Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 86
Filtrar
1.
J Int Adv Otol ; 20(4): 289-300, 2024 Jul 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39159037

RESUMEN

People with single-sided deafness (SSD) or asymmetric hearing loss (AHL) have particular difficulty understanding speech in noisy listening situations and in sound localization. The objective of this multicenter study is to evaluate the effect of a cochlear implant (CI) in adults with single-sided deafness (SSD) or asymmetric hearing loss (AHL), particularly regarding sound localization and speech intelligibility with additional interest in electric-acoustic pitch matching. A prospective longitudinal study at 7 European tertiary referral centers was conducted including 19 SSD and 16 AHL subjects undergoing cochlear implantation. Sound localization accuracy was investigated in terms of root mean square error and signed bias before and after implantation. Speech recognition in quiet and speech reception thresholds in noise for several spatial configurations were assessed preoperatively and at several post-activation time points. Pitch perception with CI was tracked using pitch matching. Data up to 12 months post activation were collected. In both SSD and AHL subjects, CI significantly improved sound localization for sound sources on the implant side, and thus overall sound localization. Speech recognition in quiet with the implant ear improved significantly. In noise, a significant head shadow effect was found for SSD subjects only. However, the evaluation of AHL subjects was limited by the small sample size. No uniform development of pitch perception with the implant ear was observed. The benefits shown in this study confirm and expand the existing body of evidence for the effectiveness of CI in SSD and AHL. Particularly, improved localization was shown to result from increased localization accuracy on the implant side.


Asunto(s)
Implantación Coclear , Implantes Cocleares , Pérdida Auditiva Unilateral , Localización de Sonidos , Percepción del Habla , Humanos , Implantación Coclear/métodos , Masculino , Localización de Sonidos/fisiología , Femenino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Estudios Prospectivos , Pérdida Auditiva Unilateral/cirugía , Pérdida Auditiva Unilateral/rehabilitación , Pérdida Auditiva Unilateral/fisiopatología , Estudios de Seguimiento , Anciano , Adulto , Europa (Continente) , Estudios Longitudinales , Resultado del Tratamiento , Inteligibilidad del Habla/fisiología , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Sordera/cirugía , Sordera/rehabilitación , Sordera/fisiopatología , Ruido
2.
Cogn Sci ; 48(8): e13486, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39155515

RESUMEN

Research shows that high- and low-pitch sounds can be associated with various meanings. For example, high-pitch sounds are associated with small concepts, whereas low-pitch sounds are associated with large concepts. This study presents three experiments revealing that high-pitch sounds are also associated with open concepts and opening hand actions, while low-pitch sounds are associated with closed concepts and closing hand actions. In Experiment 1, this sound-meaning correspondence effect was shown using the two-alternative forced-choice task, while Experiments 2 and 3 used reaction time tasks to show this interaction. In Experiment 2, high-pitch vocalizations were found to facilitate opening hand gestures, and low-pitch vocalizations were found to facilitate closing hand gestures, when performed simultaneously. In Experiment 3, high-pitched vocalizations were produced particularly rapidly when the visual target stimulus presented an open object, and low-pitched vocalizations were produced particularly rapidly when the target presented a closed object. These findings are discussed concerning the meaning of intonational cues. They are suggested to be based on cross-modally representing conceptual spatial knowledge in sensory, motor, and affective systems. Additionally, this pitch-opening effect might share cognitive processes with other pitch-meaning effects.


Asunto(s)
Tiempo de Reacción , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Gestos , Sonido , Estimulación Acústica , Señales (Psicología)
3.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 156(2): 1111-1122, 2024 Aug 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39145812

RESUMEN

Previous psychological studies have shown that musical consonance is not only determined by the frequency ratios between tones, but also by the frequency spectra of those tones. However, these prior studies used artificial tones, specifically tones built from a small number of pure tones, which do not match the acoustic complexity of real musical instruments. The present experiment therefore investigates tones recorded from a real musical instrument, the Westerkerk Carillon, conducting a "dense rating" experiment where participants (N = 113) rated musical intervals drawn from the continuous range 0-15 semitones. Results show that the traditional consonances of the major third and the minor sixth become dissonances in the carillon and that small intervals (in particular 0.5-2.5 semitones) also become particularly dissonant. Computational modelling shows that these effects are primarily caused by interference between partials (e.g., beating), but that preference for harmonicity is also necessary to produce an accurate overall account of participants' preferences. The results support musicians' writings about the carillon and contribute to ongoing debates about the psychological mechanisms underpinning consonance perception, in particular disputing the recent claim that interference is largely irrelevant to consonance perception.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Acústica , Música , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Simulación por Computador , Espectrografía del Sonido , Adolescente , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Factores de Tiempo , Acústica , Persona de Mediana Edad , Percepción Auditiva
4.
Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) ; 84(2): 136-152, 2024 May 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39087840

RESUMEN

Crossmodal correspondences (CMCs) refer to associations between seemingly arbitrary stimulus features in different sensory modalities. Pitch­size correspondences refer to the strong association of e.g., small objects with high pitches. Pitch­elevation correspondences refer to the strong association of e.g., visuospatial elevated objects with high pitches. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to study the neural components, which underlie the CMCs in pitch­size and spatial pitch­elevation. This study focuses on answering the question of whether or not different CMCs are driven by similar neural mechanisms. The comparison of congruent against incongruent trials allows the estimation of CMC effects across different CMCs. The analysis of the measured neural activity in different CMCs strongly pointed toward different mechanisms which are involved in the processing of pitch­size and pitch­elevation correspondences. Differential, whole brain effects were observed within the superior parietal lobule (SPL), cerebellum and Heschls' gyrus (HG). Further, the angular gyrus (AnG), the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) were engaged in processing the CMCs but showed different effects for processing congruent compared to incongruent stimulus presentations. Within pitch­size significant effects in the AnG and ACC were found for congruent stimulus presentations whereas for pitch­elevation, significant effects in the ACC and IPS were found for incongruent stimulus presentations. In summary, the present results indicated differential neural processing in different simple audio­visual CMCs.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Acústica , Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Humanos , Masculino , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Percepción Visual/fisiología
5.
PLoS One ; 19(7): e0307373, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39024268

RESUMEN

This study aims to investigate the development of pitch-matching, rhythmic entrainment, and socioemotional skills in children who received formal music instruction and other non-music based after school programs. Eighty-three children, averaging 6.81 years old at baseline, were enrolled in either a music, sports, or no after-school program and followed over four years. The music program involved formal and systematic instruction in music theory, instrumental technique, and performance. Most control participants had no music education; however, in some instances, participants received minimal music education at school or at church. Musical development was measured using a pitch-matching and drumming-based rhythmic entrainment task. Sharing behavior was measured using a variation of the dictator game, and empathy was assessed using three different assessments: the Index of Empathy for Children and Adolescence (trait empathy), the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (theory of mind), and a Fiction Emotion-Matching task (state empathy). Results revealed no time-related associations in pitch-matching ability; however, formal music instruction improved pitch-matching relative to controls. On the contrary, improvements in rhythmic entrainment were best explained by age-related changes rather than music instruction. This study also found limited support for a positive association between formal music instruction and socioemotional skills. That is, individuals with formal music instruction exhibited improved emotion-matching relative to those with sports training. In terms of general socioemotional development, children's trait-level affective empathy did not improve over time, while sharing, theory of mind, and state empathy did. Additionally, pitch-matching and rhythmic entrainment did not reliably predict any socioemotional measures, with associations being trivial to small. While formal music instruction benefitted pitch-matching ability and emotion-matching to an audiovisual stimulus, it was not a significant predictor of rhythmic entrainment or broader socioemotional development. These findings suggest that the transfer of music training may be most evident in near or similar domains.


Asunto(s)
Emociones , Empatía , Música , Humanos , Música/psicología , Masculino , Femenino , Niño , Empatía/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Adolescente , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Habilidades Sociales
6.
Hear Res ; 451: 109081, 2024 Sep 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39004015

RESUMEN

Speech-in-noise (SIN) perception is a fundamental ability that declines with aging, as does general cognition. We assess whether auditory cognitive ability, in particular short-term memory for sound features, contributes to both. We examined how auditory memory for fundamental sound features, the carrier frequency and amplitude modulation rate of modulated white noise, contributes to SIN perception. We assessed SIN in 153 healthy participants with varying degrees of hearing loss using measures that require single-digit perception (the Digits-in-Noise, DIN) and sentence perception (Speech-in-Babble, SIB). Independent variables were auditory memory and a range of other factors including the Pure Tone Audiogram (PTA), a measure of dichotic pitch-in-noise perception (Huggins pitch), and demographic variables including age and sex. Multiple linear regression models were compared using Bayesian Model Comparison. The best predictor model for DIN included PTA and Huggins pitch (r2 = 0.32, p < 0.001), whereas the model for SIB included the addition of auditory memory for sound features (r2 = 0.24, p < 0.001). Further analysis demonstrated that auditory memory also explained a significant portion of the variance (28 %) in scores for a screening cognitive test for dementia. Auditory memory for non-speech sounds may therefore provide an important predictor of both SIN and cognitive ability.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Acústica , Cognición , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Ruido , Enmascaramiento Perceptual , Percepción del Habla , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Ruido/efectos adversos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto , Anciano , Adulto Joven , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Teorema de Bayes , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Audiometría de Tonos Puros , Audición , Umbral Auditivo , Pruebas de Audición Dicótica
7.
Hear Res ; 450: 109075, 2024 Sep 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38986164

RESUMEN

Contemporary cochlear implants (CIs) use cathodic-leading symmetric biphasic (C-BP) pulses for electrical stimulation. It remains unclear whether asymmetric pulses emphasizing the anodic or cathodic phase may improve spectral and temporal coding with CIs. This study tested place- and temporal-pitch sensitivity with C-BP, anodic-centered triphasic (A-TP), and cathodic-centered triphasic (C-TP) pulse trains on apical, middle, and basal electrodes in 10 implanted ears. Virtual channel ranking (VCR) thresholds (for place-pitch sensitivity) were measured at both a low and a high pulse rate of 99 (Experiment 1) and 1000 (Experiment 2) pulses per second (pps), and amplitude modulation frequency ranking (AMFR) thresholds (for temporal-pitch sensitivity) were measured at a 1000-pps pulse rate in Experiment 3. All stimuli were presented in monopolar mode. Results of all experiments showed that detection thresholds, most comfortable levels (MCLs), VCR thresholds, and AMFR thresholds were higher on more basal electrodes. C-BP pulses had longer active phase duration and thus lower detection thresholds and MCLs than A-TP and C-TP pulses. Compared to C-TP pulses, A-TP pulses had lower detection thresholds at the 99-pps but not the 1000-pps pulse rate, and had lower MCLs at both pulse rates. A-TP pulses led to lower VCR thresholds than C-BP pulses, and in turn than C-TP pulses, at the 1000-pps pulse rate. However, pulse shape did not affect VCR thresholds at the 99-pps pulse rate (possibly due to the fixed temporal pitch) or AMFR thresholds at the 1000-pps pulse rate (where the overall high performance may have reduced the changes with different pulse shapes). Notably, stronger polarity effect on VCR thresholds (or more improvement in VCR with A-TP than with C-TP pulses) at the 1000-pps pulse rate was associated with stronger polarity effect on detection thresholds at the 99-pps pulse rate (consistent with more degeneration of auditory nerve peripheral processes). The results suggest that A-TP pulses may improve place-pitch sensitivity or spectral coding for CI users, especially in situations with peripheral process degeneration.


Asunto(s)
Umbral Auditivo , Implantación Coclear , Implantes Cocleares , Estimulación Eléctrica , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Anciano , Implantación Coclear/instrumentación , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Personas con Deficiencia Auditiva/psicología , Personas con Deficiencia Auditiva/rehabilitación , Estimulación Acústica , Diseño de Prótesis , Discriminación de la Altura Tonal , Factores de Tiempo
8.
Hear Res ; 450: 109073, 2024 Sep 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38996530

RESUMEN

Tinnitus denotes the perception of a non-environmental sound and might result from aberrant auditory prediction. Successful prediction of formal (e.g., type) and temporal sound characteristics facilitates the filtering of irrelevant information, also labelled as 'sensory gating' (SG). Here, we explored if and how parallel manipulations of formal prediction violations and temporal predictability affect SG in persons with and without tinnitus. Age-, education- and sex-matched persons with and without tinnitus (N = 52) participated and listened to paired-tone oddball sequences, varying in formal (standard vs. deviant pitch) and temporal predictability (isochronous vs. random timing). EEG was recorded from 128 channels and data were analyzed by means of temporal spatial principal component analysis (tsPCA). SG was assessed by amplitude suppression for the 2nd tone in a pair and was observed in P50-like activity in both timing conditions and groups. Correspondingly, deviants elicited overall larger amplitudes than standards. However, only persons without tinnitus displayed a larger N100-like deviance response in the isochronous compared to the random timing condition. This result might imply that persons with tinnitus do not benefit similarly as persons without tinnitus from temporal predictability in deviance processing. Thus, persons with tinnitus might display less temporal sensitivity in auditory processing than persons without tinnitus.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Acústica , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos , Acúfeno , Humanos , Acúfeno/fisiopatología , Acúfeno/diagnóstico , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Análisis de Componente Principal , Filtrado Sensorial , Percepción Auditiva , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven , Anciano , Percepción de la Altura Tonal
9.
PLoS One ; 19(7): e0299784, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38950011

RESUMEN

Observers can discriminate between correct versus incorrect perceptual decisions with feelings of confidence. The centro-parietal positivity build-up rate (CPP slope) has been suggested as a likely neural signature of accumulated evidence, which may guide both perceptual performance and confidence. However, CPP slope also covaries with reaction time, which also covaries with confidence in previous studies, and performance and confidence typically covary; thus, CPP slope may index signatures of perceptual performance rather than confidence per se. Moreover, perceptual metacognition-including neural correlates-has largely been studied in vision, with few exceptions. Thus, we lack understanding of domain-general neural signatures of perceptual metacognition outside vision. Here we designed a novel auditory pitch identification task and collected behavior with simultaneous 32-channel EEG in healthy adults. Participants saw two tone labels which varied in tonal distance on each trial (e.g., C vs D, C vs F), then heard a single auditory tone; they identified which label was correct and rated confidence. We found that pitch identification confidence varied with tonal distance, but performance, metacognitive sensitivity (trial-by-trial covariation of confidence with accuracy), and reaction time did not. Interestingly, however, while CPP slope covaried with performance and reaction time, it did not significantly covary with confidence. We interpret these results to mean that CPP slope is likely a signature of first-order perceptual processing and not confidence-specific signals or computations in auditory tasks. Our novel pitch identification task offers a valuable method to examine the neural correlates of auditory and domain-general perceptual confidence.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Tiempo de Reacción , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Metacognición/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología
10.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 156(1): 638-654, 2024 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39051718

RESUMEN

This experimental study investigated whether infants use iconicity in speech and gesture cues to interpret word meanings. Specifically, we tested infants' sensitivity to size sound symbolism and iconic gesture cues and asked whether combining these cues in a multimodal fashion would enhance infants' sensitivity in a superadditive manner. Thirty-six 14-17-month-old infants participated in a preferential looking task in which they heard a spoken nonword (e.g., "zudzud") while observing a small and large object (e.g., a small and large square). All infants were presented with an iconic cue for object size (small or large) (1) in the pitch of the spoken non-word (high vs low), (2) in gesture (small or large), or (3) congruently in pitch and gesture (e.g., a high pitch and small gesture indicating a small square). Infants did not show a preference for congruently sized objects in any iconic cue condition. Bayes factor analyses showed moderate to strong support for the null hypotheses. In conclusion, 14-17-month-old infants did not use iconic pitch cues, iconic gesture cues, or iconic multimodal cues (pitch and gesture) to associate speech sounds with their referents. These findings challenge theories that emphasize the role of iconicity in early language development.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Gestos , Percepción del Habla , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Femenino , Estimulación Acústica , Teorema de Bayes , Simbolismo , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Comprensión , Percepción del Tamaño
11.
Psychol Aging ; 39(3): 262-274, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38829339

RESUMEN

The redundancy hypothesis proposes that older listeners need a larger array of acoustic cues than younger listeners for effective speech perception. This research investigated this hypothesis by examining the aging effects on the use of prosodic cues in speech segmentation in Mandarin Chinese. We examined how younger and older listeners perceived prosodic boundaries using three main prosodic cues (pause, final lengthening, and pitch change) across eight conditions involving different cue combinations. The stimuli consisted of syntactically ambiguous phrase pairs, each containing two or three objects. Participants (22 younger listeners and 22 older listeners) performed a speech recognition task to judge the number of objects they heard. Both groups primarily relied on the pause cue for identifying prosodic boundaries, using final lengthening and pitch change as secondary cues. However, older listeners showed reduced sensitivity to these cues, compensating by integrating the primary cue pause with the secondary cue pitch change for more precise segmentation. The present study reveals older listeners' integration strategy in using prosodic cues for speech segmentation, supporting the redundancy hypothesis. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Señales (Psicología) , Percepción del Habla , Humanos , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto Joven , Anciano , Adulto , Persona de Mediana Edad , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Factores de Edad
12.
J Comp Psychol ; 138(2): 77-79, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38829346

RESUMEN

Comments on an article by Jay W. Schwartz , Kayleigh H. Pierson, and Alexander K. Reece (see record 2024-19488-001). In this issue, Schwartz et al. (2024) tackle the pitch rule in humans by testing to what extent we use pitch alone to judge emotional arousal across closely and distantly related animal species. The findings of Schwartz et al. open a number of intriguing possibilities for future research: Notably important additional steps would include to further investigate the accuracy of the pitch rule across closely and distantly related species. Upon this, in order to study the evolutionary ancestry of the pitch rule, it will be necessary to study its applicability across nonhumans. Particularly interesting would be the inclusion of subject species that have been found to eavesdrop on heterospecific alarm calls. Previous research (see Hoeschele, 2017 for a review) as well as present findings on human ratings of macaque versus cricket calls also suggest that we should additionally focus on sound features that compliment emotional arousal rating beyond pitch such as spectral information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta , Emociones , Humanos , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Animales , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Especificidad de la Especie
13.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 67(6): 1731-1751, 2024 Jun 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38754028

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: The present study examined whether participants respond to unperturbed parameters while experiencing specific perturbations in auditory feedback. For instance, we aim to determine if speakers adjust voice loudness when only pitch is artificially altered in auditory feedback. This phenomenon is referred to as the "accompanying effect" in the present study. METHOD: Thirty native Mandarin speakers were asked to sustain the vowel /ɛ/ for 3 s while their auditory feedback underwent single shifts in one of the three distinct ways: pitch shift (±100 cents; coded as PT), loudness shift (±6 dB; coded as LD), or first formant (F1) shift (±100 Hz; coded as FM). Participants were instructed to ignore the perturbations in their auditory feedback. Response types were categorized based on pitch, loudness, and F1 for each individual trial, such as Popp_Lopp_Fopp indicating opposing responses in all three domains. RESULTS: The accompanying effect appeared 93% of the time. Bayesian Poisson regression models indicate that opposing responses in all three domains (Popp_Lopp_Fopp) were the most prevalent response type across the conditions (PT, LD, and FM). The more frequently used response types exhibited opposing responses and significantly larger response curves than the less frequently used response types. Following responses became more prevalent only when the perturbed stimuli were perceived as voices from someone else (external references), particularly in the FM condition. In terms of isotropy, loudness and F1 tended to change in the same direction rather than loudness and pitch. CONCLUSION: The presence of the accompanying effect suggests that the motor systems responsible for regulating pitch, loudness, and formants are not entirely independent but rather interconnected to some degree.


Asunto(s)
Teorema de Bayes , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto Joven , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Adulto , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Percepción Sonora/fisiología , Retroalimentación Sensorial/fisiología , Voz/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Acústica del Lenguaje
14.
Multisens Res ; 37(2): 125-141, 2024 Apr 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38714314

RESUMEN

Trust is an aspect critical to human social interaction and research has identified many cues that help in the assimilation of this social trait. Two of these cues are the pitch of the voice and the width-to-height ratio of the face (fWHR). Additionally, research has indicated that the content of a spoken sentence itself has an effect on trustworthiness; a finding that has not yet been brought into multisensory research. The current research aims to investigate previously developed theories on trust in relation to vocal pitch, fWHR, and sentence content in a multimodal setting. Twenty-six female participants were asked to judge the trustworthiness of a voice speaking a neutral or romantic sentence while seeing a face. The average pitch of the voice and the fWHR were varied systematically. Results indicate that the content of the spoken message was an important predictor of trustworthiness extending into multimodality. Further, the mean pitch of the voice and fWHR of the face appeared to be useful indicators in a multimodal setting. These effects interacted with one another across modalities. The data demonstrate that trust in the voice is shaped by task-irrelevant visual stimuli. Future research is encouraged to clarify whether these findings remain consistent across genders, age groups, and languages.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Confianza , Voz , Humanos , Femenino , Voz/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Cara/fisiología , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Adolescente
15.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 155(5): 2990-3004, 2024 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38717206

RESUMEN

Speakers can place their prosodic prominence on any locations within a sentence, generating focus prosody for listeners to perceive new information. This study aimed to investigate age-related changes in the bottom-up processing of focus perception in Jianghuai Mandarin by clarifying the perceptual cues and the auditory processing abilities involved in the identification of focus locations. Young, middle-aged, and older speakers of Jianghuai Mandarin completed a focus identification task and an auditory perception task. The results showed that increasing age led to a decrease in listeners' accuracy rate in identifying focus locations, with all participants performing the worst when dynamic pitch cues were inaccessible. Auditory processing abilities did not predict focus perception performance in young and middle-aged listeners but accounted significantly for the variance in older adults' performance. These findings suggest that age-related deteriorations in focus perception can be largely attributed to declined auditory processing of perceptual cues. Poor ability to extract frequency modulation cues may be the most important underlying psychoacoustic factor for older adults' difficulties in perceiving focus prosody in Jianghuai Mandarin. The results contribute to our understanding of the bottom-up mechanisms involved in linguistic prosody processing in aging adults, particularly in tonal languages.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Señales (Psicología) , Percepción del Habla , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Anciano , Masculino , Femenino , Envejecimiento/psicología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Factores de Edad , Acústica del Lenguaje , Estimulación Acústica , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Lenguaje , Calidad de la Voz , Psicoacústica , Audiometría del Habla
16.
Sci Adv ; 10(20): eadm9797, 2024 May 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38748798

RESUMEN

Both music and language are found in all known human societies, yet no studies have compared similarities and differences between song, speech, and instrumental music on a global scale. In this Registered Report, we analyzed two global datasets: (i) 300 annotated audio recordings representing matched sets of traditional songs, recited lyrics, conversational speech, and instrumental melodies from our 75 coauthors speaking 55 languages; and (ii) 418 previously published adult-directed song and speech recordings from 209 individuals speaking 16 languages. Of our six preregistered predictions, five were strongly supported: Relative to speech, songs use (i) higher pitch, (ii) slower temporal rate, and (iii) more stable pitches, while both songs and speech used similar (iv) pitch interval size and (v) timbral brightness. Exploratory analyses suggest that features vary along a "musi-linguistic" continuum when including instrumental melodies and recited lyrics. Our study provides strong empirical evidence of cross-cultural regularities in music and speech.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Música , Habla , Humanos , Habla/fisiología , Masculino , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Femenino , Adulto , Publicación de Preinscripción
17.
Anim Cogn ; 27(1): 38, 2024 May 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38750339

RESUMEN

This study investigates the musical perception skills of dogs through playback experiments. Dogs were trained to distinguish between two different target locations based on a sequence of four ascending or descending notes. A total of 16 dogs of different breeds, age, and sex, but all of them with at least basic training, were recruited for the study. Dogs received training from their respective owners in a suitable environment within their familiar home settings. The training sequence consisted of notes [Do-Mi-Sol#-Do (C7-E7-G7#-C8; Hz frequency: 2093, 2639, 3322, 4186)] digitally generated as pure sinusoidal tones. The training protocol comprised 3 sequential training levels, with each level consisting of 4 sessions with a minimum of 10 trials per session. In the test phase, the sequence was transposed to evaluate whether dogs used relative pitch when identifying the sequences. A correct response by the dog was recorded as 1, while an incorrect response, occurring when the dog chose the opposite zone of the bowl, was marked as 0. Statistical analyses were performed using a binomial test. Among 16 dogs, only two consistently performed above the chance level, demonstrating the ability to recognize relative pitch, even with transposed sequences. This study suggests that dogs may have the ability to attend to relative pitch, a critical aspect of human musicality.


Asunto(s)
Música , Perros , Animales , Masculino , Femenino , Percepción Auditiva , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Estimulación Acústica
18.
Commun Biol ; 7(1): 540, 2024 May 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38714798

RESUMEN

The genetic influence on human vocal pitch in tonal and non-tonal languages remains largely unknown. In tonal languages, such as Mandarin Chinese, pitch changes differentiate word meanings, whereas in non-tonal languages, such as Icelandic, pitch is used to convey intonation. We addressed this question by searching for genetic associations with interindividual variation in median pitch in a Chinese major depression case-control cohort and compared our results with a genome-wide association study from Iceland. The same genetic variant, rs11046212-T in an intron of the ABCC9 gene, was one of the most strongly associated loci with median pitch in both samples. Our meta-analysis revealed four genome-wide significant hits, including two novel associations. The discovery of genetic variants influencing vocal pitch across both tonal and non-tonal languages suggests the possibility of a common genetic contribution to the human vocal system shared in two distinct populations with languages that differ in tonality (Icelandic and Mandarin).


Asunto(s)
Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Lenguaje , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Adulto , Islandia , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Persona de Mediana Edad , Voz/fisiología , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Pueblo Asiatico/genética
19.
Multisens Res ; 37(3): 217-241, 2024 May 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38762220

RESUMEN

Previous research has revealed congruency effects between different spatial dimensions such as right and up. In the audiovisual context, high-pitched sounds are associated with the spatial dimensions of up/above and front, while low-pitched sounds are associated with the spatial dimensions of down/below and back. This opens the question of whether there could also be a spatial association between above and front and/or below and back. Participants were presented with a high- or low-pitch stimulus at the time of the onset of the visual stimulus. In one block, participants responded according to the above/below location of the visual target stimulus if the target appeared in front of the reference object, and in the other block, they performed these above/below responses if the target appeared at the back of the reference. In general, reaction times revealed an advantage in processing the target location in the front-above and back-below locations. The front-above/back-below effect was more robust concerning the back-below component of the effect, and significantly larger in reaction times that were slower rather than faster than the median value of a participant. However, the pitch did not robustly influence responding to front/back or above/below locations. We propose that this effect might be based on the conceptual association between different spatial dimensions.


Asunto(s)
Tiempo de Reacción , Percepción Espacial , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Estimulación Acústica , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología
20.
Cognition ; 249: 105805, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38761646

RESUMEN

Absolute pitch is the name given to the rare ability to identify a musical note in an automatic and effortless manner without the need for a reference tone. Those individuals with absolute pitch can, for example, name the note they hear, identify all of the tones of a given chord, and/or name the pitches of everyday sounds, such as car horns or sirens. Hence, absolute pitch can be seen as providing a rare example of absolute sensory judgment in audition. Surprisingly, however, the intriguing question of whether such an ability presents unique features in the domain of sensory perception, or whether instead similar perceptual skills also exist in other sensory domains, has not been explicitly addressed previously. In this paper, this question is addressed by systematically reviewing research on absolute pitch using the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) method. Thereafter, we compare absolute pitch with two rare types of sensory experience, namely synaesthesia and eidetic memory, to understand if and how these phenomena exhibit similar features to absolute pitch. Furthermore, a common absolute perceptual ability that has been often compared to absolute pitch, namely colour perception, is also discussed. Arguments are provided supporting the notion that none of the examined abilities can be considered like absolute pitch. Therefore, we conclude by suggesting that absolute pitch does indeed appear to constitute a unique kind of absolute sensory judgment in humans, and we discuss some open issues and novel directions for future research in absolute pitch.


Asunto(s)
Juicio , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Sinestesia , Humanos , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Música
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA