Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 83
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Elife ; 102021 02 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33559601

RESUMEN

Neural organization of mushroom bodies is largely consistent across insects, whereas the ancestral ground pattern diverges broadly across crustacean lineages resulting in successive loss of columns and the acquisition of domed centers retaining ancestral Hebbian-like networks and aminergic connections. We demonstrate here a major departure from this evolutionary trend in Brachyura, the most recent malacostracan lineage. In the shore crab Hemigrapsus nudus, instead of occupying the rostral surface of the lateral protocerebrum, mushroom body calyces are buried deep within it with their columns extending outwards to an expansive system of gyri on the brain's surface. The organization amongst mushroom body neurons reaches extreme elaboration throughout its constituent neuropils. The calyces, columns, and especially the gyri show DC0 immunoreactivity, an indicator of extensive circuits involved in learning and memory.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Braquiuros/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Neurópilo/citología , Animales
2.
J Comp Neurol ; 529(2): 259-280, 2021 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32400022

RESUMEN

Spiders possess a wide array of sensory-driven behaviors and therefore provide rich models for studying evolutionary hypotheses about the relationship between brain morphology, sensory systems, and behavior. Despite this, only a handful of studies have examined brain variation across the order of Araneae. In this study, I present descriptions of the gross brain morphology for 19 families of spiders that vary in eye morphology. Spiders showed the most variation in the secondary eye visual pathway. Based on this variation, spiders could be categorized into four groups. Group 1 spiders had small, underdeveloped laminae, no medullae, and no mushroom bodies. Group 2 spiders had large laminae, no medullae and large mushroom bodies. Group 3 spiders had laminae and some evidence of reduced medullae and mushroom bodies. Group 4 spiders had the most complex systems, with large laminae, medullae formed from optical glomeruli, and robust mushroom bodies. Within groups, there was large variation in the shape and size of individual regions, indicating possible variation in neuronal organization. The possible evolutionary implications of the loss of a dedicated olfactory organ in spiders and its effects on the mushroom body are also discussed.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/anomalías , Ojo/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Arañas/anatomía & histología , Vías Visuales/anatomía & histología , Animales , Evolución Biológica , Encéfalo/ultraestructura , Ojo/ultraestructura , Cuerpos Pedunculados/ultraestructura , Arañas/ultraestructura , Vías Visuales/ultraestructura
3.
J Comp Neurol ; 529(3): 501-523, 2021 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32484921

RESUMEN

The hypothesis of a common origin for high-order memory centers in bilateral animals presents the question of how different brain structures, such as the vertebrate hippocampus and the arthropod mushroom bodies, are both structurally and functionally comparable. Obtaining evidence to support the hypothesis that crustaceans possess structures equivalent to the mushroom bodies that play a role in associative memories has proved challenging. Structural evidence supports that the hemiellipsoid bodies of hermit crabs, crayfish and lobsters, spiny lobsters, and shrimps are homologous to insect mushroom bodies. Although a preliminary description and functional evidence supporting such homology in true crabs (Brachyura) has recently been shown, other authors consider the identification of a possible mushroom body homolog in Brachyura as problematic. Here we present morphological and immunohistochemical data in Neohelice granulata supporting that crabs possess well-developed hemiellipsoid bodies that are resolved as mushroom bodies-like structures. Neohelice exhibits a peduncle-like tract, from which processes project into proximal and distal domains with different neuronal specializations. The proximal domains exhibit spines and en passant-like processes and are proposed here as regions mainly receiving inputs. The distal domains exhibit a "trauben"-like compartmentalized structure with bulky terminal specializations and are proposed here as output regions. In addition, we found microglomeruli-like complexes, adult neurogenesis, aminergic innervation, and elevated expression of proteins necessary for memory processes. Finally, in vivo calcium imaging suggests that, as in insect mushroom bodies, the output regions exhibit stimulus-specific activity. Our results support the shared organization of memory centers across crustaceans and insects.


Asunto(s)
Química Encefálica , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/química , Animales , Braquiuros , Encéfalo/fisiología , Química Encefálica/fisiología , Drosophila , Masculino , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología
4.
Elife ; 92020 06 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32589143

RESUMEN

Insect navigation arises from the coordinated action of concurrent guidance systems but the neural mechanisms through which each functions, and are then coordinated, remains unknown. We propose that insects require distinct strategies to retrace familiar routes (route-following) and directly return from novel to familiar terrain (homing) using different aspects of frequency encoded views that are processed in different neural pathways. We also demonstrate how the Central Complex and Mushroom Bodies regions of the insect brain may work in tandem to coordinate the directional output of different guidance cues through a contextually switched ring-attractor inspired by neural recordings. The resultant unified model of insect navigation reproduces behavioural data from a series of cue conflict experiments in realistic animal environments and offers testable hypotheses of where and how insects process visual cues, utilise the different information that they provide and coordinate their outputs to achieve the adaptive behaviours observed in the wild.


Asunto(s)
Insectos/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Fenómenos Fisiológicos del Sistema Nervioso , Navegación Espacial/fisiología , Animales , Hormigas/anatomía & histología , Hormigas/fisiología , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Drosophila/anatomía & histología , Drosophila/fisiología , Insectos/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Sistema Nervioso/anatomía & histología , Vías Nerviosas/anatomía & histología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología
5.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 6147, 2020 04 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32273557

RESUMEN

Value coding of external stimuli in general, and odor valence in particular, is crucial for survival. In flies, odor valence is thought to be coded by two types of neurons: mushroom body output neurons (MBONs) and lateral horn (LH) neurons. MBONs are classified as neurons that promote either attraction or aversion, but not both, and they are dynamically activated by upstream neurons. This dynamic activation updates the valence values. In contrast, LH neurons receive scaled, but non-dynamic, input from their upstream neurons. It remains unclear how such a non-dynamic system generates differential valence values. Recently, PD2a1/b1 LH neurons were demonstrated to promote approach behavior at low odor concentration in starved flies. Here, we demonstrate that at high odor concentrations, these same neurons contribute to avoidance in satiated flies. The contribution of PD2a1/b1 LH neurons to aversion is context dependent. It is diminished in starved flies, although PD2a1/b1 neural activity remains unchanged, and at lower odor concentration. In addition, PD2a1/b1 aversive effect develops over time. Thus, our results indicate that, even though PD2a1/b1 LH neurons transmit hard-wired output, their effect on valence can change. Taken together, we suggest that the valence model described for MBONs does not hold for LH neurons.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Olfato , Animales , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Drosophila melanogaster/anatomía & histología , Femenino , Masculino , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Sistema Nervioso/anatomía & histología , Fenómenos Fisiológicos del Sistema Nervioso , Vías Nerviosas/anatomía & histología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Odorantes , Olfato/fisiología
6.
J Comp Neurol ; 528(15): 2595-2601, 2020 10 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32266711

RESUMEN

In 1882, the Italian embryologist Giuseppe Bellonci introduced a nomenclature for structures in the stomatopod crustacean Squilla mantis that he claimed correspond to insect mushroom bodies, today recognized as cardinal centers that in insects mediate associative memory. The use of Bellonci's terminology has, through a series of misunderstandings and entrenched opinions, led to contesting views regarding whether centers in crustacean and insect brains that occupy corresponding locations and receive comparable multisensory inputs are homologous or homoplasic. The following describes the fate of terms used to denote sensory association neuropils in crustacean species and relates how those terms were deployed in the 1920s and 1930s by the Swedish neuroanatomist Bertil Hanström to claim homology in insects and crustaceans. Yet the same terminology has been repurposed by subsequent researchers to promote the very opposite view: that mushroom bodies are a derived trait of hexapods and that equivalent centers in crustaceans evolved independently.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Disonancia Cognitiva , Memoria/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Terminología como Asunto , Animales , Crustáceos , Insectos , Neurópilo/fisiología
7.
Elife ; 92020 03 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32124731

RESUMEN

Descriptions of crustacean brains have focused mainly on three highly derived lineages of malacostracans: the reptantian infraorders represented by spiny lobsters, lobsters, and crayfish. Those descriptions advocate the view that dome- or cap-like neuropils, referred to as 'hemiellipsoid bodies,' are the ground pattern organization of centers that are comparable to insect mushroom bodies in processing olfactory information. Here we challenge the doctrine that hemiellipsoid bodies are a derived trait of crustaceans, whereas mushroom bodies are a derived trait of hexapods. We demonstrate that mushroom bodies typify lineages that arose before Reptantia and exist in Reptantia thereby indicating that the mushroom body, not the hemiellipsoid body, provides the ground pattern for both crustaceans and hexapods. We show that evolved variations of the mushroom body ground pattern are, in some lineages, defined by extreme diminution or loss and, in others, by the incorporation of mushroom body circuits into lobeless centers. Such transformations are ascribed to modifications of the columnar organization of mushroom body lobes that, as shown in Drosophila and other hexapods, contain networks essential for learning and memory.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Crustáceos/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Animales , Crustáceos/genética , Especificidad de la Especie
8.
J Comp Neurol ; 528(2): 261-282, 2020 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31376285

RESUMEN

Brain centers possessing a suite of neuroanatomical characters that define mushroom bodies of dicondylic insects have been identified in mantis shrimps, which are basal malacostracan crustaceans. Recent studies of the caridean shrimp Lebbeus groenlandicus further demonstrate the existence of mushroom bodies in Malacostraca. Nevertheless, received opinion promulgates the hypothesis that domed centers called hemiellipsoid bodies typifying reptantian crustaceans, such as lobsters and crayfish, represent the malacostracan cerebral ground pattern. Here, we provide evidence from the marine hermit crab Pagurus hirsutiusculus that refutes this view. P. hirsutiusculus, which is a member of the infraorder Anomura, reveals a chimeric morphology that incorporates features of a domed hemiellipsoid body and a columnar mushroom body. These attributes indicate that a mushroom body morphology is the ancestral ground pattern, from which the domed hemiellipsoid body derives and that the "standard" reptantian hemiellipsoid bodies that typify Astacidea and Achelata are extreme examples of divergence from this ground pattern. This interpretation is underpinned by comparing the lateral protocerebrum of Pagurus with that of the crayfish Procambarus clarkii and Orconectes immunis, members of the reptantian infraorder Astacidea.


Asunto(s)
Anomuros/anatomía & histología , Evolución Biológica , Encéfalo , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Animales
9.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31705196

RESUMEN

Evolutionary transitions in social behavior are often associated with changes in species' brain architecture. A recent comparative analysis showed that the structure of brains of wasps in the family Vespidae differed between solitary and social species: the mushroom bodies, a major integrative brain region, were larger relative to brain size in the solitary species. However, the earlier study did not account for body size effects, and species' relative mushroom body size increases with body size in social Vespidae. Here we extend the previous analysis by measuring the effects of body size variation on brain structure differences between social and solitary vespid wasps. We asked whether total brain volume was greater relative to body size in the solitary species, and whether relative mushroom body size was greater in solitary species, after accounting for body size effects. Both total brain volume and relative mushroom body volume were significantly greater in the solitary species after accounting for body size differences. Therefore, body size allometry did not explain the solitary versus social species differences in brain structure. The evolutionary transition from solitary to social behavior in Vespidae was accompanied by decreases in total brain size and in relative mushroom body size.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Conducta Social , Avispas/anatomía & histología , Animales , Tamaño Corporal , Tamaño de los Órganos , Especificidad de la Especie
10.
Naturwissenschaften ; 106(9-10): 49, 2019 Aug 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31456004

RESUMEN

Social interactions may shape brain development. In primitively eusocial insects, the mushroom body (MB), an area of the brain associated with sensory integration and learning, is larger in queens than in workers. This may reflect a strategy of neural investment in queens or it may be a plastic response to social interactions in the nest. Here, we show that nest foundresses-the reproductive females who will become queens but are solitary until their first workers are born-have larger MBs than workers in the primitively eusocial sweat bee Augochlorella aurata. Whole brain size and optic lobe size do not differ between the two groups, but foundresses also have larger antennal lobes than workers. This shows that increased neural investment in MBs precedes social group formation. Larger MBs among foundresses may reflect the increased larval nutrition provisioned to future queens and the lack of social aggression from a dominant queen upon adult emergence.


Asunto(s)
Abejas/anatomía & histología , Abejas/fisiología , Predominio Social , Fenómenos Fisiológicos Nutricionales de los Animales , Animales , Conducta Animal , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología
11.
Elife ; 82019 05 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31112130

RESUMEN

Animals exhibit innate behaviours to a variety of sensory stimuli including olfactory cues. In Drosophila, one higher olfactory centre, the lateral horn (LH), is implicated in innate behaviour. However, our structural and functional understanding of the LH is scant, in large part due to a lack of sparse neurogenetic tools for this region. We generate a collection of split-GAL4 driver lines providing genetic access to 82 LH cell types. We use these to create an anatomical and neurotransmitter map of the LH and link this to EM connectomics data. We find ~30% of LH projections converge with outputs from the mushroom body, site of olfactory learning and memory. Using optogenetic activation, we identify LH cell types that drive changes in valence behavior or specific locomotor programs. In summary, we have generated a resource for manipulating and mapping LH neurons, providing new insights into the circuit basis of innate and learned olfactory behavior.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Drosophila/anatomía & histología , Drosophila/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Corteza Olfatoria/anatomía & histología , Corteza Olfatoria/fisiología , Animales , Conectoma , Vías Nerviosas/anatomía & histología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Optogenética
12.
PLoS One ; 14(3): e0213618, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30917163

RESUMEN

Strongly polyphenic social insects provide excellent models to examine the neurobiological basis of division of labor. Turtle ants, Cephalotes varians, have distinct minor worker, soldier, and reproductive (gyne/queen) morphologies associated with their behavioral profiles: small-bodied task-generalist minors lack the phragmotic shield-shaped heads of soldiers, which are specialized to block and guard the nest entrance. Gynes found new colonies and during early stages of colony growth overlap behaviorally with soldiers. Here we describe patterns of brain structure and synaptic organization associated with division of labor in C. varians minor workers, soldiers, and gynes. We quantified brain volumes, determined scaling relationships among brain regions, and quantified the density and size of microglomeruli, synaptic complexes in the mushroom body calyxes important to higher-order processing abilities that may underpin behavioral performance. We found that brain volume was significantly larger in gynes; minor workers and soldiers had similar brain sizes. Consistent with their larger behavioral repertoire, minors had disproportionately larger mushroom bodies than soldiers and gynes. Soldiers and gynes had larger optic lobes, which may be important for flight and navigation in gynes, but serve different functions in soldiers. Microglomeruli were larger and less dense in minor workers; soldiers and gynes did not differ. Correspondence in brain structure despite differences in soldiers and gyne behavior may reflect developmental integration, suggesting that neurobiological metrics not only advance our understanding of brain evolution in social insects, but may also help resolve questions of the origin of novel castes.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación Animal , Hormigas/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal , Tamaño Corporal , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Jerarquia Social , Masculino , Análisis Multivariante , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Lóbulo Óptico de Animales no Mamíferos/anatomía & histología , Tamaño de los Órganos , Fenotipo , Filogenia , Reproducción , Conducta Social
13.
Cell Tissue Res ; 374(1): 39-62, 2018 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29744590

RESUMEN

The desert locust Schistocerca gregaria is a major agricultural pest in North Africa and the Middle East. As such, it has been intensely studied, in particular with respect to population dynamics, sensory processing, feeding behavior flight and locomotor control, migratory behavior, and its neuroendocrine system. Being a long-range migratory species, neural mechanisms underlying sky compass orientation have been studied in detail. To further understand neuronal interactions in the brain of the locust, a deeper understanding of brain organization in this insect has become essential. As a follow-up of a previous study illustrating the layout of the locust brain (Kurylas et al. in J Comp Neurol 484:206-223, 2008), we analyze the cerebrum, the central brain minus gnathal ganglia, of the desert locust in more detail and provide a digital three-dimensional atlas of 48 distinguishable brain compartments and 7 major fiber tracts and commissures as a basis for future functional studies. Neuropils were three-dimensionally reconstructed from synapsin-immunostained whole mount brains. Neuropil composition and their internal organization were analyzed and compared to the neuropils of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. Most brain areas have counterparts in Drosophila. Some neuropils recognized in the locust, however, have not been identified in the fly while certain areas in the fly could not be distinguished in the locust. This study paves the way for more detailed anatomical descriptions of neuronal connections and neuronal cell types in the locust brain, facilitates interspecies comparisons among insect brains and points out possible evolutionary differences in brain organization between hemi- and holometabolous insects.


Asunto(s)
Cerebro/anatomía & histología , Clima Desértico , Saltamontes/anatomía & histología , Animales , Femenino , Imagenología Tridimensional , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Neurópilo/metabolismo
14.
Brain Behav Evol ; 90(3): 243-254, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29059675

RESUMEN

Haller's rule states that brains scale allometrically with body size in all animals, meaning that relative brain size increases with decreasing body size. This rule applies both on inter- and intraspecific comparisons. Only 1 species, the extremely small parasitic wasp Trichogramma evanescens, is known as an exception and shows an isometric brain-body size relation in an intraspecific comparison between differently sized individuals. Here, we investigated if such an isometric brain-body size relationship also occurs in an intraspecific comparison with a slightly larger parasitic wasp, Nasonia vitripennis, a species that may vary 10-fold in body weight upon differences in levels of scramble competition during larval development. We show that Nasonia exhibits diphasic brain-body size scaling: larger wasps scale allometrically, following Haller's rule, whereas the smallest wasps show isometric scaling. Brains of smaller wasps are, therefore, smaller than expected and we hypothesized that this may lead to adaptations in brain architecture. Volumetric analysis of neuropil composition revealed that wasps of different sizes differed in relative volume of multiple neuropils. The optic lobes and mushroom bodies in particular were smaller in the smallest wasps. Furthermore, smaller brains had a relatively smaller total neuropil volume and larger cellular rind than large brains. These changes in relative brain size and brain architecture suggest that the energetic constraints on brain tissue outweigh specific cognitive requirements in small Nasonia wasps.


Asunto(s)
Tamaño Corporal/fisiología , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Avispas/anatomía & histología , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Animales , Evolución Biológica , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Neurópilo/fisiología , Lóbulo Óptico de Animales no Mamíferos/anatomía & histología
15.
Elife ; 62017 09 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28949916

RESUMEN

Mushroom bodies are the iconic learning and memory centers of insects. No previously described crustacean possesses a mushroom body as defined by strict morphological criteria although crustacean centers called hemiellipsoid bodies, which serve functions in sensory integration, have been viewed as evolutionarily convergent with mushroom bodies. Here, using key identifiers to characterize neural arrangements, we demonstrate insect-like mushroom bodies in stomatopod crustaceans (mantis shrimps). More than any other crustacean taxon, mantis shrimps display sophisticated behaviors relating to predation, spatial memory, and visual recognition comparable to those of insects. However, neuroanatomy-based cladistics suggesting close phylogenetic proximity of insects and stomatopod crustaceans conflicts with genomic evidence showing hexapods closely related to simple crustaceans called remipedes. We discuss whether corresponding anatomical phenotypes described here reflect the cerebral morphology of a common ancestor of Pancrustacea or an extraordinary example of convergent evolution.


Asunto(s)
Crustáceos/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Animales , Evolución Biológica , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología
16.
Curr Opin Insect Sci ; 22: 54-61, 2017 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28805639

RESUMEN

We review research on brain development and brain evolution in the wasp family Vespidae. Basic vespid neuroanatomy and some aspects of functional neural circuitry are well-characterized, and genomic tools for exploring brain plasticity are being developed. Although relatively modest in terms of species richness, the Vespidae include species spanning much of the known range of animal social complexity, from solitary nesters to highly eusocial species with some of the largest known colonies and multiple reproductives. Eusocial species differ in behavior and ecology including variation in queen/worker caste differentiation and in diurnal/nocturnal activity. Species differences in overall brain size are strongly associated with brain allometry; relative sizes of visual processing tissues increase at faster rates than antennal processing tissues. The lower relative size of the central-processing mushroom bodies (MB) in eusocial species compared to solitary relatives suggests sociality may relax demands on individual cognitive abilities. However, queens have greater relative MB volumes than their workers, and MB development is positively associated with social dominance status in some species. Fruitful areas for future investigations of adaptive brain investment in the clade include sampling of key overlooked taxa with diverse social structures, and the analysis of neural correlations with ecological divergence in foraging resources and diel activity patterns.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Avispas/anatomía & histología , Animales , Conducta Animal , Evolución Biológica , Encéfalo/fisiología , Ambiente , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Tamaño de los Órganos , Conducta Social , Avispas/fisiología
17.
Elife ; 62017 07 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28718765

RESUMEN

Understanding memory formation, storage and retrieval requires knowledge of the underlying neuronal circuits. In Drosophila, the mushroom body (MB) is the major site of associative learning. We reconstructed the morphologies and synaptic connections of all 983 neurons within the three functional units, or compartments, that compose the adult MB's α lobe, using a dataset of isotropic 8 nm voxels collected by focused ion-beam milling scanning electron microscopy. We found that Kenyon cells (KCs), whose sparse activity encodes sensory information, each make multiple en passant synapses to MB output neurons (MBONs) in each compartment. Some MBONs have inputs from all KCs, while others differentially sample sensory modalities. Only 6% of KC>MBON synapses receive a direct synapse from a dopaminergic neuron (DAN). We identified two unanticipated classes of synapses, KC>DAN and DAN>MBON. DAN activation produces a slow depolarization of the MBON in these DAN>MBON synapses and can weaken memory recall.


Asunto(s)
Conectoma , Drosophila/anatomía & histología , Drosophila/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Animales , Aprendizaje , Memoria
18.
J Comp Neurol ; 525(12): 2615-2631, 2017 Aug 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28445613

RESUMEN

Tyramine is an important neurotransmitter, neuromodulator, and neurohormone in insects. In honeybees, it is assumed to have functions in modulating sensory responsiveness and controlling motor behavior. Tyramine can bind to two characterized receptors in honeybees, both of which are coupled to intracellular cAMP pathways. How tyramine acts on neuronal, cellular and circuit levels is unclear. We investigated the spatial brain expression of the tyramine receptor AmTAR1 using a specific antibody. This antibody detects a membrane protein of the expected molecular weight in western blot analysis. In honeybee brains, it labels different structures which process sensory information. Labeling along the antennal nerve, in projections of the dorsal lobe and in the gnathal ganglion suggest that tyramine receptors are involved in modulating gustatory and tactile perception. Furthermore, the ellipsoid body of the central complex and giant synapses in the lateral complex show AmTAR1-like immunoreactivity (AmTAR1-IR), suggesting a role of this receptor in modulating sky-compass information and/or higher sensor-motor control. Additionally, intense signals derive from the mushroom bodies, higher-order integration centers for olfactory, visual, gustatory and tactile information. To investigate whether AmTAR1-expressing brain structures are in vicinity to tyramine releasing sites, a specific tyramine antibody was applied. Tyramine-like labeling was observed in AmTAR1-IR positive structures, although it was sometimes weak and we did not always find a direct match of ligand and receptor. Moreover, tyramine-like immunoreactivity was also found in brain regions without AmTAR1-IR (optic lobes, antennal lobes), indicating that other tyramine-specific receptors may be expressed there.


Asunto(s)
Abejas/anatomía & histología , Encéfalo/citología , Neuronas/metabolismo , Receptores de Amina Biogénica/metabolismo , Tiramina/metabolismo , Actinas/metabolismo , Animales , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/metabolismo , Proteína Básica de Mielina/metabolismo , Sinapsinas/metabolismo
19.
J Insect Physiol ; 98: 214-222, 2017 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28118991

RESUMEN

The neuronal pathways involved in the processing of sex pheromone information were investigated in the hawkmoth Agrius convolvuli (Lepidoptera: Sphingidae), which uses (E,E)-11,13-hexadecadienal (E11,E13-16:Ald) as the single sex pheromone component. We first clarified the anatomical organization of the antennal lobe of A. convolvuli. Subsequently, central neurons in the antennal lobe that responded to E11,E13-16:Ald were identified. The dendritic processes of these neurons were confined within a specific glomerulus (cumulus) in the antennal lobe. The axons of these neurons projected to the inferior lateral protocerebrum and mushroom body calyx. Although the anatomical organization and morphology of individual neurons in A. convolvuli were similar to other species in the superfamily Bombycoidea, the use of cumulus as the single pathway for sex pheromone information processing was characteristic to this species.


Asunto(s)
Antenas de Artrópodos/fisiología , Quimiotaxis , Mariposas Nocturnas/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Atractivos Sexuales/farmacología , Animales , Antenas de Artrópodos/anatomía & histología , Microscopía Confocal , Mariposas Nocturnas/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Neuronas/citología , Neuronas/fisiología , Vías Olfatorias/anatomía & histología , Vías Olfatorias/fisiología
20.
PLoS One ; 11(10): e0164386, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27783640

RESUMEN

Honeybees learn color information of rewarding flowers and recall these memories in future decisions. For fine color discrimination, bees require differential conditioning with a concurrent presentation of target and distractor stimuli to form a long-term memory. Here we investigated whether the long-term storage of color information shapes the neural network of microglomeruli in the mushroom body calyces and if this depends on the type of conditioning. Free-flying honeybees were individually trained to a pair of perceptually similar colors in either absolute conditioning towards one of the colors or in differential conditioning with both colors. Subsequently, bees of either conditioning groups were tested in non-rewarded discrimination tests with the two colors. Only bees trained with differential conditioning preferred the previously learned color, whereas bees of the absolute conditioning group, and a stimuli-naïve group, chose randomly among color stimuli. All bees were then kept individually for three days in the dark to allow for complete long-term memory formation. Whole-mount immunostaining was subsequently used to quantify variation of microglomeruli number and density in the mushroom-body lip and collar. We found no significant differences among groups in neuropil volumes and total microglomeruli numbers, but learning performance was negatively correlated with microglomeruli density in the absolute conditioning group. Based on these findings we aim to promote future research approaches combining behaviorally relevant color learning tests in honeybees under free-flight conditions with neuroimaging analysis; we also discuss possible limitations of this approach.


Asunto(s)
Abejas/fisiología , Color , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/metabolismo , Animales , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Microscopía Confocal , Cuerpos Pedunculados/anatomía & histología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/química , Neurópilo/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...