Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 10.795
Filtrar
1.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(5): 20, 2024 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38727692

RESUMEN

Purpose: Vision-degrading myodesopsia (VDM) from vitreous floaters significantly degrades vision and impacts visual quality of life (VQOL), but the relationship to light scattering is poorly understood. This study compared in vitro measures of light scatter and transmission in surgically excised human vitreous to preoperative indexes of vitreous structure, visual function, and VQOL. Methods: Pure vitreous collected during vitrectomy from 8 patients with VDM had wide-angle straylight measurements and dark-field imaging, performed within 36 hours of vitrectomy. Preoperative VQOL assessment with VFQ-25, contrast sensitivity (CS) measurements with Freiburg acuity contrast testing, and quantitative ultrasonography were compared to light scattering and transmission in vitro. Results: All indices of vitreous echodensity in vivo correlated positively with straylight at 0.5° (R = 0.708 to 0.775, P = 0.049 and 0.024, respectively). Straylight mean scatter index correlated with echodensity (R = 0.71, P = 0.04) and VQOL (R = -0.82, P = 0.0075). Dark-field measures in vitro correlated with degraded CS in vivo (R = -0.69, P = 0.04). VQOL correlated with straylight mean scatter index (R = -0.823, P = 0.012). Conclusions: Increased vitreous echodensity in vivo is associated with more straylight scattering in vitro, validating ultrasonography as a clinical surrogate for light scattering. Contrast sensitivity in vivo is more degraded in the presence of dark-field scattering in vitro and VQOL is decreased in patients whose vitreous has increased light scattering. These findings could form the basis for the development of optical corrections for VDM or support new laser treatments, as well as novel pharmacotherapy.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste , Luz , Dispersión de Radiación , Agudeza Visual , Vitrectomía , Cuerpo Vítreo , Humanos , Cuerpo Vítreo/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Anciano , Calidad de Vida , Trastornos de la Visión/fisiopatología , Adulto , Ultrasonografía , Oftalmopatías/fisiopatología , Oftalmopatías/diagnóstico por imagen
2.
J Vis ; 24(5): 1, 2024 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38691088

RESUMEN

Still life paintings comprise a wealth of data on visual perception. Prior work has shown that the color statistics of objects show a marked bias for warm colors. Here, we ask about the relative chromatic contrast of these object-associated colors compared with background colors in still life paintings. We reasoned that, owing to the memory color effect, where the color of familiar objects is perceived more saturated, warm colors will be relatively more saturated than cool colors in still life paintings as compared with photographs. We analyzed color in 108 slides of still life paintings of fruit from the teaching slide collection of the Fogg University Art Museum and 41 color-calibrated photographs of fruit from the McGill data set. The results show that the relatively higher chromatic contrast of warm colors was greater for paintings compared with photographs, consistent with the hypothesis.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color , Frutas , Pinturas , Fotograbar , Humanos , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Fotograbar/métodos , Color , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología
3.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(5): 7, 2024 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38700875

RESUMEN

Purpose: This study aimed to explore the underlying mechanisms of the observed visuomotor deficit in amblyopia. Methods: Twenty-four amblyopic (25.8 ± 3.8 years; 15 males) and 22 normal participants (25.8 ± 2.1 years; 8 males) took part in the study. The participants were instructed to continuously track a randomly moving Gaussian target on a computer screen using a mouse. In experiment 1, the participants performed the tracking task at six different target sizes. In experiments 2 and 3, they were asked to track a target with the contrast adjusted to individual's threshold. The tracking performance was represented by the kernel function calculated as the cross-correlation between the target and mouse displacements. The peak, latency, and width of the kernel were extracted and compared between the two groups. Results: In experiment 1, target size had a significant effect on the kernel peak (F(1.649, 46.170) = 200.958, P = 4.420 × 10-22). At the smallest target size, the peak in the amblyopic group was significantly lower than that in the normal group (0.089 ± 0.023 vs. 0.107 ± 0.020, t(28) = -2.390, P = 0.024) and correlated with the contrast sensitivity function (r = 0.739, P = 0.002) in the amblyopic eyes. In experiments 2 and 3, with equally visible stimuli, there were still differences in the kernel between the two groups (all Ps < 0.05). Conclusions: When stimulus visibility was compensated, amblyopic participants still showed significantly poorer tracking performance.


Asunto(s)
Ambliopía , Agudeza Visual , Humanos , Ambliopía/fisiopatología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Psicofísica/métodos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología
4.
Accid Anal Prev ; 202: 107599, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38669900

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: We examined collision warning systems with different modalities and timing thresholds, assessing their impact on responses to pedestrian hazards by drivers with impaired contrast sensitivity (ICS). METHODS: Seventeen ICS (70-84 y, median CS 1.35 log units) and 17 normal vision (NV: 68-73 y, median CS 1.95) participants completed 6 city drives in a simulator with 3 bimodal warnings: visual-auditory, visual-directional-tactile, and visual-non-directional-tactile. Each modality had one drive with early and one with late warnings, triggered at 3.5 s and 2 s time-to-collision, respectively. RESULTS: ICS participants triggered more early (43 vs 37 %) and late warnings (12 vs 6 %) than NV participants and had more collisions (3 vs 0 %). Early warnings reduced time to fixate hazards (late 1.9 vs early 1.2 s, p < 0.001), brake response times (2.8 vs 1.8 s, p < 0.001) and collision rates (1.2 vs 0.02 %). With late warnings, ICS participants took 0.7 s longer to brake than NV (p < 0.001) and had an 11 % collision rate (vs 0.7 % with early warnings). Non-directional-tactile warnings yielded the lowest collision rates for ICS participants (4 vs auditory 12 vs directional-tactile 15.2 %) in late warning scenarios. All ICS participants preferred early warnings. CONCLUSIONS: While early warnings improved hazard responses and reduced collisions for ICS participants, late warnings did not, resulting in high collision rates. In contrast, both early and late warnings were helpful for NV drivers. Non-directional-tactile warnings were the most effective in reducing collisions. The findings provide insights relevant to the development of hazard warnings tailored for drivers with impaired vision.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes de Tránsito , Conducción de Automóvil , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Tiempo de Reacción , Humanos , Anciano , Masculino , Femenino , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Accidentes de Tránsito/prevención & control , Simulación por Computador , Trastornos de la Visión , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Equipos de Seguridad , Factores de Tiempo
5.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(4)2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38652553

RESUMEN

Luminance and spatial contrast provide information on the surfaces and edges of objects. We investigated neural responses to black and white surfaces in the primary visual cortex (V1) of mice and monkeys. Unlike primates that use their fovea to inspect objects with high acuity, mice lack a fovea and have low visual acuity. It thus remains unclear whether monkeys and mice share similar neural mechanisms to process surfaces. The animals were presented with white or black surfaces and the population responses were measured at high spatial and temporal resolution using voltage-sensitive dye imaging. In mice, the population response to the surface was not edge-dominated with a tendency to center-dominance, whereas in monkeys the response was edge-dominated with a "hole" in the center of the surface. The population response to the surfaces in both species exhibited suppression relative to a grating stimulus. These results reveal the differences in spatial patterns to luminance surfaces in the V1 of mice and monkeys and provide evidence for a shared suppression process relative to grating.


Asunto(s)
Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Estimulación Luminosa , Animales , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Ratones , Masculino , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Corteza Visual Primaria/fisiología , Especificidad de la Especie , Imagen de Colorante Sensible al Voltaje , Macaca mulatta
6.
BMC Ophthalmol ; 24(1): 183, 2024 Apr 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38649861

RESUMEN

AIM: To evaluate the objective visual outcomes following implantation of extended depth of focus intraocular lens (EDOF IOL) in individuals with varying axial lengths (AL) and targeted refraction. METHODS: This retrospective study comprised age-matched eyes that underwent implantation of the EDOF IOL. Eyes were categorized based on AL into groups: control group with AL < 26 mm; high myopia group with AL ≥ 26 mm. Each group was then subdivided based on postoperative spherical equivalent (SE). Follow-up at three months included assessment of uncorrected visual acuity at different distances, contrast sensitivity (CS), refractive outcomes, and spectacle independence. RESULTS: Overall, this study included 100 eyes from 100 patients, comprising 50 males (50.00%) and 50 females (50.00%), with 20 eyes in each group. In the control group, the uncorrected distance visual acuity (UDVA) at 5 and 3 m (m) in the - 1.50 to -0.75 group was inferior to that of the - 0.75 to 0.00 group (P = 0.004). Conversely, the uncorrected near visual acuity (UNVA) at 33 cm in the - 1.50 to -0.75 group was superior to that of the - 0.75 to 0.00 group (P = 0.005). Within the high myopia group, the UDVA at 5 and 3 m in the - 2.25 to -1.50 group was worse than in the - 0.75 to 0.00 group (P = 0.009 and 0.008, respectively). However, the UNVA at 33 cm in the - 2.25 to -1.50 group was better than in the - 0.75 to 0.00 group (P = 0.020). No significant differences were observed among the groups for corrected distance visual acuity (CDVA) (P > 0.05). Additionally, in the high myopia group, the CS of the - 2.25 to -1.50 group was lower compared to that of the - 0.75 to 0.00 group (P = 0.017). Among high myopia patients, 90.00% with refraction ranging from - 1.50 to -0.75 reported achieving overall spectacle independence. CONCLUSIONS: Implantation of extended depth of focus intraocular lenses (IOLs) yields satisfactory visual and refractive outcomes in eyes with axial myopia. Among high myopia patients, a refraction ranging from - 1.50 to -0.75 diopters achieves superior visual quality compared to other postoperative myopic diopters.


Asunto(s)
Implantación de Lentes Intraoculares , Lentes Intraoculares , Miopía , Refracción Ocular , Agudeza Visual , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Estudios Retrospectivos , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Refracción Ocular/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Miopía/fisiopatología , Miopía/cirugía , Anciano , Diseño de Prótesis , Adulto , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Facoemulsificación , Seudofaquia/fisiopatología , Longitud Axial del Ojo , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Estudios de Seguimiento
7.
Optom Vis Sci ; 101(4): 195-203, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38684062

RESUMEN

SIGNIFICANCE: Spatio-Temporal Optical Phase technology utilizes film pairs containing optical elements applied to standard single-vision spectacle lenses. This technology provides a dynamic optical cue that may have efficacy in reducing the rate of myopia progression, but the visual performance of this technology is unknown. PURPOSE: This study aimed to assess the visual performance of film pairs containing optical elements (tests) and a film pair with no optical elements (control). METHODS: In this randomized, single-masked, bilateral wear study, 42 participants aged 18 to 40 years wore four test designs (E, F-1, G, and F-2) and the control. Subjective data (subjective ratings [1 to 10 scale]: clarity of vision [far-away, intermediate, near] and vision [at night, while walking, overall satisfaction], and willingness to purchase [yes/no response]) were collected after 3 days. Visual acuity (VA)-based measures (monocular high/low-contrast VA [6 m], contrast sensitivity [6 m], and binocular high-contrast VA [6 m and 40 cm]) were collected at dispensing. Visual acuity-based measures were also collected while wearing spectacles with no film. Analyses were performed using linear mixed models and the χ2 test. Significance was set at 5%. RESULTS: The control performed better than any test for all subjective ratings (mean differences, 1.6 to 3.1 units: p<0.001), willingness to purchase (p<0.001), and designs F-1 and F-2 for binocular high-contrast VA at 40 cm (p=0.001 and p=0.01, respectively). Clarity of vision was significantly worse with F-2 compared with F-1 and G (p<0.001 and p=0.02, respectively). There were no differences between tests for any other subjective rating (p>0.1), willingness to purchase (p=0.11), or any VA-based measure (p>0.08). There were no differences between control and spectacles with no film for any VA-based measure (p>0.08). CONCLUSIONS: All four test film pairs reduced visual performance compared with control to a degree comparable with other myopia management devices. There was no difference in visual performance between three of the four test film pairs.


Asunto(s)
Anteojos , Agudeza Visual , Humanos , Adulto , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Femenino , Masculino , Adolescente , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Miopía/fisiopatología , Miopía/terapia , Método Simple Ciego , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Diseño de Equipo , Refracción Ocular/fisiología
8.
Turk J Ophthalmol ; 54(2): 63-68, 2024 Apr 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38644781

RESUMEN

Objectives: To evaluate the clinical results of a new trifocal intraocular lens (IOL) with sinusoidal design by comparing with a traditional trifocal IOL. Materials and Methods: A total of 79 patients undergoing uneventful microincisional cataract surgery with bilateral implantation of one of two types of trifocal IOLs, the Acriva Trinova IOL (VSY) or Acrysof IQ PanOptix IOL (Alcon), were enrolled in this prospective study. Visual and refractive outcomes, contrast sensitivity (CS), and defocus curve were assessed at 3 months after surgery. Patient satisfaction and incidence of photic phenomena were also evaluated. Results: The number of patients/eyes were 48/96 in the Trinova group and 31/62 in the PanOptix group. There were no significant differences between the groups for monocular and binocular corrected/uncorrected distance or intermediate (at 60 cm) and near visual acuities (VA) postoperatively. The Trinova group had statistically significantly better intermediate VA at 80 cm than the PanOptix group (p<0.05). The CS results of both groups were within the normal limits. In the binocular defocus curve of both IOLs, we observed a peak of good VA at 0.0 diopters defocus and a useful wide range for intermediate distances. The incidence of photic phenomena in the Trinova group was lower at postoperative 1 month (p<0.05) but this difference disappeared at 3 months. A total of 47 patients (97.9%) in the Trinova group and 30 patients (96.7%) in the PanOptix group stated that they would recommend the same IOL. Conclusion: Both trifocal IOLs provide good visual quality outcomes and patient satisfaction.


Asunto(s)
Lentes Intraoculares Multifocales , Presbiopía , Diseño de Prótesis , Refracción Ocular , Agudeza Visual , Humanos , Estudios Prospectivos , Femenino , Masculino , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Anciano , Persona de Mediana Edad , Presbiopía/fisiopatología , Presbiopía/cirugía , Refracción Ocular/fisiología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Satisfacción del Paciente , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Facoemulsificación , Estudios de Seguimiento , Implantación de Lentes Intraoculares/métodos , Lentes Intraoculares
9.
Zhonghua Yi Xue Za Zhi ; 104(16): 1391-1396, 2024 Apr 23.
Artículo en Chino | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38644289

RESUMEN

Objective: To explore the effects of different types of intraocular lens (IOL) implantation on patient's visual quality and function after phacoemulsification. Methods: The clinical data of patients with monocular cataract who underwent phacoemulsification in the Department of Ophthalmology, People's Hospital Affiliated to Shandong First Medical University between December 2021 and May 2023 were retrospectively analyzed. According to the types of IOL, the patients were divided into monofocal group, bifocal group and depth of focus extension group. Three months later, uncorrected distance visual acuity (UCDVA), best corrected distance visual acuity (BCDVA), uncorrected intermediate visual acuity (UCIVA), best corrected intermediate visual acuity (BCIVA), uncorrected near visual acuity (UCNVA) and best corrected near visual acuity (BCNVA) were detected. Contrast sensitivity and total wavefront aberration were measured by visual function analyzer. Satisfaction with visual quality was evaluated by hospital-made satisfaction questionnaire. Results: A total of 92 patients were included, with 31 males and 61 females, and their age was (61.8±5.2) years. There were 43, 28 and 21 cases in monofocal group, bifocal group and depth of focus extension group, respectively. No statistically significant difference was found in clinical baseline data among the three groups. UCIVA, UCDVA, BCIVA and BCDVA in depth of focus extension group were 1.01±0.13, 0.92±0.18, 1.21±0.19 and 1.20±0.23, respectively, which were higher than those in monofocal group (0.62±0.12, 0.74±0.13, 1.02±0.17, 1.07±0.19, respectively) and bifocal group (0.67±0.15, 0.78±0.14, 1.01±0.16, 1.01±0.18, respectively), while absolute value of spherical equivalent [(-0.42±0.07) D] was lower than that in the other two groups [ (-0.49±0.05) D and (-0.45±0.08) D] (both P<0.05). UCNVA and BCNVA in bifocal group were 0.91±0.18 and 1.25±0.18, which were higher than those in depth of focus extension group (0.63±0.24 and 1.19±0.17) (both P<0.05). There were no significant differences in contrast sensitivity among the three groups under day vision or between monofocal group and bifocal group under night vision (all P>0.05), but the contrast sensitivity was higher in depth of focus extension group under night vision (3.0, 6.0, 12.0 c/d) than other two groups (all P<0.05). The score of ocular discomfort was the highest in bifocal group, followed by depth of focus extension group and monofocal group (both P<0.05). The score of visual interference in bifocal group was lower than that in monofocal group and depth of focus extension group (both P<0.05). The scores of subjective feeling in bifocal group and depth of focus extension group were higher than that in monofocal group (both P<0.05). The reading score was the highest in bifocal group, followed by depth of focus extension group and monofocal group (both P<0.05). There was no significant difference in total low-order aberration among the three groups (P=0.472). The total aberration and higher-order aberration [(0.74±0.35) µm and (0.41±0.12) µm] were the highest in monofocal group, followed by bifocal group [(0.61±0.21) µm and (0.22±0.09) µm] and depth of focus extension group [(0.46±0.13) µm and (0.06±0.09) µm] (all P<0.05). Conclusions: IOL implantation with depth of focus extension can enhance visual range, night vision and contrast sensitivity, and thus effectively improve postoperative visual quality and function in cataract patients. The bifocal IOL can better improve the patient's UCNVA and BCNVA, resulting in high satisfaction with visual quality.


Asunto(s)
Catarata , Implantación de Lentes Intraoculares , Lentes Intraoculares , Facoemulsificación , Agudeza Visual , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estudios Retrospectivos , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Satisfacción del Paciente , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
10.
Transl Vis Sci Technol ; 13(4): 3, 2024 Apr 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38564201

RESUMEN

Purpose: The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effects of posterior vitreous detachment (PVD) on visual quality in patients with high myopia, as well as investigate the associated factors of photopic and mesopic contrast sensitivity function (CSF) in high myopia. Methods: Visual quality was comprehensively assessed in patients with high myopia. Visual acuity, contrast sensitivity (CS) at four spatial frequencies (3, 6, 12, and 18 cycles per degree [c.p.d.]) under photopic and mesopic conditions, as well as the modulation transfer function cutoff value (MTFcutoff), the objective scatter index (OSI), the Strehl ratio (SR), and internal aberrations, were measured in this cross-sectional study. Results: This study included 94 eyes from 47 subjects with bilateral high myopia, including 23 eyes with complete PVD (cPVD), 21 eyes with partial PVD (pPVD), and 50 eyes without PVD (nPVD). There was no significant difference in visual quality between the cPVD group and the nPVD group. Whereas in eyes with pPVD, there was a degradation of overall photopic CSF (versus nPVD, P = 0.048), photopic CS at 3 c.p.d. (versus cPVD, P = 0.009 and versus nPVD, P = 0.032), photopic CS at 18 c.p.d. (versus nPVD, P = 0.033), overall mesopic CSF (versus nPVD, P = 0.033), and secondary astigmatism (versus cPVD, P = 0.044). Under photopic conditions, the factors affecting CSF were pPVD and SR, whereas the factors affecting mesopic CSF were pPVD, OSI, and ganglion cell-inner plexiform layer thickness. Conclusions: The pPVD impaired visual quality in patients with high myopia compared to nPVD or cPVD, and pPVD could be a factor explaining CSF at both photopic and mesopic illumination. Translational Relevance: Clinicians need to closely monitor patients with high myopia with pPVD due to the potential decline in visual quality and the development of vitreoretinal disorders.


Asunto(s)
Miopía , Desprendimiento del Vítreo , Humanos , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Estudios Transversales , Miopía/complicaciones , Miopía/diagnóstico , Retina
11.
J Vis ; 24(4): 5, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38573602

RESUMEN

The contrast sensitivity function (CSF) is a fundamental visual model explaining our ability to detect small contrast patterns. CSFs found many applications in engineering, where they can be used to optimize a design for perceptual limits. To serve such a purpose, CSFs must explain possibly a complete set of stimulus parameters, such as spatial and temporal frequency, luminance, and others. Although numerous contrast sensitivity measurements can be found in the literature, none fully explains the complete space of stimulus parameters. Therefore, in this work, we first collect and consolidate contrast sensitivity measurements from 18 studies, which explain the sensitivity variation across the parameters of interest. Then, we build an analytical contrast sensitivity model that explains the data from all those studies. The proposed castleCSF model explains the sensitivity as the function of spatial and temporal frequencies, an arbitrary contrast modulation direction in the color space, mean luminance, and chromaticity of the background, eccentricity, and stimulus area. The proposed model uses the same set of parameters to explain the data from 18 studies with an error of 3.59 dB. The consolidated contrast sensitivity data and the code for the model are publicly available at https://github.com/gfxdisp/castleCSF/.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste , Humanos
12.
Vision Res ; 219: 108396, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38640684

RESUMEN

Recent studies suggest that binocular adding S+ and differencing S- channels play an important role in binocular vision. To test for such a role in the context of binocular contrast detection and binocular summation, we employed a surround masking paradigm consisting of a central target disk surrounded by a mask annulus. All stimuli were horizontally oriented 0.5c/d sinusoidal gratings. Correlated stimuli were identical in interocular spatial phase while anticorrelated stimuli were opposite in interocular spatial phase. There were four target conditions: monocular left eye, monocular right eye, binocular correlated and binocular anticorrelated, and three surround mask conditions: no surround, binocularly correlated and binocularly anticorrelated. We observed consistent elevation of detection thresholds for monocular and binocular targets across the two binocular surround mask conditions. In addition, we found an interaction between the type of surround and the type of binocular target: both detection and summation were relatively enhanced by surround masks and targets with opposite interocular phase relationships and reduced by surround masks and targets with the same interocular phase relationships. The data were reasonably well accounted for by a model of binocular combination termed MAX (S+S-), in which the decision variable is the probability summation of modeled S+ and S- channel responses, with a free parameter determining the relative gains of the two channels. Our results support the existence of two channels involved in binocular combination, S+ and S-, whose relative gains are adjustable by surround context.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste , Enmascaramiento Perceptual , Estimulación Luminosa , Umbral Sensorial , Visión Binocular , Humanos , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica , Visión Monocular/fisiología , Adulto
13.
Indian J Ophthalmol ; 72(Suppl 3): S505-S508, 2024 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38648459

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: The Pelli-Robson and LEA contrast sensitivity charts are commonly used in clinical settings to measure contrast sensitivity. Although the Pelli-Robson chart is considered the gold standard, it is limited by its bulky size. The LEA chart, on the contrary, offers a more practical and portable option that is still reliable. This has led to questions about whether we can predict Pelli-Robson scores based on LEA scores. This study developed a conversion method to help transition from the LEA chart to the Pelli-Robson chart and validate the conversion score. METHODS: In this retrospective study, we analyzed the relationship between LEA and the Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity test. Our study examined a total of 120 eyes. We developed a conversion table through the equipercentile equating method. Subsequently, we assessed the reliability and accuracy of this algorithm for converting LEA results into Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity scores. RESULTS: The study used a conversion table to convert LEA scores to Pelli-Robson scores. The conversion table achieved a reliability of 0.91 based on intraclass correlation, and the algorithm had an accuracy of 81.6% within a 1-point difference from the raw score. CONCLUSIONS: This study reported a reliable and comparable conversion algorithm for transforming LEA scores into converted estimated Pelli-Robson scores, thereby improving the usefulness of existing data in both clinical and research contexts.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste , Humanos , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Estudios Retrospectivos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas de Visión/métodos , Pruebas de Visión/instrumentación , Oftalmología/métodos , Adulto Joven , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Neurología/métodos , Algoritmos , Anciano , Adolescente
14.
J Vis ; 24(4): 21, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38656529

RESUMEN

Conscious perception is preceded by long periods of unconscious processing. These periods are crucial for analyzing temporal information and for solving the many ill-posed problems of vision. An important question is what starts and ends these windows and how they may be interrupted. Most experimental paradigms do not offer the methodology required for such investigation. Here, we used the sequential metacontrast paradigm, in which two streams of lines, expanding from the center to the periphery, are presented, and participants are asked to attend to one of the motion streams. If several lines in the attended motion stream are offset, the offsets are known to integrate mandatorily and unconsciously, even if separated by up to 450 ms. Using this paradigm, we here found that external visual objects, such as an annulus, presented during the motion stream, do not disrupt mandatory temporal integration. Thus, if a window is started once, it appears to remain open even in the presence of disruptions that are known to interrupt visual processes normally. Further, we found that interrupting the motion stream with a gap disrupts temporal integration but does not terminate the overall unconscious processing window. Thus, while temporal integration is key to unconscious processing, not all stimuli in the same processing window are integrated together. These results strengthen the case for unconscious processing taking place in windows of sensemaking, during which temporal integration occurs in a flexible and perceptually meaningful manner.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Estimulación Luminosa , Inconsciente en Psicología , Humanos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Masculino , Femenino , Factores de Tiempo , Atención/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología
15.
Curr Biol ; 34(9): 1940-1952.e5, 2024 May 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38640924

RESUMEN

The primary visual cortex (V1) and the superior colliculus (SC) both occupy stations early in the processing of visual information. They have long been thought to perform distinct functions, with the V1 supporting the perception of visual features and the SC regulating orienting to visual inputs. However, growing evidence suggests that the SC supports the perception of many of the same visual features traditionally associated with the V1. To distinguish V1 and SC contributions to visual processing, it is critical to determine whether both areas causally contribute to the detection of specific visual stimuli. Here, mice reported changes in visual contrast or luminance near their perceptual threshold while white noise patterns of optogenetic stimulation were delivered to V1 or SC inhibitory neurons. We then performed a reverse correlation analysis on the optogenetic stimuli to estimate a neuronal-behavioral kernel (NBK), a moment-to-moment estimate of the impact of V1 or SC inhibition on stimulus detection. We show that the earliest moments of stimulus-evoked activity in the SC are critical for the detection of both luminance and contrast changes. Strikingly, there was a robust stimulus-aligned modulation in the V1 contrast-detection NBK but no sign of a comparable modulation for luminance detection. The data suggest that behavioral detection of visual contrast depends on both V1 and SC spiking, whereas mice preferentially use SC activity to detect changes in luminance. Electrophysiological recordings showed that neurons in both the SC and V1 responded strongly to both visual stimulus types, while the reverse correlation analysis reveals when these neuronal signals actually contribute to visually guided behaviors.


Asunto(s)
Optogenética , Estimulación Luminosa , Colículos Superiores , Percepción Visual , Animales , Ratones , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Corteza Visual Primaria/fisiología , Masculino , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Neuronas/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Femenino , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología
16.
Eye (Lond) ; 38(Suppl 1): 9-14, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38580741

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND/OBJECTIVE: To evaluate the visual performance of a purely refractive extended depth of focus (EDF) intraocular lens (IOL). SUBJECTS/METHODS: A prospective, multi-center, randomized, subject/evaluator-masked study. Subjects were bilaterally implanted with the EDF test (Model ZEN00V, N = 60) or an enhanced monofocal control (Model ICB00, N = 57) IOL. Monocular corrected distance (CDVA), intermediate (DCIVA), near acuities (DCNVA) and patient reported visual symptoms were evaluated at the 6-month visit. Monocular mesopic contrast sensitivity (CS) and depth of focus (DOF) testing were assessed at 3 months. RESULTS: CDVA (Mean ± SD) was -0.06 ± 0.08 for test and -0.05 ± 0.08 logMAR for control groups. DCIVA was 0.13 ± 0.08 for test and 0.18 ± 0.14 logMAR for control groups (p = 0.0127). DCNVA was 0.37 ± 0.10 for test and 0.43 ± 0.16 logMAR for control groups (p = 0.0137). Test lens was statistically superior for intermediate and near. Overall, 91.7% (halos), 95.0% (starbursts) and 95.0% (glare) of test lens patients reported that they did not experience, were not bothered, or were slightly bothered by specific visual symptoms, compared to 98.2%, 100% and 96.5% in the control group. The DOF range over which monocular visual acuity was 0.20 logMAR or better was -1.6 D for the test lens. Mesopic CS was comparable between both groups, falling within 0.11 log units for all measured cycles per degree with and without glare. CONCLUSION: The EDF IOL demonstrated extended range of vision and statistically superior intermediate and near performance compared to the monofocal IOL. Distance visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and dysphotopsia profile were similar to the monofocal IOL.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste , Percepción de Profundidad , Implantación de Lentes Intraoculares , Lentes Intraoculares , Seudofaquia , Agudeza Visual , Humanos , Estudios Prospectivos , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Femenino , Masculino , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Anciano , Persona de Mediana Edad , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Seudofaquia/fisiopatología , Diseño de Prótesis , Refracción Ocular/fisiología , Facoemulsificación , Método Doble Ciego , Satisfacción del Paciente
17.
J Vis ; 24(4): 24, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38683571

RESUMEN

The perceived slant of a stereoscopic surface is altered by the presence of a surrounding surface, a phenomenon termed stereo slant contrast. Previous studies have shown that a slanted surround causes a fronto-parallel surface to appear slanted in the opposite direction, an instance of "bidirectional" contrast. A few studies have examined slant contrast using slanted as opposed to fronto-parallel test surfaces, and these also have shown slant contrast. Here, we use a matching method to examine slant contrast over a wide range of combinations of surround and test slants, one aim being to determine whether stereo slant contrast transfers across opposite directions of test and surround slant. We also examine the effect of the test on the perceived slant of the surround. Test slant contrast was found to be bidirectional in virtually all test-surround combinations and transferred across opposite test and surround slants, with little or no decline in magnitude as the test-surround slant difference approached the limit. There was a weak bidirectional effect of the test slant on the perceived slant of the surround. We consider how our results might be explained by four mechanisms: (a) normalization of stereo slant to vertical; (b) divisive normalization of stereo slant channels in a manner analogous to the tilt illusion; (c) interactions between center and surround disparity-gradient detectors; and (d) uncertainty in slant estimation. We conclude that the third of these (interactions between center and surround disparity-gradient detectors) is the most likely cause of stereo slant contrast.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste , Percepción de Profundidad , Humanos , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Visión Binocular/fisiología
18.
Vision Res ; 219: 108393, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38579405

RESUMEN

Recent studies have revealed that pupillary response changes depend on perceptual factors such as subjective brightness caused by optical illusions and luminance. However, the manner in which the perceptual factor that is derived from the glossiness perception of object surfaces affects the pupillary response remains unclear. We investigated the relationship between the glossiness perception and pupillary response through a glossiness rating experiment that included recording the pupil diameter. We prepared general object images (original) and randomized images (shuffled) that comprised the same images with randomized small square regions as stimuli. The image features were controlled by matching the luminance histogram. The observers were asked to rate the perceived glossiness of the stimuli presented for 3,000 ms and the changes in their pupil diameters were recorded. Images with higher glossiness ratings constricted the pupil size more than those with lower glossiness ratings at the peak constriction of the pupillary responses during the stimulus duration. The linear mixed-effects model demonstrated that the glossiness rating, image category (original/shuffled), variance of the luminance histogram, and stimulus area were most effective in predicting the pupillary responses. These results suggest that the illusory brightness obtained by the image regions of high-glossiness objects, such as specular highlights, induce pupil constriction.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Luminosa , Pupila , Humanos , Pupila/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Ilusiones Ópticas/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología
19.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 86(4): 1303-1317, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38468024

RESUMEN

Proximity and feature similarity are two important determinants of perceptual grouping in vision. When viewing visual scenes conveying both grouping options simultaneously, people most usually detect proximity groups faster than similarity groups. This article demonstrates that perceptual judgments of grouping orientation guided by either proximity or contrast similarity are indicative of a sequential organization of grouping operations in the visual pathway, which lends a temporal processing advantage to proximity grouping (Experiment 1). Invoking the double-factorial paradigm, latent cognitive architecture for perceptual grouping is also investigated in a task with redundant signals (Experiment 2). Reaction time data from this task is assessed in terms of the race model inequality, workload capacity analysis, and interaction contrasts of means and survivor functions. Again, empirical benchmarks indicate serial processing of proximity groups and similarity groups, with a self-terminating stopping rule for processing. A subset of participants exhibit atypical performance metrics, hinting at possible individual differences in configural visual processing.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Tiempo de Reacción , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Atención , Adulto Joven , Orientación , Adulto , Juicio , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Vías Visuales/fisiología
20.
J Neurosci ; 44(19)2024 May 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38548339

RESUMEN

Perception is a probabilistic process dependent on external stimulus properties and one's internal state. However, which internal states influence perception and via what mechanisms remain debated. We studied how spontaneous alpha-band activity (8-13 Hz) and pupil fluctuations impact visual detection and confidence across stimulus contrast levels (i.e., the contrast response function, CRF). In human subjects of both sexes, we found that low prestimulus alpha power induced an "additive" shift in the CRF, whereby stimuli were reported present more frequently at all contrast levels, including contrast of zero (i.e., false alarms). Conversely, prestimulus pupil size had a "multiplicative" effect on detection such that stimuli occurring during large pupil states (putatively corresponding to higher arousal) were perceived more frequently as contrast increased. Signal detection modeling reveals that alpha power changes detection criteria equally across the CRF but not detection sensitivity (d'), whereas pupil-linked arousal modulated sensitivity, particularly for higher contrasts. Interestingly, pupil size and alpha power were positively correlated, meaning that some of the effect of alpha on detection may be mediated by pupil fluctuations. However, pupil-independent alpha still induced an additive shift in the CRF corresponding to a criterion effect. Our data imply that low alpha boosts detection and confidence by an additive factor, rather than by a multiplicative scaling of contrast responses, a profile which captures the effect of pupil-linked arousal. We suggest that alpha power and arousal fluctuations have dissociable effects on behavior. Alpha reflects the baseline level of visual excitability, which can vary independent of arousal.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo alfa , Nivel de Alerta , Pupila , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Pupila/fisiología , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Adulto , Ritmo alfa/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...