Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
Más filtros











Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Biol Cybern ; 117(4-5): 389-406, 2023 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37733033

RESUMEN

Foveation can be defined as the organic action of directing the gaze towards a visual region of interest to acquire relevant information selectively. With the recent advent of event cameras, we believe that taking advantage of this visual neuroscience mechanism would greatly improve the efficiency of event data processing. Indeed, applying foveation to event data would allow to comprehend the visual scene while significantly reducing the amount of raw data to handle. In this respect, we demonstrate the stakes of neuromorphic foveation theoretically and empirically across several computer vision tasks, namely semantic segmentation and classification. We show that foveated event data have a significantly better trade-off between quantity and quality of the information conveyed than high- or low-resolution event data. Furthermore, this compromise extends even over fragmented datasets. Our code is publicly available online at: https://github.com/amygruel/FoveationStakes_DVS .


Asunto(s)
Computadores , Visión Ocular
2.
Am J Ophthalmol Case Rep ; 32: 101923, 2023 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37693691

RESUMEN

Purpose: To describe the mechanisms of postoperative foveal restoration in three patients with bacillary layer detachment (BALAD) associated with macula-off rhegmatogenous retinal detachment. Observations: BALAD associated with macula-off rhegmatogenous retinal detachment presented with two different morphologies: with an intact foveal roof (case 1) and lamellar, with an open foveal roof (cases 2 and 3). In case 1 visual acuity significantly improved and foveal morphology completely restored at postoperative month 6, with a marked increase in foveal thickness. Case 2 presented with a lamellar BALAD in a long-standing retinal detachment, and it was treated with scleral buckling with an unfavourable evolution into a full-thickness hole in the early postoperative period. It was then addressed with internal limiting membrane peeling and inverted flap, which resulted in the resolution of the lesion, but with limited postoperative visual and anatomical recovery. Case 3 lamellar BALAD was directly addressed with pars plana vitrectomy, ILM peeling and inverted flap, with a remarkable foveal anatomical restoration and visual acuity improvement over the follow-up period. Conclusions and importance: The process of foveation may play a key role in the healing process of BALAD associated with rhegmatogenous retinal detachment. Lamellar BALAD should be considered and treated as a FTMH associated with retinal detachment. The two BALAD subtypes may represent different clinical stages of the BALAD spectrum.

3.
J. optom. (Internet) ; 15(4)October - December 2022. tab, graf
Artículo en Inglés | IBECS | ID: ibc-210188

RESUMEN

Purpose: This pilot study aimed to investigate the maximum extension of foveal fixation in the horizontal direction among young adults in both emmetropes and myopes.Methods35 participants (28 emmetropes and 7 myopes) were included. Participants with restricted extra-ocular mobility, end gaze nystagmus, and/or any other ocular pathology were excluded. Visual acuity (VA) was used as a surrogate measure of foveal fixation. VA was determined using a staircase procedure with 8 reversals. The average of the last 5 reversals was taken as the thresholds. VA acuity was measured at different gaze eccentricities along nasal and temporal visual field meridian. The eccentricity at which VA drops significantly was taken as the maximum extent of foveal fixation. A bilinear fit regression model was used to investigate the drop in the VA in both nasal and the temporal direction.ResultsEmmetropes can foveate up to 35 ± 2° in nasal and 40 ± 3° in temporal direction and myopes can foveate up to 38° in both nasal and temporal directions. Paired student t-test showed a significant difference in foveal fixation between nasal and temporal direction for emmetropes (P<0.001) but not in myopes (P = 0.168). An unpaired student t-test showed a significant difference in foveal fixation for nasal direction between myopes and emmetropes (P = 0.01). However, no statistically significant difference was found in foveal fixation for temporal direction between myopes and emmetropes (P = 0.792).ConclusionThe eye rotation does not necessarily match with the extent of foveal fixation at extreme eye rotation. Eyes can fixate only up to 35° nasally and 40° temporally maintaing their maximum visual acuity. (AU)


Asunto(s)
Humanos , Adulto Joven , Emetropía , Fóvea Central , Miopía , Proyectos Piloto , Rotación , Agudeza Visual
4.
J Optom ; 15(4): 293-298, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34952806

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: This pilot study aimed to investigate the maximum extension of foveal fixation in the horizontal direction among young adults in both emmetropes and myopes. METHODS: 35 participants (28 emmetropes and 7 myopes) were included. Participants with restricted extra-ocular mobility, end gaze nystagmus, and/or any other ocular pathology were excluded. Visual acuity (VA) was used as a surrogate measure of foveal fixation. VA was determined using a staircase procedure with 8 reversals. The average of the last 5 reversals was taken as the thresholds. VA acuity was measured at different gaze eccentricities along nasal and temporal visual field meridian. The eccentricity at which VA drops significantly was taken as the maximum extent of foveal fixation. A bilinear fit regression model was used to investigate the drop in the VA in both nasal and the temporal direction. RESULTS: Emmetropes can foveate up to 35 ± 2° in nasal and 40 ± 3° in temporal direction and myopes can foveate up to 38° in both nasal and temporal directions. Paired student t-test showed a significant difference in foveal fixation between nasal and temporal direction for emmetropes (P<0.001) but not in myopes (P = 0.168). An unpaired student t-test showed a significant difference in foveal fixation for nasal direction between myopes and emmetropes (P = 0.01). However, no statistically significant difference was found in foveal fixation for temporal direction between myopes and emmetropes (P = 0.792). CONCLUSION: The eye rotation does not necessarily match with the extent of foveal fixation at extreme eye rotation. Eyes can fixate only up to 35° nasally and 40° temporally maintaing their maximum visual acuity.


Asunto(s)
Emetropía , Miopía , Fóvea Central , Humanos , Proyectos Piloto , Rotación , Adulto Joven
5.
Sensors (Basel) ; 21(12)2021 Jun 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34200616

RESUMEN

Cybersickness is one of the major roadblocks in the widespread adoption of mixed reality devices. Prolonged exposure to these devices, especially virtual reality devices, can cause users to feel discomfort and nausea, spoiling the immersive experience. Incorporating spatial blur in stereoscopic 3D stimuli has shown to reduce cybersickness. In this paper, we develop a technique to incorporate spatial blur in VR systems inspired by the human physiological system. The technique makes use of concepts from foveated imaging and depth-of-field. The developed technique can be applied to any eye tracker equipped VR system as a post-processing step to provide an artifact-free scene. We verify the usefulness of the proposed system by conducting a user study on cybersickness evaluation. We used a custom-built rollercoaster VR environment developed in Unity and an HTC Vive Pro Eye headset to interact with the user. A Simulator Sickness Questionnaire was used to measure the induced sickness while gaze and heart rate data were recorded for quantitative analysis. The experimental analysis highlighted the aptness of our foveated depth-of-field effect in reducing cybersickness in virtual environments by reducing the sickness scores by approximately 66%.


Asunto(s)
Mareo por Movimiento , Realidad Virtual , Emociones , Humanos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Interfaz Usuario-Computador
6.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 6(1): 45, 2021 06 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34170426

RESUMEN

Eye tracking is a useful tool for studying human cognition, both in the laboratory and in real-world applications. However, there are cases in which eye tracking is not possible, such as in high-security environments where recording devices cannot be introduced. After facing this challenge in our own work, we sought to test the effectiveness of using artificial foveation as an alternative to eye tracking for studying visual search performance. Two groups of participants completed the same list comparison task, which was a computer-based task designed to mimic an inventory verification process that is commonly performed by international nuclear safeguards inspectors. We manipulated the way in which the items on the inventory list were ordered and color coded. For the eye tracking group, an eye tracker was used to assess the order in which participants viewed the items and the number of fixations per trial in each list condition. For the artificial foveation group, the items were covered with a blurry mask except when participants moused over them. We tracked the order in which participants viewed the items by moving their mouse and the number of items viewed per trial in each list condition. We observed the same overall pattern of performance for the various list display conditions, regardless of the method. However, participants were much slower to complete the task when using artificial foveation and had more variability in their accuracy. Our results indicate that the artificial foveation method can reveal the same pattern of differences across conditions as eye tracking, but it can also impact participants' task performance.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Movimientos Oculares , Animales , Computadores , Ratones
7.
Sensors (Basel) ; 20(13)2020 Jul 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32635446

RESUMEN

Energy and storage restrictions are relevant variables that software applications should be concerned about when running in low-power environments. In particular, computer vision (CV) applications exemplify well that concern, since conventional uniform image sensors typically capture large amounts of data to be further handled by the appropriate CV algorithms. Moreover, much of the acquired data are often redundant and outside of the application's interest, which leads to unnecessary processing and energy spending. In the literature, techniques for sensing and re-sampling images in non-uniform fashions have emerged to cope with these problems. In this study, we propose Application-Oriented Retinal Image Models that define a space-variant configuration of uniform images and contemplate requirements of energy consumption and storage footprints for CV applications. We hypothesize that our models might decrease energy consumption in CV tasks. Moreover, we show how to create the models and validate their use in a face detection/recognition application, evidencing the compromise between storage, energy, and accuracy.

8.
Sensors (Basel) ; 20(8)2020 Apr 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32316257

RESUMEN

Pan-tilt-zoom (PTZ) and omnidirectional cameras serve as a video-mediated communication interface for telemedicine. Most cases use either PTZ or omnidirectional cameras exclusively; even when used together, images from the two are shown separately on 2D displays. Conventional foveated imaging techniques may offer a solution for exploiting the benefits of both cameras, i.e., the high resolution of the PTZ camera and the wide field-of-view of the omnidirectional camera, but displaying the unified image on a 2D display would reduce the benefit of "omni-" directionality. In this paper, we introduce a foveated imaging pipeline designed to support virtual reality head-mounted displays (HMDs). The pipeline consists of two parallel processes: one for estimating parameters for the integration of the two images and another for rendering images in real time. A control mechanism for placing the foveal region (i.e., high-resolution area) in the scene and zooming is also proposed. Our evaluations showed that the proposed pipeline achieved, on average, 17 frames per second when rendering the foveated view on an HMD, and showed angular resolution improvement on the foveal region compared with the omnidirectional camera view. However, the improvement was less significant when the zoom level was 8× and more. We discuss possible improvement points and future research directions.


Asunto(s)
Telemedicina/instrumentación , Telemedicina/métodos , Grabación en Video/instrumentación , Computadores , Diseño de Equipo , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Grabación en Video/métodos , Realidad Virtual
9.
Behav Res Methods ; 51(5): 2074-2084, 2019 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30875024

RESUMEN

Infantile nystagmus (IN) describes a regular, repetitive movement of the eyes. A characteristic feature of each cycle of the IN eye movement waveform is a period in which the eyes are moving at minimal velocity. This so-called "foveation" period has long been considered the basis for the best vision in individuals with IN. In recent years, the technology for measuring eye movements has improved considerably, but there remains the challenge of calibrating the direction of gaze in tracking systems when the eyes are continuously moving. Identifying portions of the nystagmus waveform suitable for calibration typically involves time-consuming manual selection of the foveation periods from the eye trace. Without an accurate calibration, the exact parameters of the waveform cannot be determined. In this study, we present an automated method for segmenting IN waveforms with the purpose of determining the foveation positions to be used for calibration of an eye tracker. On average, the "point of regard" was found to be within 0.21° of that determined by hand-marking by an expert observer. This method enables rapid clinical quantification of waveforms and the possibility of gaze-contingent research paradigms being performed with this patient group.


Asunto(s)
Calibración , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular , Automatización , Movimientos Oculares , Humanos , Nistagmo Patológico , Agudeza Visual
10.
J Binocul Vis Ocul Motil ; 68(4): 122-133, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30332339

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION AND PURPOSE: To demonstrate the utility of using eye-movement data to reveal the diagnostic characteristics of infantile nystagmus syndrome (INS), determine treatment, and both estimate and document therapeutic improvements in three patients with well-developed foveation periods, fairly broad, lateral gaze "nulls," head turns, strabismus, and complex, multiplanar nystagmus. PATIENTS AND METHODS: Infrared reflection, magnetic search coil, and high-speed digital video systems were used to record the eye movements of INS patients, pre- and post-Kestenbaum null-point correction surgery (horizontal or vertical). Data were analyzed and estimations made, using the eXpanded Nystagmus Acuity Function (NAFX) that is part of the OMtools toolbox for MATLAB. RESULTS: In all three subjects (S1-S3), both peak NAFX and longest foveation domain (LFD) improved from their pre-Kestenbaum values. S1: 0.700-0.745 (6.4%) and 25-34° (36%), respectively. S2: 0.445-0.633 (42.4%) and >40° to >50° (10%), respectively. S3: 0.250-0.300 (20%) and 13° to ≫18° (see text), respectively. CONCLUSIONS: S1: Even at the high ends of the pre-therapy NAFX and LFD spectra, INS foveation (and therefore, visual-function) improvements may be adequate to justify nystagmus surgery and provide clinical improvements beneficial to the patient. S2: INS foveation improvements in the vertical plane are equal to those originally estimated using the horizontal data in prior patients. S3: Two apparent NAFX peaks can be converted into a very broad peak by surgery based on the preferred lower peak.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Nistagmo Congénito/fisiopatología , Nistagmo Congénito/cirugía , Músculos Oculomotores/cirugía , Procedimientos Quirúrgicos Oftalmológicos , Adolescente , Adulto , Electronistagmografía , Femenino , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Cabeza/fisiopatología , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Músculos Oculomotores/fisiopatología , Postura , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven
11.
J Neurophysiol ; 120(2): 421-438, 2018 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29641309

RESUMEN

The caudal fastigial nuclei (cFN) are the output nuclei by which the medio-posterior cerebellum influences the production of visual saccades. We investigated in two head-restrained monkeys their contribution to the generation of interceptive saccades toward a target moving centrifugally by analyzing the consequences of a unilateral inactivation (10 injection sessions). We describe here the effects on saccades made toward a centrifugal target that moved along the horizontal meridian with a constant (10, 20, or 40°/s), increasing (from 0 to 40°/s over 600 ms), or decreasing (from 40 to 0°/s over 600 ms) speed. After muscimol injection, the monkeys were unable to foveate the current location of the moving target. The horizontal amplitude of interceptive saccades was reduced during contralesional target motions and hypermetric during ipsilesional ones. For both contralesional and ipsilesional saccades, the magnitude of dysmetria increased with target speed. However, the use of accelerating and decelerating targets revealed that the dependence of dysmetria upon target velocity was not due to the current velocity but to the required amplitude of saccade. We discuss these results in the framework of two hypotheses, the so-called "dual drive" and "bilateral" hypotheses. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Unilateral inactivation of the caudal fastigial nucleus impairs the accuracy of saccades toward a moving target. Like saccades toward a static target, interceptive saccades are hypometric when directed toward the contralesional side and hypermetric when they are ipsilesional. The dysmetria depends on target velocity, but the use of accelerating or decelerating targets reveals that velocity is not the crucial parameter. We extend the bilateral fastigial control of saccades and fixation to the production of interceptive saccades.


Asunto(s)
Núcleos Cerebelosos/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Animales , Núcleos Cerebelosos/efectos de los fármacos , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular , Agonistas de Receptores de GABA-A/administración & dosificación , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Muscimol/administración & dosificación , Desempeño Psicomotor/efectos de los fármacos , Movimientos Sacádicos/efectos de los fármacos
12.
Prog Brain Res ; 236: 243-268, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29157414

RESUMEN

In primates, the appearance of an object moving in the peripheral visual field elicits an interceptive saccade that brings the target image onto the foveae. This foveation is then maintained more or less efficiently by slow pursuit eye movements and subsequent catch-up saccades. Sometimes, the tracking is such that the gaze direction looks spatiotemporally locked onto the moving object. Such a spatial synchronism is quite spectacular when one considers that the target-related signals are transmitted to the motor neurons through multiple parallel channels connecting separate neural populations with different conduction speeds and delays. Because of the delays between the changes of retinal activity and the changes of extraocular muscle tension, the maintenance of the target image onto the fovea cannot be driven by the current retinal signals as they correspond to past positions of the target. Yet, the spatiotemporal coincidence observed during pursuit suggests that the oculomotor system is driven by a command estimating continuously the current location of the target, i.e., where it is here and now. This inference is also supported by experimental perturbation studies: when the trajectory of an interceptive saccade is experimentally perturbed, a correction saccade is produced in flight or after a short delay, and brings the gaze next to the location where unperturbed saccades would have landed at about the same time, in the absence of visual feedback. In this chapter, we explain how such correction can be supported by previous visual signals without assuming "predictive" signals encoding future target locations. We also describe the basic neural processes which gradually yield the synchronization of eye movements with the target motion. When the process fails, the gaze is driven by signals related to past locations of the target, not by estimates to its upcoming locations, and a catch-up is made to reinitiate the synchronization.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Animales , Humanos
13.
J Neurosci ; 37(47): 11469-11484, 2017 11 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29054879

RESUMEN

Humans visually process the world with varying spatial resolution and can program their eye movements optimally to maximize information acquisition for a variety of everyday tasks. Diseases such as macular degeneration can change visual sensory processing, introducing central vision loss (a scotoma). However, humans can learn to direct a new preferred retinal location to regions of interest for simple visual tasks. Whether such learned compensatory saccades are optimal and generalize to more complex tasks, which require integrating information across a large area of the visual field, is not well understood. Here, we explore the possible effects of central vision loss on the optimal saccades during a face identification task, using a gaze-contingent simulated scotoma. We show that a new foveated ideal observer with a central scotoma correctly predicts that the human optimal point of fixation to identify faces shifts from just below the eyes to one that is at the tip of the nose and another at the top of the forehead. However, even after 5000 trials, humans of both sexes surprisingly do not change their initial fixations to adapt to the new optimal fixation points to faces. In contrast, saccades do change for tasks such as object following and to a lesser extent during search. Our findings argue against a central brain motor-compensatory mechanism that generalizes across tasks. They instead suggest task specificity in the learning of oculomotor plans in response to changes in front-end sensory processing and the possibility of separate domain-specific representations of learned oculomotor plans in the brain.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The mechanism by which humans adapt eye movements in response to central vision loss is still not well understood and carries importance for gaining a fundamental understanding of brain plasticity. We show that although humans adapt their eye movements for simpler tasks such as object following and search, these adaptations do not generalize to more complex tasks such as face identification. We provide the first computational model to predict where humans with central vision loss should direct their eye movements in face identification tasks, which could become a critical tool in making patient-specific recommendations. Based on these results, we suggest a novel theory for oculomotor learning: a distributed representation of learned eye-movement plans represented in domain-specific areas of the brain.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Desempeño Psicomotor , Movimientos Sacádicos , Escotoma/fisiopatología , Percepción Visual , Adaptación Fisiológica , Reconocimiento Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Músculos Oculomotores/inervación , Adulto Joven
14.
J Neurophysiol ; 118(5): 2890-2901, 2017 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28904104

RESUMEN

Following the suggestion that a command encoding current target location feeds the oculomotor system during interceptive saccades, we tested the involvement of the deep superior colliculus (dSC). Extracellular activity of 52 saccade-related neurons was recorded in three monkeys while they generated saccades to targets that were static or moving along the preferred axis, away from (outward) or toward (inward) a fixated target with a constant speed (20°/s). Vertical and horizontal motions were tested when possible. Movement field (MF) parameters (boundaries, preferred vector, and firing rate) were estimated after spline fitting of the relation between the average firing rate during the motor burst and saccade amplitude. During radial target motions, the inner MF boundary shifted in the motion direction for some, but not all, neurons. Likewise, for some neurons, the lower boundaries were shifted upward during upward motions and the upper boundaries downward during downward motions. No consistent change was observed during horizontal motions. For some neurons, the preferred vectors were also shifted in the motion direction for outward, upward, and "toward the midline" target motions. The shifts of boundary and preferred vector were not correlated. The burst firing rate was consistently reduced during interceptive saccades. Our study demonstrates an involvement of dSC neurons in steering the interceptive saccade. When observed, the shifts of boundary in the direction of target motion correspond to commands related to past target locations. The absence of shift in the opposite direction implies that dSC activity does not issue predictive commands related to future target location.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The deep superior colliculus is involved in steering the saccade toward the current location of a moving target. During interceptive saccades, the active population consists of a continuum of cells ranging from neurons issuing commands related to past locations of the target to neurons issuing commands related to its current location. The motor burst of collicular neurons does not contain commands related to the future location of a moving target.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Microelectrodos , Estimulación Luminosa , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador
15.
Front Neurosci ; 8: 294, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25278828

RESUMEN

Vision in foveate animals is an active process that requires rapid and constant decision-making. For example, when a new object appears in the visual field, we can quickly decide to inspect it by directing our eyes to the object's location. We studied the contribution of primate area V4 to these types of rapid foveation decisions. Animals performed a reaction time task that required them to report when any shape appeared within a peripherally-located noisy stimulus by making a saccade to the stimulus location. We found that about half of the randomly sampled V4 neurons not only rapidly and precisely represented the appearance of this shape, but they were also predictive of the animal's saccades. A neuron's ability to predict the animal's saccades was not related to the specificity with which the cell represented a single type of shape but rather to its ability to signal whether any shape was present. This relationship between sensory sensitivity and behavioral predictiveness was not due to global effects such as alertness, as it was equally likely to be observed for cells with increases and decreases in firing rate. Careful analysis of the timescales of reliability in these neurons implies that they reflect both feedforward and feedback shape detecting processes. In approximately 7% of our recorded sample, individual neurons were able to predict both the delay and precision of the animal's shape detection performance. This suggests that a subset of V4 neurons may have been directly and causally contributing to task performance and that area V4 likely plays a critical role in guiding rapid, form-based foveation decisions.

16.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 54(13): 7943-51, 2013 Dec 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24222308

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Infantile nystagmus syndrome (INS) is an involuntary oscillation of the eyes that has been reported to impair vision and worsen under stress. This investigation aimed to measure visual function in terms of visual acuity (VA) and response time (RT), when INS subjects are placed under stress. METHODS: A total of 23 subjects with INS and 20 control subjects performed a 2-alternative forced choice (2AFC) staircase procedure identifying the gap in a Landolt C, under 4 experimental conditions: initial acclimatization (A); task demand (TD), during which subjects received a small electrical shock for every incorrect answer; anticipatory anxiety (AA), during which subjects received a small shock at random intervals; and relaxed (R). Arousal was monitored with galvanic skin conductance (SkC). In addition to VA and eye movements, RTs were recorded. RESULTS: The SkC was higher in the TD and AA periods and lower during A and R. Shock significantly increased nystagmus amplitude (P < 0.01) and intensity (P < 0.007), and reduced foveation periods (FPs, P < 0.022). In both groups, VA was not reduced, but showed a slight improvement. However, shock increased RT (P < 0.009), and INS subjects were slower than controls (P < 0.0005). CONCLUSIONS: Increased arousal ("stress") provoked more intense nystagmus eye movements. As seen in other studies, stress did not reduce VA despite the shorter FPs. Although VA and FP can correlate across subjects, there would appear to be little correlation, if any, within a subject. However, RTs did increase with stress and shorter FPs, which may have an adverse impact on the visual performance of those with INS.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Nistagmo Congénito/fisiopatología , Músculos Oculomotores/fisiopatología , Estrés Fisiológico/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Electronistagmografía/métodos , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Agudeza Visual , Adulto Joven
17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22461850

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Current methods of determining licence retainment or cancellation is through on-road driving tests. Previous research has shown that occupational therapists frequently assess drivers' visual attention while sitting in the back seat on the opposite side of the driver. Since the eyes of the driver are not always visible, assessment by eye contact becomes problematic. Such procedural drawbacks may challenge validity and reliability of the visual attention assessments. In terms of correctly classified attention, the aim of the study was to establish the accuracy and the inter-rater reliability of driving assessments of visual attention from the back seat. Furthermore, by establishing eye contact between the assessor and the driver through an additional mirror on the wind screen, the present study aimed to establish how much such an intervention would enhance the accuracy of the visual attention assessment. METHODS: Two drivers with Parkinson's disease (PD) and six control drivers drove a fixed route in a driving simulator while wearing a head mounted eye tracker. The eye tracker data showed where the foveal visual attention actually was directed. These data were time stamped and compared with the simultaneous manual scoring of the visual attention of the drivers. In four of the drivers, one with Parkinson's disease, a mirror on the windscreen was set up to arrange for eye contact between the driver and the assessor. Inter-rater reliability was performed with one of the Parkinson drivers driving, but without the mirror. RESULTS: Without mirror, the overall accuracy was 56% when assessing the three control drivers and with mirror 83%. However, for the PD driver without mirror the accuracy was 94%, whereas for the PD driver with a mirror the accuracy was 90%. With respect to the inter-rater reliability, a 73% agreement was found. CONCLUSION: If the final outcome of a driving assessment is dependent on the subcategory of a protocol assessing visual attention, we suggest the use of an additional mirror to establish eye contact between the assessor and the driver. The clinicians' observations on-road should not be a standalone assessment in driving assessments. Instead, eye trackers should be employed for further analyses and correlation in cases where there is doubt about a driver's attention.

18.
Open Ophthalmol J ; 4: 42-51, 2010 Jul 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21270949

RESUMEN

AIM: To determine if acetazolamide, an effective treatment for certain inherited channelopathies, has therapeutic effects on infantile nystagmus syndrome (INS) in a well-studied subject, compare them to other therapies in the same subject and to tenotomy and reattachment (T&R) in other subjects. METHODS: Eye-movement data were taken using a high-speed digital video recording system. Nystagmus waveforms were analyzed by applying an eXpanded Nystagmus Acuity Function (NAFX) at different gaze angles and determining the Longest Foveation Domain (LFD). RESULTS: Acetazolamide improved foveation by both a 59.7% increase in the peak value of the NAFX function (from 0.395 to 0.580) and a 70% broadening of the NAFX vs Gaze Angle curve (the LFD increased from 20° to 34°). The resulting U-shaped improvement in the percent NAFX vs Gaze Angle curve, varied from ~60% near the NAFX peak to over 1000% laterally. The therapeutic improvements in NAFX from acetazolamide (similar to T&R) were intermediate between those of soft contact lenses and convergence, the latter was best; for LFD improvements, acetazolamide and contact lenses were equivalent and less effective than convergence. Computer simulations suggested that damping the central oscillation driving INS was insufficient to produce the foveation improvements and increased NAFX values. CONCLUSION: Acetazolamide resulted in improved-foveation INS waveforms over a broadened range of gaze angles, probably acting at more than one site. This raises the question of whether hereditary INS involves an inherited channelopathy, and whether other agents with known effects on ion channels should be investigated as therapy for this condition.

19.
Artículo en Coreano | WPRIM (Pacífico Occidental) | ID: wpr-216722

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To report herein on Nystagmus Acuity Estimator Function (NAEF) based on the foveation time, obtained by analyzing waveforms of infantile nystagmus patients and comparing the results with the patients' actual visual acuity. METHODS: Electro-oculographic data of 27 patients with infantile nystagmus were reviewed. Data of patients only with jerk type nystagmus and reliable visual acuity were analyzed. The foveation time was measured, and NAEF was calculated and compared with the patients' actual best corrected visual acuity. RESULTS: A correlation analysis of the patients' best corrected visual acuity with NAEF was performed, and the retrieved coefficient was 0.4266. The p-value calculated using the Pearson correlation coefficient was 0.0282, implying that high NAEF correlates positively with visual acuity. CONCLUSIONS: Estimated visual acuity, calculated based on the waveforms, positively correlates with the patients' actual visual acuity with statistical significance. However, since the foveation time can be measured only in the patients with jerk-type nystagmus waveforms, further study should be performed on the measurement of the foveation time with other waveforms. Furthermore, the present study shows that such analysis is possible with electrooculogram settings in most general hospitals.


Asunto(s)
Humanos , Electrooculografía , Hospitales Generales , Agudeza Visual
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA