Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 60
Filtrar
1.
Anim Cogn ; 27(1): 8, 2024 Mar 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38429588

RESUMEN

Predation risk may affect the foraging behavior of birds. However, there has been little research on the ability of domestic birds to perceive predation risk and thus adjust their feeding behavior. In this study, we tested whether domestic budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) perceived predation risk after the presentation of specimens and sounds of sparrowhawks (Accipiter nisus), domestic cats (Felis catus), and humans, and whether this in turn influenced their feeding behavior. When exposed to visual or acoustic stimuli, budgerigars showed significantly longer latency to feed under sparrowhawk, domestic cat, and human treatments than with controls. Budgerigars responded more strongly to acoustic stimuli than visual stimuli, and they showed the longest latency to feed and the least number of feeding times in response to sparrowhawk calls. Moreover, budgerigars showed shorter latency to feed and greater numbers of feeding times in response to human voices than to sparrowhawk or domestic cat calls. Our results suggest that domestic budgerigars may identify predation risk through visual or acoustic signals and adjust their feeding behavior accordingly.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Melopsittacus , Humanos , Animales , Gatos , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Melopsittacus/fisiología , Conducta Predatoria , Acústica , Sonido
2.
Int. microbiol ; 27(1): 127-142, Feb. 2024. graf
Artículo en Inglés | IBECS | ID: ibc-230249

RESUMEN

Digestive and respiratory tracts are inhabited by rich bacterial communities that can vary between their different segments. In comparison with other bird taxa with developed caeca, parrots that lack caeca have relatively lower variability in intestinal morphology. Here, based on 16S rRNA metabarcoding, we describe variation in microbiota across different parts of parrot digestive and respiratory tracts both at interspecies and intraspecies levels. In domesticated budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus), we describe the bacterial variation across eight selected sections of respiratory and digestive tracts, and three non-destructively collected sample types (faeces, and cloacal and oral swabs). Our results show important microbiota divergence between the upper and lower digestive tract, but similarities between respiratory tract and crop, and also between different intestinal segments. Faecal samples appear to provide a better proxy for intestinal microbiota composition than the cloacal swabs. Oral swabs had a similar bacterial composition as the crop and trachea. For a subset of tissues, we confirmed the same pattern also in six different parrot species. Finally, using the faeces and oral swabs in budgerigars, we revealed high oral, but low faecal microbiota stability during a 3-week period mimicking pre-experiment acclimation. Our findings provide a basis essential for microbiota-related experimental planning and result generalisation in non-poultry birds.(AU)


Asunto(s)
Humanos , Animales , Loros/metabolismo , Tracto Gastrointestinal/microbiología , Microbiota , Bacterias/genética , ARN Ribosómico 16S/genética , Sistema Respiratorio/microbiología , Tracto Gastrointestinal/metabolismo , Microbiología , Técnicas Microbiológicas , Microbiota/genética , Periquitos
3.
Int Microbiol ; 27(1): 127-142, 2024 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37222909

RESUMEN

Digestive and respiratory tracts are inhabited by rich bacterial communities that can vary between their different segments. In comparison with other bird taxa with developed caeca, parrots that lack caeca have relatively lower variability in intestinal morphology. Here, based on 16S rRNA metabarcoding, we describe variation in microbiota across different parts of parrot digestive and respiratory tracts both at interspecies and intraspecies levels. In domesticated budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus), we describe the bacterial variation across eight selected sections of respiratory and digestive tracts, and three non-destructively collected sample types (faeces, and cloacal and oral swabs). Our results show important microbiota divergence between the upper and lower digestive tract, but similarities between respiratory tract and crop, and also between different intestinal segments. Faecal samples appear to provide a better proxy for intestinal microbiota composition than the cloacal swabs. Oral swabs had a similar bacterial composition as the crop and trachea. For a subset of tissues, we confirmed the same pattern also in six different parrot species. Finally, using the faeces and oral swabs in budgerigars, we revealed high oral, but low faecal microbiota stability during a 3-week period mimicking pre-experiment acclimation. Our findings provide a basis essential for microbiota-related experimental planning and result generalisation in non-poultry birds.


Asunto(s)
Microbiota , Loros , Animales , Loros/genética , ARN Ribosómico 16S/genética , Sistema Respiratorio/microbiología , Bacterias/genética
4.
Hear Res ; 441: 108927, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38096707

RESUMEN

Cochlear synaptopathy is a common pathology in humans associated with aging and potentially sound overexposure. Synaptopathy is widely expected to cause "hidden hearing loss," including difficulty perceiving speech in noise, but support for this hypothesis is controversial. Here in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus), we evaluated the impact of long-term cochlear synaptopathy on behavioral discrimination of Gaussian noise (GN) and low-noise noise (LNN) signals processed to have a flatter envelope. Stimuli had center frequencies of 1-3kHz, 100-Hz bandwidth, and were presented at sensation levels (SLs) from 10 to 30dB. We reasoned that narrowband, low-SL stimuli of this type should minimize spread of excitation across auditory-nerve fibers, and hence might reveal synaptopathy-related defects if they exist. Cochlear synaptopathy was induced without hair-cell injury using kainic acid (KA). Behavioral threshold tracking experiments characterized the minimum stimulus duration above which animals could reliably discriminate between LNN and GN. Budgerigar thresholds for LNN-GN discrimination ranged from 40 to 60ms at 30dB SL, were similar across frequencies, and increased for lower SLs. Notably, animals with long-term 39-77% estimated synaptopathy performed similarly to controls, requiring on average a ∼7.5% shorter stimulus duration (-0.7±1.0dB; mean difference ±SE) for LNN-GN discrimination. Decision-variable correlation analyses of detailed behavioral response patterns showed that individual animals relied on envelope cues to discriminate LNN and GN, with lesser roles of FM and energy cues; no difference was found between KA-exposed and control groups. These results suggest that long-term cochlear synaptopathy does not impair discrimination of low-level signals with different envelope statistics.


Asunto(s)
Pérdida Auditiva Provocada por Ruido , Melopsittacus , Humanos , Animales , Cóclea/patología , Ácido Kaínico/toxicidad , Estimulación Acústica/efectos adversos , Umbral Auditivo/fisiología , Pérdida de Audición Oculta , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos del Tronco Encefálico/fisiología , Pérdida Auditiva Provocada por Ruido/etiología , Pérdida Auditiva Provocada por Ruido/patología
5.
Curr Biol ; 33(24): 5415-5426.e4, 2023 12 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38070505

RESUMEN

Parrots have enormous vocal imitation capacities and produce individually unique vocal signatures. Like songbirds, parrots have a nucleated neural song system with distinct anterior (AFP) and posterior forebrain pathways (PFP). To test if song systems of parrots and songbirds, which diverged over 50 million years ago, have a similar functional organization, we first established a neuroscience-compatible call-and-response behavioral paradigm to elicit learned contact calls in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Using variational autoencoder-based machine learning methods, we show that contact calls within affiliated groups converge but that individuals maintain unique acoustic features, or vocal signatures, even after call convergence. Next, we transiently inactivated the outputs of AFP to test if learned vocalizations can be produced by the PFP alone. As in songbirds, AFP inactivation had an immediate effect on vocalizations, consistent with a premotor role. But in contrast to songbirds, where the isolated PFP is sufficient to produce stereotyped and acoustically normal vocalizations, isolation of the budgerigar PFP caused a degradation of call acoustic structure, stereotypy, and individual uniqueness. Thus, the contribution of AFP and the capacity of isolated PFP to produce learned vocalizations have diverged substantially between songbirds and parrots, likely driven by their distinct behavioral ecology and neural connectivity.


Asunto(s)
Loros , Pájaros Cantores , Voz , Animales , Humanos , Loros/fisiología , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , alfa-Fetoproteínas , Prosencéfalo
6.
Front Vet Sci ; 10: 1309185, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38144467

RESUMEN

A 2-year-old male budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus) died after a 1-day history of fracture of the rostral rhinotheca with pale mucous membranes, dyspnea, dull mentation, and ataxia. Histopathology revealed an infiltrative neoplasm composed of interweaving streams of spindle cells effacing the dermis and bone of the rostral upper beak as well as a ganglion and two cranial nerves. No visceral metastasis was observed. Neoplastic cells exhibited strong cytoplasmic immunolabeling for alpha-smooth muscle actin (α-SMA) and lacked immunolabeling for S100, Melan-A, PNL2, and cytokeratin AE1/AE3. These findings were consistent with a locally invasive leiomyosarcoma Leiomyosarcomas arise from the smooth muscle and are locally invasive with rare metastases. In birds, leiomyosarcomas are mostly reported to arise from the spleen, gastrointestinal, and reproductive tracts. In the case report herein, we describe the histological and immunohistochemical features of a primary beak leiomyosarcoma in a budgerigar associated with a fracture located at the rostral rhinotheca. Leiomyosarcoma arising from the beak has not been described in the literature.

7.
Microbiol Resour Announc ; 12(11): e0045323, 2023 Nov 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37811960

RESUMEN

Here, we report a parvovirus genome identified in Melopsittacus undulatus. The genome is 4,547 bp long and codes for two major open reading frames (ORFs): the non-structural replicase protein 1 (NS1) and the structural capsid gene (VP1). Phylogenetic analysis shows that this virus belongs to the genus Chaphamaparvovirus.

8.
J Assoc Res Otolaryngol ; 24(5): 473-485, 2023 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37798548

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Loss of auditory nerve afferent synapses with cochlear hair cells, called cochlear synaptopathy, is a common pathology in humans caused by aging and noise overexposure. The perceptual consequences of synaptopathy in isolation from other cochlear pathologies are still unclear. Animal models provide an effective approach to resolve uncertainty regarding the physiological and perceptual consequences of auditory nerve loss, because neural lesions can be induced and readily quantified. The budgerigar, a parakeet species, has recently emerged as an animal model for synaptopathy studies based on its capacity for vocal learning and ability to behaviorally discriminate simple and complex sounds with acuity similar to humans. Kainic acid infusions in the budgerigar produce a profound reduction of compound auditory nerve responses, including wave I of the auditory brainstem response, without impacting physiological hair cell measures. These results suggest selective auditory nerve damage. However, histological correlates of neural injury from kainic acid are still lacking. METHODS: We quantified the histological effects caused by intracochlear infusion of kainic acid (1 mM; 2.5 µL), and evaluated correlations between the histological and physiological assessments of auditory nerve status. RESULTS: Kainic acid infusion in budgerigars produced pronounced loss of neural auditory nerve soma (60% on average) in the cochlear ganglion, and of peripheral axons, at time points 2 or more months following injury. The hair cell epithelium was unaffected by kainic acid. Neural loss was significantly correlated with reduction of compound auditory nerve responses and auditory brainstem response wave I. CONCLUSION: Compound auditory nerve responses and wave I provide a useful index of cochlear synaptopathy in this animal model.


Asunto(s)
Pérdida Auditiva Provocada por Ruido , Melopsittacus , Humanos , Animales , Ácido Kaínico/toxicidad , Estimulación Acústica , Umbral Auditivo/fisiología , Nervio Coclear , Cóclea/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos del Tronco Encefálico/fisiología , Sinapsis
9.
Avian Pathol ; 52(6): 420-425, 2023 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37565272

RESUMEN

Budgerigar fledgling disease (BFD) is a contagious disease caused by avian polyomavirus (APV) in psittacine birds and causes high mortality rates. Here, eight APV-positive cases were confirmed from dead parrots or parrot tissue samples by polymerase chain reaction (PCR). Full-length genome sequencing showed high nucleotide identity (98.84-100%) between the APV strains. Phylogenetic analysis revealed that two genogroups were cocirculating in South Korea. The nucleotide sequences of five strains, collected from different parrot species, were identical; however, pathological lesions were observed in only two parrots, both aged 2 months. Pathology included necrotic spots in the liver, subcutaneous haemorrhage, hepatomegaly, ascites, intranuclear inclusion bodies, hepatocyte karyomegaly, hepatic necrosis, and bile duct proliferation. This suggests that the pathogenicity of APV might be host age-dependent regardless of the host species. This study improves our understanding of APV pathogenicity and provides a more detailed genetic characterization of APV strains.RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS Eight APV strains were identified in South Korea from 2019 to 2021.By phylogenetic analysis, South Korean APV strains were classified into two clades.

10.
Hear Res ; 435: 108812, 2023 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37269601

RESUMEN

Schroeder-phase harmonic tone complexes can have a flat temporal envelope and rising or falling instantaneous-frequency sweeps within F0 periods, depending on the phase-scaling parameter C. Human tone-detection thresholds in a concurrent Schroeder masker are 10-15 dB lower for positive C values (rising frequency sweeps) compared to negative (falling sweeps), potentially due to cochlear mechanics, though this hypothesis remains controversial. Birds provide an interesting model for studies of Schroeder masking because many species produce vocalizations containing frequency sweeps. Prior behavioral studies in birds suggest less behavioral threshold difference between maskers with opposite C values than in humans, but focused on low masker F0s and did not explore neural mechanisms. We performed behavioral Schroeder-masking experiments in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) using a wide range of masker F0 and C values. Signal frequency was 2800 Hz. Neural recordings from the midbrain characterized encoding of behavioral stimuli in awake animals. Behavioral thresholds increased with increasing masker F0 and showed minimal difference between opposite C values, consistent with prior budgerigar studies. Midbrain recordings showed prominent temporal and rate-based encoding of Schroeder F0, and in many cases, marked asymmetry in Schroeder responses between C polarities. Neural thresholds for Schroeder-masked tone detection were often based on a response decrement compared to the masker alone, consistent with prominent modulation tuning in midbrain neurons, and were generally similar between opposite C values. The results highlight the likely importance of envelope cues in Schroeder masking and show that differences in supra-threshold Schroeder responses do not necessarily result in neural threshold differences.


Asunto(s)
Melopsittacus , Humanos , Animales , Umbral Auditivo/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Cóclea/fisiología
11.
Proc Biol Sci ; 290(1998): 20230596, 2023 05 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37161333

RESUMEN

Many birds move fast into dark nest cavities forcing the visual system to adapt to low light intensities. Their visual system takes between 15 and 60 min for complete dark adaptation, but little is known about the visual performance of birds during the first seconds in low light intensities. In a forced two-choice behavioural experiment we studied how well budgerigars can discriminate stimuli of different luminance directly after entering a darker environment. The birds made their choices within about 1 s and did not wait to adapt their visual system to the low light intensities. When moving from a bright facility into an environment with 0.5 log unit lower illuminance, the budgerigars detected targets with a luminance of 0.825 cd m-2 on a black background. When moving into an environment with 1.7 or 3.5 log units lower illuminance, they detected targets with luminances between 0.106 and 0.136 cd m-2. In tests with two simultaneously displayed targets, the birds discriminated similar luminance differences between the targets (Weber fraction of 0.41-0.54) in all light levels. Our results support the notion that partial adaptation of bird eyes to the lower illumination occurring within 1 s allows them to safely detect and feed their chicks.


Asunto(s)
Aclimatación , Melopsittacus , Animales , Luz , Pollos , Ojo , Iluminación
12.
Animals (Basel) ; 12(18)2022 Sep 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36139315

RESUMEN

This study examined the effects of exposure to coloured LED lights on some behavioural responses, food and water consumption, and live weight in budgerigars kept in captivity using behavioural tests to compare different lighting conditions. Budgerigars' feeding, comfort, social, fear, and resting behaviours and behavioural reactions to a new environment were studied. Twelve male birds were assigned to four groups, with three birds in each group. They were provided with food and water ad libitum in wire cages at 25 ± 2 °C in a room. The study was conducted with 10 h of natural light, 6 h of LED light (white, blue, yellow, and red LED lights), and 8 h of darkness. Their behaviours in the cages were observed. Home cage avoidance (HCA) and two open field tests (OFT) were performed. No difference was observed in the LW and food consumption, but the blue and yellow groups consumed less water. The blue group displayed more comfort and social behaviours. In the HCA test, the red group demonstrated higher reactions. Furthermore, the birds in the yellow group stayed closer to the centre of the platform in the OFT I test, and they chose the compartment simulating their familiar environment in the OFT II test. Consequently, it was concluded that first yellow and then blue lights may be used to prevent stress-related problems for these birds. Our findings could be used to improve the hand-rearing conditions of captive budgerigars.

13.
Behav Processes ; 202: 104742, 2022 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36038023

RESUMEN

Auditory patterns carry information in human speech at multiple levels, including the surface relationships between sounds within words in phonology and the abstract structures of syntax. The sequences of other animal vocalizations, such as birdsong, can also be described as auditory patterns, but few studies have probed how the sequences are perceived at multiple levels. Past work shows that a small parrot species, the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus), exceeds other birds in sequence perception and is even sensitive to abstract structure. But it is not known what level of auditory analysis is dominant in perception or what limits might exist in sensitivity to abstract structure. Here, budgerigars were tested on their ability to discriminate changes in an auditory pattern, AAB, i.e. sound-same different, to ask how they attended to surface relationships among the sounds and the abstract relationships of same/different among the elements. The results show that the budgerigars primarily used surface transitions between the sounds when discriminating the sequences, but were able to use the abstract relationships to a limited extent, largely restricted to two elements. This study provides insight into how budgerigars extract information from conspecific vocalizations and how their capacities compare to human speech perception.


Asunto(s)
Melopsittacus , Animales , Percepción Auditiva , Discriminación en Psicología , Humanos , Especificidad de la Especie , Vocalización Animal
14.
J Vet Sci ; 23(5): e67, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36038188

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Budgerigar fledgling disease polyomavirus (BFDV) is the pathogen that causes budgerigar fledgling disease in psittacine species. The clinical signs of PBFV infection include ascites, hepatitis, and crop stasis. BFDV is associated with a high mortality rate in nestling birds. In contrast, adult birds only have mild symptoms such as feather dystrophy. OBJECTIVES: This study aimed to determine the prevalence, genetic characteristics, and phylogenetic analysis of BFDV in pet parrots in Korea. METHODS: Fecal and tissue samples were collected from 217 pet parrots from 10 veterinary hospitals including Chungbuk National University Veterinary Hospital. The molecular screening was performed using polymerase chain reaction (PCR) analysis of the small t/large T antigen gene segment. Full-length genome sequencing with the Sanger and phylogenetic analysis were performed on BFDV-positive samples. RESULTS: The PCR results based on the small t/large T antigen gene marker indicated that BFDV DNA was present in 10 out of 217 screened samples. A whole-genome sequence was obtained from six strains and phylogenetic analysis revealed no significant relationship existed between the species and geographical locations amongst them. CONCLUSIONS: The prevalence of BFDV infection in South Korea is not high when compared to the prevalence of BFDV in other parts of the world, however, it has been reported sporadically in various species and geographic locations. The whole-genome analysis revealed 0.2%-0.3% variation in intragenomic homogeneity among the six strains analyzed. Korean strains are separately on the phylogenetic tree from their counterparts from China and Japan which might reflect the substantial genetic variation.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedades de las Aves , Infecciones por Circoviridae , Circovirus , Melopsittacus , Loros , Poliomavirus , Animales , Antígenos Virales de Tumores , Enfermedades de las Aves/epidemiología , Infecciones por Circoviridae/veterinaria , Circovirus/genética , Filogenia , Poliomavirus/genética
15.
Curr Zool ; 68(3): 315-323, 2022 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35592343

RESUMEN

In recent years, researchers have been attempting to relate differences in personality (e.g., boldness, aggressiveness, exploration tendency) to variation in cognition (performances in tasks that require learning, reasoning, attention, or memory, etc.) both theoretically and empirically. However, it is unclear on what basis personality and cognition might be associated with each other. Previous theory suggests a connection between fast-slow personality types and cognitive speed-accuracy tradeoffs. In this study, we tested this hypothesis in budgerigars and found that, in their 1st associative learning, birds with fast personality (less fearful of handling stress) were fast learners in the beginning, while slow personality individuals improved faster, but both types of birds did not differ in accuracy. However, these relationships were context-dependent. No significant relationship was found in subsequent learning tasks (reversal learning and a 2nd associative learning) in the familiar context (task setup and apparatus similar to the 1st associative learning). We then conducted a problem-solving experiment with novel setup and apparatus to test 1 possible explanation that the association between personality and cognition in the 1st associative learning might be caused by noncognitive constraint, such as fearfulness when facing novel task setup and apparatus. We found that fast individuals interacted more with the problem box and solved it, whereas the slow birds were not. We suggest that personalities can influence cognitive performances and trigger a cognitive speed-improvement tradeoff under the novel context. However, there are no consistent cognitive styles that co-varied with different personalities.

16.
J Avian Med Surg ; 36(1): 53-57, 2022 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35526164

RESUMEN

Currently, drug-induced stimulation of appetite is not commonly performed in hyporexic or anorexic companion psittacine birds. Instead, to prevent a catabolic state and weight loss, supplemental feedings are routinely performed by crop gavage. However, crop gavage is not without complications and is stressful to the patient and labor intensive. The objective of this study was to evaluate the effect of midazolam on food intake in healthy budgerigars. In a randomized, blinded, controlled study, change in food intake after intramuscular administration of midazolam (1 mg/kg) or a placebo-control treatment (0.9% saline) was evaluated in 12 healthy adult budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Food intake was quantified for 1 hour before and after drug administration. Birds were monitored for feeding behavior as well as signs of sedation. After midazolam administration, a median 6-fold (1.1-28) increase in food intake was recorded. In 3 of 6 (50%) birds, the food intake increase after midazolam administration was >10-fold (median 17-fold [10-28]), whereas in the remaining 3 birds, food intake increased by only 1.7-fold (1.1-1.8). The median amount of food ingested (16.7 g/kg [3.2-43.2 g/kg]) was significantly higher after midazolam administration compared with the control group (1.9 g/kg [0.0-19.7 g/kg], P = .015). The median time birds spent displaying feeding behavior after the midazolam injection was 18% (0-43%), compared with 1% (0-20%) in the control group after saline injection. Five of 6 (83%) birds showed signs consistent with mild sedation after midazolam administration. This study demonstrates that midazolam is an appetite stimulant in budgerigars. Future studies are needed to evaluate whether midazolam's effects on food intake are dose dependent and whether the duration of effect exceeds 1 hour.


Asunto(s)
Melopsittacus , Loros , Animales , Ingestión de Alimentos , Melopsittacus/fisiología , Midazolam/farmacología
17.
Vet Pathol ; 59(2): 333-339, 2022 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35001765

RESUMEN

Medullary bone is a calcium-rich, labile bone normally occurring in female birds with each egg-laying cycle. The stimulus for formation of medullary bone is, in part, the cyclic increase in serum estrogens produced by preovulatory ovarian follicles. Increased bone density due to formation of medullary bone, particularly in pneumatic bones, has been termed polyostotic hyperostosis, even if physiologic. This study investigated the formation of medullary bone in nonpneumatic (femur) and pneumatic (humerus) bones in sexually mature male budgerigars submitted for autopsy. Of the 21 sexually mature male budgerigars submitted for autopsy, 7 (33%) had medullary bone in 1 or more bones examined. All 7 male budgerigars with medullary bone had a testicular neoplasm, which was morphologically consistent with a testicular sustentacular cell tumor, seminoma, or interstitial cell tumor. Medullary bone was not present in the 14 cases with other diseases. Medullary bone formation in pneumatic and nonpneumatic bones can occur in male budgerigars with testicular neoplasms. Radiographic increases in medullary bone density, particularly in the humerus, could provide antemortem indication of testicular neoplasia in male budgerigars.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedades de las Aves , Melopsittacus , Neoplasias Testiculares , Animales , Enfermedades de las Aves/patología , Femenino , Masculino , Neoplasias Testiculares/veterinaria
18.
Vet Pathol ; 59(1): 143-151, 2022 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34794345

RESUMEN

Xanthogranulomatosis is an inflammatory lesion characterized by lipid-containing macrophages, extracellular lipid, hemorrhage, and necrosis. We describe disseminated intracoelomic xanthogranulomatosis in 5 eclectus parrots (Eclectus roratus) and 2 budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Postmortem, clinicopathologic, and historical case material was reviewed. Ages ranged from 3 to 24 years; there were 5 males and 2 females. Table food was included in the diet of 3/5 cases, and animal products were included in 2/3 cases. Common clinicopathologic abnormalities included leukocytosis (4/5 cases) and elevated concentrations of bile acids (3/4 cases) and cholesterol within 6 months prior to death (2/4 cases). At postmortem examination, all 7 birds had grossly visible, irregular, soft, tan to yellow, amorphous plaques distributed on the surfaces of the viscera and body wall. Histologic evaluation and oil red O stain revealed xanthogranulomatous inflammation with phagocytized and extracellular lipid, necrosis, cholesterol clefts, fibrosis, and mineralization. Infectious agents were not identified with special stains in all cases. Concurrent hepatobiliary disease was present in 6/7 cases, and 6/7 had lipid accumulation within the parenchyma of various visceral organs. Five cases had atherosclerosis of great vessels. We describe a unique form of disseminated coelomic xanthogranulomatosis in 2 psittacine species. This condition should be recognized as a differential diagnosis in cases of disseminated coelomic mass formation and coelomic distension in psittacine birds, particularly in eclectus parrots and budgerigars.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedades de las Aves , Melopsittacus , Loros , Animales , Enfermedades de las Aves/diagnóstico , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Femenino , Masculino
19.
J Neurosci ; 41(34): 7206-7223, 2021 08 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34266898

RESUMEN

Hearing in noise is a problem often assumed to depend on encoding of energy level by channels tuned to target frequencies, but few studies have tested this hypothesis. The present study examined neural correlates of behavioral tone-in-noise (TIN) detection in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus, either sex), a parakeet species with human-like behavioral sensitivity to many simple and complex sounds. Behavioral sensitivity to tones in band-limited noise was assessed using operant-conditioning procedures. Neural recordings were made in awake animals from midbrain-level neurons in the inferior colliculus, the first processing stage of the ascending auditory pathway with pronounced rate-based encoding of stimulus amplitude modulation. Budgerigar TIN detection thresholds were similar to human thresholds across the full range of frequencies (0.5-4 kHz) and noise levels (45-85 dB SPL) tested. Also as in humans, thresholds were minimally affected by a challenging roving-level condition with random variation in background-noise level. Many midbrain neurons showed a decreasing response rate as TIN signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) was increased by elevating the tone level, a pattern attributable to amplitude-modulation tuning in these cells and the fact that higher SNR tone-plus-noise stimuli have flatter amplitude envelopes. TIN thresholds of individual neurons were as sensitive as behavioral thresholds under most conditions, perhaps surprisingly even when the unit's characteristic frequency was tuned an octave or more away from the test frequency. A model that combined responses of two cell types enhanced TIN sensitivity in the roving-level condition. These results highlight the importance of midbrain-level envelope encoding and off-frequency neural channels for hearing in noise.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Detection of target sounds in noise is often assumed to depend on energy-level encoding by neural processing channels tuned to the target frequency. In contrast, we found that tone-in-noise sensitivity in budgerigars was often greatest in midbrain neurons not tuned to the test frequency, underscoring the potential importance of off-frequency channels for perception. Furthermore, the results highlight the importance of envelope processing for hearing in noise, especially under challenging conditions with random variation in background noise level over time.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Acústica , Vías Auditivas/fisiología , Umbral Auditivo/fisiología , Condicionamiento Operante/fisiología , Colículos Inferiores/fisiología , Melopsittacus/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Relación Señal-Ruido , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Señales (Psicología) , Electrodos Implantados , Femenino , Colículos Inferiores/citología , Masculino , Ruido , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...