furthermore, a large global similarity effect in pseudo-words correlated with a low score on short-term memory skills and grapheme knowledge. CONCLUSIONS: Increasing task demands led to a more detailed assessment of rhyme processing skills. Current assessment paradigms should therefore be extended with more demanding conditions. In light of the views on rhyme processing, we propose that a combination of global and analytical strategies is used to make a correct rhyme judgment.


Asunto(s)
Concienciación , Fonética , Lectura , Aprendizaje Verbal , Atención , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Países Bajos , Semántica
10.
Front Psychol ; 3: 190, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22715332

RESUMEN

During spoken language interpretation, listeners rapidly relate the meaning of each individual word to what has been said before. However, spoken words often contain spurious other words, like day in daisy, or dean in sardine. Do listeners also relate the meaning of such unintended, spurious words to the prior context? We used ERPs to look for transient meaning-based N400 effects in sentences that were completely plausible at the level of words intended by the speaker, but contained an embedded word whose meaning clashed with the context. Although carrier words with an initial embedding (day in daisy) did not elicit an embedding-related N400 effect relative to matched control words without embedding, carrier words with a final embedding (dean in sardine) did elicit such an effect. Together with prior work from our lab and the results of a Shortlist B simulation, our findings suggest that listeners do semantically interpret embedded words, albeit not under all conditions. We explain the latter by assuming that the sense-making system adjusts its hypothesis for how to interpret the external input at every new syllable, in line with recent ideas of active sampling in perception.

11.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 24(4): 878-87, 2012 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21812565

RESUMEN

How do native listeners process grammatical errors that are frequent in non-native speech? We investigated whether the neural correlates of syntactic processing are modulated by speaker identity. ERPs to gender agreement errors in sentences spoken by a native speaker were compared with the same errors spoken by a non-native speaker. In line with previous research, gender violations in native speech resulted in a P600 effect (larger P600 for violations in comparison with correct sentences), but when the same violations were produced by the non-native speaker with a foreign accent, no P600 effect was observed. Control sentences with semantic violations elicited comparable N400 effects for both the native and the non-native speaker, confirming no general integration problem in foreign-accented speech. The results demonstrate that the P600 is modulated by speaker identity, extending our knowledge about the role of speaker's characteristics on neural correlates of speech processing.


Asunto(s)
Comprensión/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Lenguaje , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Traducción , Adulto Joven
12.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 22(11): 2618-26, 2010 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19702463

RESUMEN

In an ERP experiment, we examined whether listeners, when making sense of spoken utterances, take into account the meaning of spurious words that are embedded in longer words, either at their onsets (e.g., pie in pirate) or at their offsets (e.g., pain in champagne). In the experiment, Dutch listeners heard Dutch words with initial or final embeddings presented in a sentence context that did or did not support the meaning of the embedded word, while equally supporting the longer carrier word. The N400 at the carrier words was modulated by the semantic fit of the embedded words, indicating that listeners briefly relate the meaning of initial- and final-embedded words to the sentential context, even though these words were not intended by the speaker. These findings help us understand the dynamics of initial sense-making and its link to lexical activation. In addition, they shed new light on the role of lexical competition and the debate concerning the lexical activation of final-embedded words.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Semántica , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Vocabulario , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
13.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 32(1): 178-96, 2006 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16478335

RESUMEN

Effects on spoken-word recognition of prevoicing differences in Dutch initial voiced plosives were examined. In 2 cross-modal identity-priming experiments, participants heard prime words and nonwords beginning with voiced plosives with 12, 6, or 0 periods of prevoicing or matched items beginning with voiceless plosives and made lexical decisions to visual tokens of those items. Six-period primes had the same effect as 12-period primes. Zero-period primes had a different effect, but only when their voiceless counterparts were real words. Listeners could nevertheless discriminate the 6-period primes from the 12- and 0-period primes. Phonetic detail appears to influence lexical access only to the extent that it is useful: In Dutch, presence versus absence of prevoicing is more informative than amount of prevoicing.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Aprendizaje por Asociación de Pares , Fonética , Percepción del Habla , Toma de Decisiones , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Lectura , Semántica , Acústica del Lenguaje
14.
Dyslexia ; 10(4): 265-88, 2004 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15573960

RESUMEN

We report on a prospective longitudinal research programme exploring the connection between language acquisition deficits and dyslexia. The language development profile of children at-risk for dyslexia is compared to that of age-matched controls as well as of children who have been diagnosed with specific language impairment (SLI). The experiments described concern the perception and production of grammatical morphology, categorical perception of speech sounds, phonological processing (non-word repetition), mispronunciation detection, and rhyme detection. The results of each of these indicate that the at-risk children as a group underperform in comparison to the controls, and that, in most cases, they approach the SLI group. It can be concluded that dyslexia most likely has precursors in language development, also in domains other than those traditionally considered conditional for the acquisition of literacy skills. The dyslexia-SLI connection awaits further, particularly qualitative, analyses.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia/epidemiología , Dislexia/genética , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/epidemiología , Preescolar , Dislexia/diagnóstico , Femenino , Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad , Humanos , Lactante , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Lingüística , Masculino , Fonética , Factores de Riesgo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA