Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 92
Filtrar
1.
Dev Psychobiol ; 66(6): e22539, 2024 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39164829

RESUMEN

Infants' nonverbal expressions-a broad smile or a sharp cry-are powerful at eliciting reactions. Although parents' reactions to their own infants' expressions are relatively well understood, here we studied whether adults more generally exhibit behavioral and physiological reactions to unfamiliar infants producing various expressions. We recruited U.S. emerging adults (N = 84) prior to parenthood, 18-25 years old, 68% women, ethnically (20% Hispanic/Latino) and racially (7% Asian, 13% Black, 1% Middle Eastern, 70% White, 8% multiracial) diverse. They observed four 80-s audio-video clips of unfamiliar 2- to 6-month-olds crying, smiling, yawning, and sitting calmly (emotionally neutral control). Each compilation video depicted 9 different infants (36 clips total). We found adults mirrored behaviorally and physiologically: more positive facial expressions to infants smiling, and more negative facial expressions and pupil dilation-indicating increases in arousal-to infants crying. Adults also yawned more and had more pupil dilation when observing infants yawning. Together, these findings suggest that even nonparent emerging adults are highly sensitive to unfamiliar infants' expressions, which they naturally "catch" (i.e., behaviorally and physiologically mirror), even without instructions. Such sensitivity may have-over the course of humans' evolutionary history-been selected for, to facilitate adults' processing of preverbal infants' expressions to meet their needs.


Asunto(s)
Emociones , Expresión Facial , Bostezo , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Bostezo/fisiología , Adulto , Lactante , Adulto Joven , Adolescente , Emociones/fisiología , Llanto/fisiología , Conducta del Lactante/fisiología , Percepción Social , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología
2.
J Psychiatr Res ; 176: 422-429, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38959825

RESUMEN

Facial mimicry serves as an evolutionarily rooted important interpersonal communication process that touches on the concepts of socialization and empathy. Facial electromyography (EMG) of the corrugator muscle and the zygomaticus muscle was recorded while male forensic psychopathic patients and controls watched morphed angry or happy facial expressions. We tested the hypothesis that psychopathic patients would show weaker short latency facial mimicry (that is, within 600 ms after stimulus onset) than controls. Exclusively in the group of 20 psychopathic patients, we tested in a placebo-controlled crossover within-subject design the hypothesis that oxytocin would enhance short-latency facial mimicry. Compared with placebo, we found no oxytocin-related significant short-latency responses of the corrugator and the zygomaticus. However, compared with 19 normal controls, psychopathic patients in the placebo condition showed significantly weaker short-latency zygomaticus responses to happy faces, while there was a trend toward significantly weaker short-latency corrugator responses to angry faces. These results are consistent with a recent study of facial EMG responses in adolescents with psychopathic traits. We therefore posit a lifetime developmental deficit in psychopathy pertaining short-latency mimicry of emotional facial expressions. Ultimately, this deficit in mimicking angry and happy expressions may hinder the elicitation of empathy, which is known to be impaired in psychopathy.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno de Personalidad Antisocial , Electromiografía , Expresión Facial , Músculos Faciales , Oxitocina , Humanos , Masculino , Oxitocina/administración & dosificación , Oxitocina/farmacología , Adulto , Trastorno de Personalidad Antisocial/fisiopatología , Músculos Faciales/efectos de los fármacos , Músculos Faciales/fisiología , Músculos Faciales/fisiopatología , Adulto Joven , Emociones/fisiología , Emociones/efectos de los fármacos , Estudios Cruzados , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Reconocimiento Facial/efectos de los fármacos , Tiempo de Reacción/efectos de los fármacos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
3.
Infancy ; 29(5): 729-749, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39024123

RESUMEN

Memory develops across the course of the first years of life and is influenced by daily experiences, such as exposure to media like books and television. Memory as tapped by Deferred imitation (DI) requires that toddlers form a representation of the target actions that they can later use to reproduce the actions and in addition to measuring memory for real live events, it can also be used to measure memory for events viewed through media. Toddlers are frequently exposed to multiple forms of digital media in addition to more traditional forms of picture book reading. In a within-subjects design, memory was assessed with a DI task in 2-year-olds (n = 89) using the Frankfurt Imitation Test. Deferred imitation was assessed after live and video demonstrations. Parents completed a survey about children's media use. Picture book reading for less than 30 min a day predicted lower memory scores for actions demonstrated live. Watching video content for more than 1 h a day predicted lower memory scores for actions demonstrated on video. Results are interpreted in terms of individual differences in experiences of traditional and digital media and the development of symbolic understanding.


Asunto(s)
Libros , Memoria , Lectura , Humanos , Preescolar , Femenino , Masculino , Desarrollo Infantil , Televisión , Conducta Imitativa
4.
Soc Neurosci ; 19(2): 124-136, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39023438

RESUMEN

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is defined by distinctive socio-cognitive behaviors that deviate from typical patterns. Notably, social imitation skills appear to be particularly impacted, manifesting early on in development. This paper compared the behavior and inter-brain dynamics of dyads made up of two typically developing (TD) participants with mixed dyads made up of ASD and TD participants during social imitation tasks. By combining kinematics and EEG-hyperscanning, we show that individuals with ASD exhibited a preference for the follower rather than the lead role in imitating scenarios. Moreover, the study revealed inter-brain synchrony differences, with low-alpha inter-brain synchrony differentiating control and mixed dyads. The study's findings suggest the importance of studying interpersonal phenomena in dynamic and ecological settings and using hyperscanning methods to capture inter-brain dynamics during actual social interactions.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Encéfalo , Electroencefalografía , Conducta Social , Humanos , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Masculino , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/fisiopatología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Adulto , Femenino , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Interacción Social
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(27): e2404925121, 2024 Jul 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38917006

RESUMEN

Humans tend to spontaneously imitate others' behavior, even when detrimental to the task at hand. The action observation network (AON) is consistently recruited during imitative tasks. However, whether automatic imitation is mediated by cortico-cortical projections from AON regions to the primary motor cortex (M1) remains speculative. Similarly, the potentially dissociable role of AON-to-M1 pathways involving the ventral premotor cortex (PMv) or supplementary motor area (SMA) in automatic imitation is unclear. Here, we used cortico-cortical paired associative stimulation (ccPAS) to enhance or hinder effective connectivity in PMv-to-M1 and SMA-to-M1 pathways via Hebbian spike-timing-dependent plasticity (STDP) to test their functional relevance to automatic and voluntary motor imitation. ccPAS affected behavior under competition between task rules and prepotent visuomotor associations underpinning automatic imitation. Critically, we found dissociable effects of manipulating the strength of the two pathways. While strengthening PMv-to-M1 projections enhanced automatic imitation, weakening them hindered it. On the other hand, strengthening SMA-to-M1 projections reduced automatic imitation but also reduced interference from task-irrelevant cues during voluntary imitation. Our study demonstrates that driving Hebbian STDP in AON-to-M1 projections induces opposite effects on automatic imitation that depend on the targeted pathway. Our results provide direct causal evidence of the functional role of PMv-to-M1 projections for automatic imitation, seemingly involved in spontaneously mirroring observed actions and facilitating the tendency to imitate them. Moreover, our findings support the notion that SMA exerts an opposite gating function, controlling M1 to prevent overt motor behavior when inadequate to the context.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Imitativa , Corteza Motora , Plasticidad Neuronal , Humanos , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
6.
Ann Acad Med Singap ; 53(3): 152-169, 2024 Mar 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38920243

RESUMEN

Introduction: This systematic review is aimed at (1) evaluating the association between media portrayals of suicides and subsequent copycat suicides or attempts among the general public in Asia, (2) understanding the factors associated with copycat suicides and (3) determining the positive impacts of the media reporting of suicides (e.g. increased help-seeking, coping). Method: A systematic review and narrative synthesis of English and Chinese articles from 8 electronic databases (i.e. PsycINFO, MEDLINE, Embase, CINAHL, Web of Science, Ariti, China National Knowledge Infrastructure and OpenGrey) from January 2000 to May 2023 was conducted. Observational studies were included, and the data were analysed through narrative synthesis. The protocol was registered with PROSPERO (CRD42021281535). Results: Among the 32 studies included (n=29 for evidence synthesis) in the review, there is good-quality evidence to show that copycat suicides and suicide attempts increase after media reports of a suicide, regardless of country, celebrity status, study design, type of media, mode of suicide or follow-up period. Females, younger age groups and those sharing similar characteristics as the deceased in publicised suicides (age, gender) were more susceptible to negative impact. Reporting of the mode of death of the deceased increased suicides by the same method among the public. Conclusion: Media portrayals of suicide appear to have a negative impact on copycat suicides at the population level in Asia. Thus, in addition to tighter media control, healthcare systems, professional medical bodies and community outreach services should work collaboratively to promote early help-seeking in those with psychological distress.


Asunto(s)
Medios de Comunicación de Masas , Suicidio , Humanos , Suicidio/estadística & datos numéricos , Suicidio/psicología , Asia/epidemiología , Intento de Suicidio/estadística & datos numéricos , Intento de Suicidio/psicología , Conducta de Búsqueda de Ayuda , Conducta Imitativa , Adaptación Psicológica , Factores Sexuales , Factores de Edad , Femenino
7.
Exp Brain Res ; 242(8): 1831-1840, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38842756

RESUMEN

Recent studies on the imitation of intransitive gestures suggest that the body part effect relies mainly upon the direct route of the dual-route model through a visuo-transformation mechanism. Here, we test the visuo-constructive hypothesis which posits that the visual complexity may directly potentiate the body part effect for meaningless gestures. We predicted that the difference between imitation of hand and finger gestures would increase with the visuo-spatial complexity of gestures. Second, we aimed to identify some of the visuo-spatial predictors of meaningless finger imitation skills. Thirty-eight participants underwent an imitation task containing three distinct set of gestures, that is, meaningful gestures, meaningless gestures with low visual complexity, and meaningless gestures with higher visual complexity than the first set of meaningless gestures. Our results were in general agreement with the visuo-constructive hypothesis, showing an increase in the difference between hand and finger gestures, but only for meaningless gestures with higher visuo-spatial complexity. Regression analyses confirm that imitation accuracy decreases with resource-demanding visuo-spatial factors. Taken together, our results suggest that the body part effect is highly dependent on the visuo-spatial characteristics of the gestures.


Asunto(s)
Gestos , Conducta Imitativa , Percepción Espacial , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Mano/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología
8.
Dev Neurorehabil ; 27(3-4): 83-92, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38709153

RESUMEN

Lack of eye contact and imitation deficits are frequently targeted in behavioral interventions for children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). In this study, we examined the effects of prompting and modeling on the imitation skills and eye contact of three Arabic-speaking young children with ASD in Syria. A multiple baseline design with a withdrawal component was used to evaluate the effects of the intervention in a clinical setting, at a center for children with special needs, and in follow-up sessions conducted in the participants' homes. All participants' imitative responses and eye contact increased when prompting and modeling were used. Our findings support the effectiveness of prompting and modeling on imitation skills.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Conducta Imitativa , Humanos , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/rehabilitación , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/fisiopatología , Siria , Masculino , Preescolar , Femenino , Terapia Conductista/métodos , Niño
9.
Cognition ; 249: 105831, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38797053

RESUMEN

It is well-established that people tend to mimic one another's actions, a crucial aspect of social interactions. Anticipating imitation has been shown to boost motor activation and reaction times for congruent actions. However, prior research predominantly focused on dyads, leaving gaps in our knowledge regarding group dynamics. This study addresses this gap, conducting three experiments using extensive online data. Participants engaged in anticipated imitation tasks with one versus three virtual agents. The results across all three experiments (n = 77; n = 239; n = 457) consistently support the existence of an anticipated imitation effect, with faster reaction times for congruent actions. Furthermore, the research unveils a social facilitation effect, with participants reacting more swiftly when anticipating three agents compared to one. However, we did not find the expected increase of the congruency effect with multiple agents; rather, the data indicates that anticipating multiple agents instead decreases this effect. These findings are discussed within the framework of ideomotor theory, offering insights into how they relate to recent research on the automatic imitation of multiple agents.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Imitativa , Humanos , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Interacción Social , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
10.
Infancy ; 29(5): 713-728, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38767109

RESUMEN

This study aimed to assess which action component (movement or goal) infants prioritize in their imitation behavior when they get information about its relevance from two important sources: perceptual goal saliency and experimenter's verbal information. 16- to 18-month-olds (N = 72) observed how the experimenter moved a toy mouse with a hopping or sliding movement onto one of two empty spaces (low goal saliency) or 2D circles (medium saliency), or inside one of two 3D houses (high saliency). Before the demonstration, the experimenter verbally announced the movement style or the goal. Results showed that verbal action descriptions did not influence infants' imitation. However, matching previous findings, infants imitated the goal more often than the movement in the high-saliency condition, and the movement more often than the goal in the low-saliency condition. Moreover, in the novel medium-saliency condition, infants imitated both components equally often. Thus, selective imitation varied as a function of perceptual goal saliency, but not of verbal cues. This suggests that perceptual features can enhance infants' bottom-up processing and imitation of action components, while the impact of top-down processes based on verbal cues may vary depending on task characteristics and infants' verbal abilities, inducing a need for further research.


Asunto(s)
Objetivos , Conducta Imitativa , Conducta del Lactante , Humanos , Lactante , Femenino , Masculino , Conducta del Lactante/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología
11.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(4)2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38629797

RESUMEN

Apraxia localization has relied on voxel-based, lesion-symptom mapping studies in left hemisphere stroke patients. Studies on the neural substrates of different manifestations of apraxia in neurodegenerative disorders are scarce. The primary aim of this study was to look into the neural substrates of different manifestations of apraxia in a cohort of corticobasal syndrome patients (CBS) by use of cortical thickness. Twenty-six CBS patients were included in this cross-sectional study. The Goldenberg apraxia test (GAT) was applied. 3D-T1-weighted images were analyzed via the automated recon-all Freesurfer version 6.0 pipeline. Vertex-based multivariate General Linear Model analysis was applied to correlate GAT scores with cortical thickness. Deficits in imitation of meaningless gestures correlated with bilateral superior parietal atrophy, extending to the angular and supramarginal gyri, particularly on the left. Finger imitation relied predominantly on superior parietal lobes, whereas the left angular and supramarginal gyri, in addition to superior parietal lobes, were critical for hand imitation. The widespread bilateral clusters of atrophy in CBS related to apraxia indicate different pathophysiological mechanisms mediating praxis in neurodegenerative disorders compared to vascular lesions, with implications both for our understanding of praxis and for the rehabilitation approaches of patients with apraxia.


Asunto(s)
Apraxias , Degeneración Corticobasal , Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas , Humanos , Estudios Transversales , Apraxias/diagnóstico por imagen , Apraxias/etiología , Apraxias/patología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas/complicaciones , Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas/diagnóstico por imagen , Atrofia , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología
12.
Food Res Int ; 184: 114210, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38609211

RESUMEN

When casein is replaced with starch in imitation cheese, the functionality changes. Three different microscopy methods were applied to understand the microstructural differences in the product depending on which component dominates the microstructure. Confocal Laser Scanning Microscopy (CLSM) for component identification. Scanning Electron Microscopy (SEM) and Cryogenic Scanning Electron Microscopy (Cryo-SEM) for studying surface structures. Differences in the surface structures were detected between SEM and Cryo-SEM. In SEM, starch appeared rough and protein smooth, while in Cryo-SEM no starch roughness of the surface was found. A change in starch modification and effects of protein prehydration was also analysed. Adding octenyl succinic anhydride (OSA) modified starch for emulsifying properties resulted in a microstructure with fragmented protein at a protein level of 7 %, but not at 9 or 12 %. Protein prehydration had limited effect on microstructure. On a macrostructural level, the change to an emulsifying starch increased hardness in imitation cheese with 7 and 9 % protein. Protein prehydration slightly decreased the hardness, but the difference was not significant at all concentrations. This research provides valuable information about the microstructure of imitation cheese at a 50/50 composition, how the microstructure changes with an emulsifying starch and what occurs after a protein prehydration was included in the production.


Asunto(s)
Queso , Conducta Imitativa , Microscopía Electrónica de Rastreo , Caseínas , Almidón
13.
Neural Netw ; 174: 106251, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38552352

RESUMEN

Expert demonstrations in imitation learning often contain different behavioral modes, e.g., driving modes such as driving on the left, keeping the lane, and driving on the right in the driving tasks. Although most existing multi-modal imitation learning methods allow learning from demonstrations of multiple modes, they have strict constraints on the data of each mode, generally requiring a near data ratio of all modes. Otherwise, it tends to fall into a mode collapse or only learn the data distribution of the mode that has the largest data volume. To address the problem, an algorithm that balances real-fake loss and classification loss by modifying the output of the discriminator, referred to as BAlanced Generative Adversarial Imitation Learning (BAGAIL), is proposed. With this modification, the generator is only rewarded for generating real trajectories with correct modes. BAGAIL is therefore able to deal with imbalanced expert demonstrations and carry out efficient learning for each mode. The learning process of BAGAIL is divided into a pre-training stage and an imitation learning stage. During the pre-training stage, BAGAIL initializes the generator parameters by means of conditional Behavioral Cloning, laying the foundation for the direction of parameter optimization. During the imitation learning stage, BAGAIL optimizes the parameters by using the adversary between the generator and the modified discriminator so that the finally obtained policy can successfully learn the distribution of imbalanced expert data. The experiments showed that BAGAIL accurately distinguished different behavioral modes with imbalanced demonstrations. What is more, the learning result of each mode is close to the expert standard and more stable than other multi-modal imitation learning methods.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Imitativa , Aprendizaje , Algoritmos , Políticas , Recompensa
14.
PLoS One ; 19(3): e0300874, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38517933

RESUMEN

Adults infer others' communicative intentions, or lack thereof, from various types of information. Young children may be initially limited to attributions based on a small set of ostensive signals. It is unknown when richer pragmatic inferences about communicative intentions emerge in development. We sought novel type of evidence for such inferences in 17-to-19-month-olds. We hypothesized that toddlers recognize adults' smartphone use in face-to-face interactions as incongruous with ostension and would rely on this interpretation when inferring the communicative intention of a model in a new imitation task conducted entirely online, dubbed the Sock Ball Task. In Experiment 1 with a between-subject design, we tested the hypothesis by assessing toddlers' (N = 48) imitation of sub-efficient means and the goal-outcome presented by a model, who interrupted her ostensive demonstration either by using a smartphone or by fiddling with her wristwatch, depending on the condition. We expected toddlers to imitate the sub-efficient means more faithfully in the wristwatch condition than in the smartphone condition. But there was no significant effect of condition on imitation of neither means nor goal. Thus, our hypothesis was not borne out by the results. In Experiment 2, using a within-subject design, we first assessed toddlers' (N = 24) performance in a no-demonstration baseline and then again after a no-disruption ostensive demonstration. In all three conditions with ostensive demonstration (Experiment 1: smartphone, wristwatch; Experiment 2: no-disruption), toddlers produced the demonstrated sub-efficient means significantly above the baseline level. In the no-disruption condition, goals were also imitated significantly above the baseline level. We conclude that the Sock Ball Task is a valid research tool for studying toddler imitation of novel means actions with objects. We end by discussing suggestions for improving the task in future studies.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Imitativa , Intención , Humanos , Femenino , Adulto , Preescolar , Teléfono Inteligente , Comunicación , Motivación
15.
Emotion ; 24(6): 1376-1385, 2024 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38512197

RESUMEN

Although emotional mimicry is ubiquitous in social interactions, its mechanisms and roles remain disputed. A prevalent view is that imitating others' expressions facilitates emotional understanding, but the evidence is mixed and almost entirely based on facial emotions. In a preregistered study, we asked whether inhibiting orofacial mimicry affects authenticity perception in vocal emotions. Participants listened to authentic and posed laughs and cries, while holding a pen between the teeth and lips to inhibit orofacial responses (n = 75), or while responding freely without a pen (n = 75). They made authenticity judgments and rated how much they felt the conveyed emotions (emotional contagion). Mimicry inhibition decreased the accuracy of authenticity perception in laughter and crying, and in posed and authentic vocalizations. It did not affect contagion ratings, however, nor performance in a cognitive control task, ruling out the effort of holding the pen as an explanation for the decrements in authenticity perception. Laughter was more contagious than crying, and authentic vocalizations were more contagious than posed ones, regardless of whether mimicry was inhibited or not. These findings confirm the role of mimicry in emotional understanding and extend it to auditory emotions. They also imply that perceived emotional contagion can be unrelated to mimicry. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Llanto , Emociones , Expresión Facial , Risa , Percepción Social , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Risa/fisiología , Llanto/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología
16.
Motor Control ; 28(2): 210-224, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38364817

RESUMEN

We review a body of literature related to the drawing and recognition of geometrical two-dimensional linear drawings including letters. Handwritten letters are viewed not as two-dimensional geometrical objects but as one-dimensional trajectories of the tip of the implement. Handwritten letters are viewed as composed of a small set of kinematic primitives. Recognition of objects is mediated by processes of their creation (actual or imagined)-the imitation principle, a particular example of action-perception coupling. The concept of spatial directional field guiding the trajectories is introduced and linked to neuronal population vectors. Further, we link the kinematic description to the theory of control with spatial referent coordinates. This framework allows interpreting a number of experimental observations and clinical cases of agnosia. It also allows formulating predictions for new experimental studies of writing.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Imitativa , Lectura , Humanos , Escritura
17.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 241: 105862, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38320357

RESUMEN

Children are strong imitators, which sometimes leads to overimitation of causally unnecessary actions. Here, we tested whether learning from a peer decreases this tendency. First, 65 7- to 10-year-old children performed the Hook task (i.e., retrieve a reward from a jar with tools) with child or adult demonstrators. The overimitation rate was lower after watching a peer versus an adult. Second, we tested whether experiencing peer-to-peer learning versus adult-driven learning (i.e., Montessori or traditional pedagogy) affected overimitation. Here, 66 4- to 18-year-old children and adolescents performed the Hook task with adult demonstrators only. Montessori-schooled children had a lower propensity to overimitate. These findings emphasize the importance of the teaching model across the school years. Whereas peer models favor selective imitation, adult models encourage overimitation.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Imitativa , Instituciones Académicas , Niño , Adulto , Humanos , Adolescente , Preescolar , Recompensa
19.
Cogn Emot ; 38(4): 530-548, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38303660

RESUMEN

Research indicates that emotional mimicry predominantly occurs in response to affiliative displays, such as happiness, while the mimicry of antagonistic displays, like anger, is seldom observed in social contexts. However, contextual factors, including the identity of the displayer (e.g. social similarity with the observer) and whose action triggered the emotional reaction (i.e. to whom display is directed), can modulate the meaning of the display. In two experiments, participants observed happiness, sadness, and anger expressed by individuals with similar or different social attitudes in response to actions from either a participant or another person. Results demonstrated that three manipulated factors - displayer social similarity, whose action caused an emotional display, and the type of emotional display - affected participants' perception of the display. In turn, mimicry was predominantly observed in response to happiness (Experiments 1 and 2), to a lesser extent to sadness (Experiment 1), and not to anger. Furthermore, participants mimicked individuals who were more socially similar (Experiment 1), while whose action caused an emotional reaction did not influence mimicry. The findings suggest that when the context mitigates the meaning of negative or antagonistic facial displays, it does not necessarily increase the inclination to mimic them.


Asunto(s)
Ira , Expresión Facial , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto Joven , Conducta Imitativa , Percepción Social , Emociones/fisiología , Adulto , Felicidad
20.
Psychogeriatrics ; 24(2): 404-414, 2024 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38290836

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Gesture imitation, a simple tool for assessing visuospatial/visuoconstructive functions, is reportedly useful for screening and diagnosing dementia. However, gesture imitation performance in healthy older adults is largely unknown, as are the factors associated with lower performance. To address these unknowns, we examined the gesture imitation performance of a large number of community-dwelling older adults aged ≥65 years in Arao City, Kumamoto Prefecture (southern Japan). METHODS: The examiner presented the participants with eight gesture patterns and considered it a success if they could imitate them within 10 s. The success rate of each gesture imitation was calculated for three diagnostic groups: cognitively normal (CN) (n = 1184), mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (n = 237), and dementia (n = 47). Next, we reorganised the original gesture imitation battery by combining six selected gestures with the following scoring method: if the participants successfully imitated the gestures, immediately or within 5 s, two points were assigned. If they succeeded within 5-10 s, one point was assigned. The sensitivity and specificity of the battery were investigated to detect the dementia and MCI groups. Factors associated with gesture imitation battery scores were examined. RESULTS: Except one complex gesture, the success rate of imitation in the CN group was high, approximately 90%. The sensitivity and specificity of the gesture imitation battery for discriminating between the dementia and CN groups and between the MCI and CN groups were 70%/88%, and 45%/75%, respectively. Ageing, male sex, and a diagnosis of dementia or MCI were associated with lower scores on the gesture imitation battery. CONCLUSION: Gesture imitation tasks alone may not be sufficient to detect MCI. However, by combining gestures with set time limits, gesture imitation tasks can be a low-burden and effective method for detecting dementia, even in community medicine, such as during health check-ups.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva , Demencia , Humanos , Masculino , Anciano , Gestos , Conducta Imitativa , Vida Independiente , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Demencia/diagnóstico
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA