Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 6.467
Filtrar
1.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(5): 33, 2024 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38771569

RESUMEN

Purpose: This study explored early (contrast discrimination) and intermediate (global form perception) visual processing in primary subtypes of glaucoma: primary open-angle glaucoma (POAG) and primary angle-closure glaucoma (PACG). We aimed to understand early and intermediate visual processing in POAG and PACG, matched for similar visual field defect severity. Methods: Early visual processing was measured using a contrast discrimination task described by Porkorny and Smith (1997), and intermediate processing using a global form perception task using glass pattern coherence thresholds. Thresholds were determined centrally and at a single midperipheral location (12.5°) in a quadrant without visual field defects. Controls were tested in corresponding quadrants to individuals with glaucoma. Results: Sixty participants (20 POAG, 20 PACG, and 20 age-matched controls), aged 50 to 77 years, were included. Visual field defects were matched between POAG and PACG, with mean deviation values of -6.53 ± 4.46 (range: -1.5 to -16.85) dB and -6.2 ± 4.24 (range: -1.37 to -16.42) dB, respectively. Two-Way ANOVA revealed significant differences in thresholds between the glaucoma groups and the control group for both contrast discrimination and global form perception tasks, with higher thresholds in the glaucoma groups. Post hoc analyses showed no significant contrast discrimination difference between POAG and PACG, but POAG had significantly higher thresholds than PACG for form perception. Conclusions: In form perception, POAG showed slightly worse performance than PACG, suggesting that individuals with POAG may experience more severe functional damage than PACG of similar visual field severity.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste , Percepción de Forma , Glaucoma de Ángulo Cerrado , Glaucoma de Ángulo Abierto , Campos Visuales , Humanos , Glaucoma de Ángulo Abierto/fisiopatología , Glaucoma de Ángulo Cerrado/fisiopatología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Anciano , Masculino , Femenino , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Presión Intraocular/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Pruebas del Campo Visual
2.
Vision Res ; 220: 108400, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38603923

RESUMEN

It is well known that objects become grouped in perceptual organization when they share some visual feature, like a common direction of motion. Less well known is that grouping can change how people perceive a set of objects. For example, when a pair of shapes consistently share a common region of space, their aspect ratios tend to be perceived as more similar (are attracted toward each other). Conversely, when shapes are assigned to different regions in space their aspect ratios repel from each other. Here we examine whether the visual system produce both attractive and repulsive distortions when the state of grouping between a pair of shapes changes on a moment-to-moment basis. Observers viewed a pair of ellipses that differed in terms of how flat or tall they were and reported the aspect ratio of one ellipse from the pair. Each ellipse was defined by a cloud of coherently-moving dots, and the dots within the two ellipses had either the same or different directions of motion, varying from trial-to-trial. We found that the cued ellipse's aspect ratio was reported to be repelled from the aspect ratio of the uncued ellipse when the shapes had different directions of motion compared to when they had the same direction of motion. These results suggest that the visual system can adaptively alter visual experience based on grouping, in particular, repelling the appearance of objects when they do not appear to go together, and it can do so quickly and flexibly.


Asunto(s)
Juicio , Percepción de Movimiento , Estimulación Luminosa , Humanos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Juicio/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Masculino , Femenino , Psicofísica , Adulto Joven , Análisis de Varianza , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología
3.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(6): 605-625, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38573695

RESUMEN

Object-based warping is a visual illusion in which dots appear farther apart from each other when superimposed on an object. Previous research found that the illusion's strength varies with the perceived objecthood of the display. We tested whether objecthood alone determines the strength of the visual illusion or if low-level factors separable from objecthood also play a role. In Experiments 1-2, we varied low-level features to assess their impact on the warping illusion. We found that the warping illusion is equally strong for a variety of shapes but varies with the elements by which shape is defined. Shapes composed of continuous edges produced larger warping effects than shapes defined by disconnected elements. In Experiment 3, we varied a display's objecthood while holding low-level features constant. Displays with matched low-level features produced warping effects of the same size even when the perceived unity of the elements in the display varied. In Experiments 4-6, we tested whether displays with low-level features predicted to be important in spatial warping produced the visual illusion even when the display weakly configured into a single object. Results showed that the presence of low-level features like contour solidity and convexity determined warping effect sizes over and above what could be accounted for by the display's perceived objecthood. Our findings challenge the view that the spatial warping illusion is solely object-based. Other factors like the solidity of contours and contours' position relative to reference dots appear to play separate and important roles in determining warping effect sizes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Ilusiones Ópticas , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Humanos , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Ilusiones Ópticas/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Percepción Espacial/fisiología
4.
Jpn J Ophthalmol ; 68(3): 183-191, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38598144

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To assess the impact of glaucoma on perceiving three-dimensional (3D) shapes based on monocular depth cues. STUDY DESIGN: Clinical observational study. METHODS: Twenty glaucoma patients, subjected to binocular visual-field sensitivity (binocular-VFS) tests using a Humphrey Visual Field Analyzer, and 20 age-matched healthy volunteers, underwent two tasks: identifying the nearest vertex of a 3D shape using monocular shading (3D-SfS), texture (3D-SfT), or motion (3D-SfM) cues, and distinguishing elementary one-dimensional (1D) features of these cues. The association of the visual-field index (VFI) of binocular-VFS with 3D shape perception in glaucoma patients was also examined. RESULTS: Glaucoma patients demonstrated reduced accuracy in distinguishing 1D luminance brightness and a larger "error-in-depth" between the perceived and actual depths for 3D-SfM and 3D-SfS compared to healthy volunteers. Six glaucoma patients with a 100% VFI for binocular-VFS exhibited a similar error-in-depth to the other fourteen glaucoma patients; they had a larger error-in-depth for 3D-SfM compared to healthy volunteers. No correlation between the error-in-depth values and the VFI values of binocular-VFS was observed. CONCLUSIONS: The 3D shape perception in glaucoma patients varies based on the depth cue's characteristics. Impaired 1D discrimination and larger thresholds for 3D-SfM in glaucoma patients with a 100% VFI for binocular-VFS indicate more pronounced perceptual deficits of lower-level elementary features for 3D-SfS and higher-level visual processing of 3D shapes for 3D-SfM. The effects of the location and degree of binocular visual-field defects on 3D shape perception remain to be elucidated. Our research provides insights into the 3D shape extraction mechanism in glaucoma.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Percepción de Profundidad , Glaucoma , Visión Binocular , Visión Monocular , Campos Visuales , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Anciano , Glaucoma/fisiopatología , Glaucoma/diagnóstico , Visión Monocular/fisiología , Pruebas del Campo Visual , Presión Intraocular/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Adulto
5.
J Vis ; 24(4): 23, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38662346

RESUMEN

This paper reviews projection models and their perception in realistic pictures, and proposes hypotheses for three-dimensional (3D) shape and space perception in pictures. In these hypotheses, eye fixations, and foveal vision play a central role. Many past theories and experimental studies focus solely on linear perspective. Yet, these theories fail to explain many important perceptual phenomena, including the effectiveness of nonlinear projections. Indeed, few classical paintings strictly obey linear perspective, nor do the best distortion-avoidance techniques for wide-angle computational photography. The hypotheses here employ a two-stage model for 3D human vision. When viewing a picture, the first stage perceives 3D shape for the current gaze. Each fixation has its own perspective projection, but, owing to the nature of foveal and peripheral vision, shape information is obtained primarily for a small region of the picture around the fixation. As a viewer moves their eyes, the second stage continually integrates some of the per-gaze information into an overall interpretation of a picture. The interpretation need not be geometrically stable or consistent over time. It is argued that this framework could explain many disparate pictorial phenomena, including different projection styles throughout art history and computational photography, while being consistent with the constraints of human 3D vision. The paper reviews open questions and suggests new studies to explore these hypotheses.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Fóvea Central/fisiología
6.
Vision Res ; 219: 108394, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38579407

RESUMEN

Contour Integration (CI) is the ability to integrate elemental features into objects and is a basic visual process essential for object perception and recognition, and for functioning in visual environments. It is now well documented that people with schizophrenia (SZ), in addition to having cognitive impairments, also have several visual perceptual deficits, including in CI. Here, we retrospectively characterize the performance of both SZ and neurotypical individuals (NT) on a series of contour shapes, made up of Gabor elements, that varied in terms of closure and curvature. Participants in both groups performed a CI training task that included 7 different families of shapes (Lines, Ellipse, Blobs, Squiggles, Spiral, Circle and Letters) for up to 40 sessions. Two parameters were manipulated in the training task: Orientation Jitter (OJ, i.e., orientation deviations of individual Gabor elements from ideal for each shape) and Inducer Number (IN, i.e., number of Gabor elements defining the shape). Results show that both OJ and IN thresholds significantly differed between the groups, with higher (OJ) and lower (IN) thresholds observed in the controls. Furthermore, we found significant effects as a function of the contour shapes, with differences between groups emerging with contours that were considered more complex, e.g., due to having a higher degree of curvature (Blobs, Spiral, Letters). These data can inform future work that aims to characterize visual integration impairments in schizophrenia.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Esquizofrenia , Humanos , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatología , Adulto , Femenino , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estudios Retrospectivos , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adulto Joven
7.
Nature ; 627(8005): 821-829, 2024 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38448584

RESUMEN

Animals in the natural world constantly encounter geometrically complex landscapes. Successful navigation requires that they understand geometric features of these landscapes, including boundaries, landmarks, corners and curved areas, all of which collectively define the geometry of the environment1-12. Crucial to the reconstruction of the geometric layout of natural environments are concave and convex features, such as corners and protrusions. However, the neural substrates that could underlie the perception of concavity and convexity in the environment remain elusive. Here we show that the dorsal subiculum contains neurons that encode corners across environmental geometries in an allocentric reference frame. Using longitudinal calcium imaging in freely behaving mice, we find that corner cells tune their activity to reflect the geometric properties of corners, including corner angles, wall height and the degree of wall intersection. A separate population of subicular neurons encode convex corners of both larger environments and discrete objects. Both corner cells are non-overlapping with the population of subicular neurons that encode environmental boundaries. Furthermore, corner cells that encode concave or convex corners generalize their activity such that they respond, respectively, to concave or convex curvatures within an environment. Together, our findings suggest that the subiculum contains the geometric information needed to reconstruct the shape and layout of naturalistic spatial environments.


Asunto(s)
Ambiente , Percepción de Forma , Hipocampo , Neuronas , Animales , Femenino , Masculino , Ratones , Calcio/análisis , Calcio/metabolismo , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Hipocampo/citología , Hipocampo/fisiología , Neuronas/metabolismo , Neuronas/fisiología , Propiedades de Superficie
8.
eNeuro ; 11(3)2024 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38423791

RESUMEN

The cortical visual area, V4, has been considered to code contours that contribute to the intermediate-level representation of objects. The neural responses to the complex contour features intrinsic to natural contours are expected to clarify the essence of the representation. To approach the cortical coding of natural contours, we investigated the simultaneous coding of multiple contour features in monkey (Macaca fuscata) V4 neurons and their population-level representation. A substantial number of neurons showed significant tuning for two or more features such as curvature and closure, indicating that a substantial number of V4 neurons simultaneously code multiple contour features. A large portion of the neurons responded vigorously to acutely curved contours that surrounded the center of classical receptive field, suggesting that V4 neurons tend to code prominent features of object contours. The analysis of mutual information (MI) between the neural responses and each contour feature showed that most neurons exhibited similar magnitudes for each type of MI, indicating that many neurons showing the responses depended on multiple contour features. We next examined the population-level representation by using multidimensional scaling analysis. The neural preferences to the multiple contour features and that to natural stimuli compared with silhouette stimuli increased along with the primary and secondary axes, respectively, indicating the contribution of the multiple contour features and surface textures in the population responses. Our analyses suggested that V4 neurons simultaneously code multiple contour features in natural images and represent contour and surface properties in population.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Corteza Visual , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa
9.
Nat Hum Behav ; 8(2): 320-335, 2024 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37996497

RESUMEN

Many surface cues support three-dimensional shape perception, but humans can sometimes still see shape when these features are missing-such as when an object is covered with a draped cloth. Here we propose a framework for three-dimensional shape perception that explains perception in both typical and atypical cases as analysis-by-synthesis, or inference in a generative model of image formation. The model integrates intuitive physics to explain how shape can be inferred from the deformations it causes to other objects, as in cloth draping. Behavioural and computational studies comparing this account with several alternatives show that it best matches human observers (total n = 174) in both accuracy and response times, and is the only model that correlates significantly with human performance on difficult discriminations. We suggest that bottom-up deep neural network models are not fully adequate accounts of human shape perception, and point to how machine vision systems might achieve more human-like robustness.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Humanos , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Redes Neurales de la Computación , Señales (Psicología)
10.
Neural Comput ; 36(1): 33-74, 2023 Dec 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38052088

RESUMEN

Under difficult viewing conditions, the brain's visual system uses a variety of recurrent modulatory mechanisms to augment feedforward processing. One resulting phenomenon is contour integration, which occurs in the primary visual (V1) cortex and strengthens neural responses to edges if they belong to a larger smooth contour. Computational models have contributed to an understanding of the circuit mechanisms of contour integration, but less is known about its role in visual perception. To address this gap, we embedded a biologically grounded model of contour integration in a task-driven artificial neural network and trained it using a gradient-descent variant. We used this model to explore how brain-like contour integration may be optimized for high-level visual objectives as well as its potential roles in perception. When the model was trained to detect contours in a background of random edges, a task commonly used to examine contour integration in the brain, it closely mirrored the brain in terms of behavior, neural responses, and lateral connection patterns. When trained on natural images, the model enhanced weaker contours and distinguished whether two points lay on the same versus different contours. The model learned robust features that generalized well to out-of-training-distribution stimuli. Surprisingly, and in contrast with the synthetic task, a parameter-matched control network without recurrence performed the same as or better than the model on the natural-image tasks. Thus, a contour integration mechanism is not essential to perform these more naturalistic contour-related tasks. Finally, the best performance in all tasks was achieved by a modified contour integration model that did not distinguish between excitatory and inhibitory neurons.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Corteza Visual , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Aprendizaje
11.
J Vis ; 23(14): 4, 2023 Dec 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38091030

RESUMEN

Gestalten in visual perception are defined by emergent properties of the whole, which cannot be predicted from the sum of its parts; rather, they arise by virtue of inherent principles, the Laws of Seeing. This review attempts to assign neurophysiological correlates to select emergent properties in motion and contour perception and proposes parallels to the processing of local versus global attributes by classical versus contextual receptive fields. The aim is to identify Gestalt neurons in the visual system to account for the Laws of Seeing in causal terms and to explain "Why do things look as they do" (Koffka, 1935, p. 76).


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Percepción de Movimiento , Humanos , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología
12.
J Vis ; 23(7): 2, 2023 07 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37405737

RESUMEN

Eye tracking studies suggest that refixations-fixations to locations previously visited-serve to recover information lost or missed during earlier exploration of a visual scene. These studies have largely ignored the role of precursor fixations-previous fixations on locations the eyes return to later. We consider the possibility that preparations to return later are already made during precursor fixations. This process would mark precursor fixations as a special category of fixations, that is, distinct in neural activity from other fixation categories such as refixations and fixations to locations visited only once. To capture the neural signals associated with fixation categories, we analyzed electroencephalograms (EEGs) and eye movements recorded simultaneously in a free-viewing contour search task. We developed a methodological pipeline involving regression-based deconvolution modeling, allowing our analyses to account for overlapping EEG responses owing to the saccade sequence and other oculomotor covariates. We found that precursor fixations were preceded by the largest saccades among the fixation categories. Independent of the effect of saccade length, EEG amplitude was enhanced in precursor fixations compared with the other fixation categories 200 to 400 ms after fixation onsets, most noticeably over the occipital areas. We concluded that precursor fixations play a pivotal role in visual perception, marking the continuous occurrence of transitions between exploratory and exploitative modes of eye movement in natural viewing behavior.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular , Percepción de Forma , Humanos , Movimientos Oculares , Movimientos Sacádicos , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología
13.
J Neurosci ; 43(29): 5378-5390, 2023 07 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37369590

RESUMEN

Radial frequency (RF) patterns, created by sinusoidal modulations of a circle's radius, are processed globally when RF is low. These closed shapes therefore offer a useful way to interrogate the human visual system for global processing of curvature. RF patterns elicit greater responses than those to radial gratings in V4 and more anterior face-selective regions of the ventral visual pathway. This is largely consistent with work on nonhuman primates showing curvature processing emerges in V4, but is evident also higher up the ventral visual stream. Rather than contrasting RF patterns with other stimuli, we presented them at varied frequencies in a regimen that allowed tunings to RF to be derived from 8 human participants (3 female). We found tuning to low RF in lateral occipital areas and to some extent in V4. In a control experiment, we added a high-frequency ripple to the stimuli disrupting the local contour. Low-frequency tuning to these stimuli remained in the ventral visual stream, underscoring its role in global processing of shape curvature. We then used representational similarity analysis to show that, in lateral occipital areas, the neural representation was related to stimulus similarity, when it was computed with a model that captured how stimuli are perceived. We therefore show that global processing of shape curvature emerges in the ventral visual stream as early as V4, but is found more strongly in lateral occipital regions, which exhibit responses and representations that relate well to perception.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We show that tuning to low radial frequencies, known to engage global shape processing mechanisms, was localized to lateral occipital regions. When low-level stimulus properties were accounted for such tuning emerged in V4 and LO2 in addition to the object-selective region LO. We also documented representations of global shape properties in lateral occipital regions, and these representations were predicted well by a proxy of the perceptual difference between the stimuli.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Vías Visuales , Animales , Humanos , Femenino , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Radio (Anatomía) , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Lóbulo Occipital , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa
14.
J Vis ; 23(5): 10, 2023 05 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37171805

RESUMEN

A new source of information is proposed for the perception of three-dimensional (3D) shape from shading that identifies surface concavities from the curvature of the luminance field. Two experiments measured the abilities of human observers to identify concavities on smoothly curved shaded surfaces depicted with several different patterns of illumination and several different material properties. Observers were required to identify any apparent concavities along designated cross sections of the depicted objects and to mark each concavity with an adjustable dot. To analyze the results, we computed both the surface curvature and the luminance curvature along each image cross section. The results revealed that most responses were in concave regions of the luminance profiles, although they were often shifted in phase relative to the curvature of the depicted surfaces. This pattern of performance was surprisingly robust over large changes in the pattern of illumination or surface material properties. Our analysis predicts that observers should make false alarm responses in regions where a luminance concavity does not correspond to a surface concavity, and our empirical results confirm that prediction.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Profundidad , Percepción de Forma , Humanos , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Iluminación , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Propiedades de Superficie , Percepción de Forma/fisiología
15.
J Neurosci ; 43(22): 4129-4143, 2023 05 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37185098

RESUMEN

The mechanisms involved in transforming early visual signals to curvature representations in V4 are unknown. We propose a hierarchical model that reveals V1/V2 encodings that are essential components for this transformation to the reported curvature representations in V4. Then, by relaxing the often-imposed prior of a single Gaussian, V4 shape selectivity is learned in the last layer of the hierarchy from Macaque V4 responses. We found that V4 cells integrate multiple shape parts from the full spatial extent of their receptive fields with similar excitatory and inhibitory contributions. Our results uncover new details in existing data about shape selectivity in V4 neurons that with additional experiments can enhance our understanding of processing in this area. Accordingly, we propose designs for a stimulus set that allow removing shape parts without disturbing the curvature signal to isolate part contributions to V4 responses.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Selectivity to convex and concave shape parts in V4 neurons has been repeatedly reported. Nonetheless, the mechanisms that yield such selectivities in the ventral stream remain unknown. We propose a hierarchical computational model that incorporates findings of the various visual areas involved in shape processing and suggest mechanisms that transform the shape signal from low-level features to convex/concave part representations. Learning shape selectivity from Macaque V4 responses in the final processing stage in our model, we found that V4 neurons integrate shape parts from the full spatial extent of their receptive field with both facilitatory and inhibitory contributions. These results reveal hidden information in existing V4 data that with additional experiments can enhance our understanding of processing in V4.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Corteza Visual , Animales , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Macaca , Neuronas/fisiología , Encéfalo , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa
16.
Psychol Res ; 87(8): 2594-2602, 2023 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37027040

RESUMEN

The evaluation of angular vs. curved forms has a long history in psychology but few of the many studies conducted have examined actual degree of angularity. In two experiments, we present observers with randomly positioned and randomly oriented texture displays of angles viewed within a circular frame. The angle conditions varied from 0° to 180° in 20° increments, covering the entire spectrum of possibilities including acute, obtuse, right, and straight line angles. In Experiment 1, 25 undergraduates rated the perceived beauty of these displays. In Experiment 2, the same stimulus set and procedure were used with 27 participants instead judging perceived threat. Based on the findings in the literature, we predicted that sharper angles would be judged less beautiful and more threatening. The results were mostly confirmed. Acute angles are preferred less but there are also distinct preferences for right angles and straight lines, perhaps due to their greater familiarity in constructed environments. There was a consistent and anticipated finding for threat in the second study: the sharper an angle, the greater its perceived threat. Fear of sharp objects as assessed in a personality questionnaire was found to positively correlate with threat judgements. Future work should look more closely at degree of angularity in embedded object contours and at individual response differences.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Humanos , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Personalidad , Individualidad , Juicio/fisiología , Miedo
17.
Neuroscience ; 514: 79-91, 2023 03 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36736613

RESUMEN

In previous psychophysical work we found that luminance contrast is integrated over retinal area subject to contrast gain control. If different mechanisms perform this operation for a range of superimposed retinal regions of different sizes, this could provide the basis for size-coding. To test this idea we included two novel features in a standard adaptation paradigm to discount more pedestrian accounts of repulsive size-aftereffects. First, we used spatially jittering luminance-contrast adaptors to avoid simple contour displacement aftereffects. Second, we decoupled adaptor and target spatial frequency to avoid the well-known spatial frequency shift aftereffect. Empirical results indicated strong evidence of a bidirectional size adaptation aftereffect. We show that the textbook population model is inappropriate for our results, and develop our existing model of contrast perception to include multiple size mechanisms with divisive surround-suppression from the largest mechanism. For a given stimulus patch, this delivers a blurred step-function of responses across the population, with contrast and size encoded by the height and lateral position of the step. Unlike for textbook population coding schemes, our human results (N = 4 male, N = 4 female) displayed two asymmetries: (i) size aftereffects were greatest for targets smaller than the adaptor, and (ii) on that side of the function, results did not return to baseline, even when targets were 25% of adaptor diameter. Our results and emergent model properties provide evidence for a novel dimension of visual coding (size) and a novel strategy for that coding, consistent with previous results on contrast detection and discrimination for various stimulus sizes.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Percepción de Forma , Estimulación Luminosa , Retina , Percepción del Tamaño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica/métodos , Retina/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología
18.
Vision Res ; 205: 108174, 2023 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36630779

RESUMEN

The tilt aftereffect (TAE) is observed when adaptation to a tilted contour alters the perceived tilt of a subsequently presented contour. Thus far, TAE has been treated as a local aftereffect observed only at the location of the adapter. Whether and how TAE spreads to other locations in the visual field has not been systematically studied. Here, we sought an answer to this question by measuring TAE magnitudes at locations including but not limited to the adapter location. The adapter was a tilted grating presented at the same peripheral location throughout an experimental session. In a single trial, participants indicated the perceived tilt of a test grating presented after the adapter at one of fifteen locations in the same visual hemifield as the adapter. We found non-zero TAE magnitudes in all locations tested, showing that the effect spreads across the tested visual hemifield. Next, to establish a link between neuronal activity and behavioral results and to predict the possible neuronal origins of the spread, we built a computational model based on known characteristics of the visual cortex. The simulation results showed that the model could successfully capture the pattern of the behavioral results. Furthermore, the pattern of the optimized receptive field sizes suggests that mid-level visual areas, such as V4, could be critically involved in TAE and its spread across the visual field.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Percepción de Forma , Humanos , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Campos Visuales
19.
Cortex ; 158: 96-109, 2023 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36495732

RESUMEN

A fundamental aspect of object detection is assigning a border to one (figure) side but not the other (ground) side. Figures are shaped; grounds appear shapeless near the figure border. Accumulating evidence supports the view that the mechanism of figure assignment is inhibitory competition with the figure perceived on the winning side. Suppression has been observed on the groundside of figure borders. One prediction is that more suppression will be observed when the groundside competes more for figural status. We tested this prediction by assessing BOLD activation on the groundside of two types of stimuli with articulated borders: AEnov and AEfam stimuli. In both stimulus types, multiple image-based priors (symmetry, closure, small area, enclosure by a larger region) favored the inside as the figure. In AEfam but not AEnov stimuli, the figural prior of familiar configuration present on the outside competes for figural status. Observers perceived the insides of both types of stimuli as novel figures and the outsides as shapeless grounds. Previously, we observed lower BOLD activation in early visual areas representing the grounds of AEfam than AEnov stimuli, although unexpectedly, activation was above baseline. With articulated borders, it can be difficult to exclude figure activation from ground ROIs. Here, our ground ROIs better excluded figure activation; we also added straight-edge (SE) control stimuli and increased the sample size. In early visual areas representing the grounds, we observed lower BOLD activation on the groundside of AEfam than AEnov stimuli and below-baseline BOLD activation on the groundside of SE and AEfam stimuli. These results, indicating that greater suppression is applied to groundsides that competed more for figural status but lost the competition, support a Bayesian model of figure assignment in which proto-objects activated at both low and high levels where image features and familiar configurations are represented, respectively, compete for figural status.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Humanos , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Teorema de Bayes , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
20.
PLoS One ; 17(7): e0268351, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35802625

RESUMEN

This study demonstrates the functional importance of the Surround context relayed laterally in V1 by the horizontal connectivity, in controlling the latency and the gain of the cortical response to the feedforward visual drive. We report here four main findings: 1) a centripetal apparent motion sequence results in a shortening of the spiking latency of V1 cells, when the orientation of the local inducer and the global motion axis are both co-aligned with the RF orientation preference; 2) this contextual effects grows with visual flow speed, peaking at 150-250°/s when it matches the propagation speed of horizontal connectivity (0.15-0.25 mm/ms); 3) For this speed range, the axial sensitivity of V1 cells is tilted by 90° to become co-aligned with the orientation preference axis; 4) the strength of modulation by the surround context correlates with the spatiotemporal coherence of the apparent motion flow. Our results suggest an internally-generated binding process, linking local (orientation /position) and global (motion/direction) features as early as V1. This long-range diffusion process constitutes a plausible substrate in V1 of the human psychophysical bias in speed estimation for collinear motion. Since it is demonstrated in the anesthetized cat, this novel form of contextual control of the cortical gain and phase is a built-in property in V1, whose expression does not require behavioral attention and top-down control from higher cortical areas. We propose that horizontal connectivity participates in the propagation of an internal "prediction" wave, shaped by visual experience, which links contour co-alignment and global axial motion at an apparent speed in the range of saccade-like eye movements.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Percepción de Movimiento , Corteza Visual , Atención , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Movimiento (Física) , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...