Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 4.004
Filtrar
1.
Physiol Res ; 73(2): 205-216, 2024 Apr 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38710050

RESUMEN

ADHD is a common chronic neurodevelopmental disorder and is characterized by persistent inattention, hyperactivity, impulsivity and are often accompanied by learning and memory impairment. Great evidence has shown that learning and memory impairment of ADHD plays an important role in its executive function deficits, which seriously affects the development of academic, cognitive and daily social skills and will cause a serious burden on families and society. With the increasing attention paid to learning and memory impairment in ADHD, relevant research is gradually increasing. In this article, we will present the current research results of learning and memory impairment in ADHD from the following aspects. Firstly, the animal models of ADHD, which display the core symptoms of ADHD as well as with learning and memory impairment. Secondly, the molecular mechanism of has explored, including some neurotransmitters, receptors, RNAs, etc. Thirdly, the susceptibility gene of ADHD related to the learning and impairment in order to have a more comprehensive understanding of the pathogenesis. Key words: Learning and memory, ADHD, Review.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Trastornos de la Memoria , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Humanos , Animales , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Aprendizaje , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje/psicología , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje/etiología , Memoria
2.
JAMA Intern Med ; 184(5): 590, 2024 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38436984

RESUMEN

This JAMA Internal Medicine Patient Page describes memory loss and dementia and how they may be treated.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Memoria , Humanos , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología
3.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord ; 53(2): 74-82, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38408448

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Previous studies have indicated a correlation between perceived stress and cognitive decline. However, it remains unknown whether high levels of perceived stress can result in motoric cognitive risk (MCR) syndrome. This study investigated the relationship between perceived stress and MCR in a community-based population. METHODS: The study cohort comprised 852 elderly individuals from the Rugao Longitudinal Aging Cohort. Perceived stress was assessed using the 10-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10), while MCR was defined as the coexistence of subjective memory complaints (SMCs) and slow gait speed. RESULTS: The average age of the study participants is 79.84 ± 4.34 years. The mean score of PSS-10 among participants is 10.32 (range = 0-33; [SD] = 5.71), with a median score of 10.00 (6.00, 14.00). The prevalence of MCR is 9.3%. In the logistic regression analysis, for each 1-SD (5.71) increase in the global PSS-10 score, the risk of MCR increased by 40% (95% CI 1.09-1.80). Additionally, in the aspect of two components of MCR, with a 1-SD increase (5.71) in the global PSS-10 score, there was a 50% (95% CI 1.29-1.75) increase in the risk of SMCs and a 27% (95% CI 1.04-1.55) increase in the risk of slow gait speed. In terms of specific walking speed, there was a reverse correlation between the global PSS-10 score and walking speed (r = -0.14, p < 0.001). CONCLUSIONS: This study provided preliminary evidence that high levels of perceived stress were associated with the risk of MCR in a community-dwelling population.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Disfunción Cognitiva , Estrés Psicológico , Humanos , Masculino , Anciano , Femenino , Estrés Psicológico/epidemiología , Estrés Psicológico/psicología , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Disfunción Cognitiva/epidemiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Estudios Longitudinales , Velocidad al Caminar , Longevidad , Factores de Riesgo , Prevalencia , Estudios de Cohortes , Trastornos de la Memoria/epidemiología , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
4.
Brain Inj ; 38(3): 170-176, 2024 02 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38287215

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Memory problems are among the most frequently reported cognitive complaints by individuals with an acquired brain injury (ABI). Processing speed and working memory deficits are often the result of ABI. These cognitive deficits significantly impact the acquisition and retention of information necessary for memory formation. This study investigated the influence of processing speed and working memory on immediate and delayed recall for verbal and visual memory, as well as overall memory recall in adults living with a chronic ABI. METHODS: Sixty-three participants living with a chronic ABI, who were at least one-year post-injury, were cognitively assessed with the CNS-Vital Signs (CNS-VS) computerized cognitive battery and Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. RESULTS: The CNS-VS Processing Speed significantly predicted delayed recall for verbal memory and overall memory performance. The CNS-VS Working Memory was not a significant predictor of memory recall. CONCLUSIONS: Processing speed deficits negatively impact memory in individuals with a chronic ABI. These findings suggest the memory recall of adults with a chronic ABI is associated with poor processing speed and poor acquisition of information. Therefore, cognitive rehabilitation that improves processing speed should be the focus for individuals with ABI to improve memory performance as well as impaired processing speed.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Encefálicas , Lesión Encefálica Crónica , Adulto , Humanos , Velocidad de Procesamiento , Memoria , Daño Encefálico Crónico , Lesiones Encefálicas/rehabilitación , Cognición , Lesión Encefálica Crónica/complicaciones , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
5.
Exp Clin Psychopharmacol ; 32(2): 236-244, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37732959

RESUMEN

Stereotype threat occurs when individuals from stigmatized groups feel they are expected to conform to a negative stereotype associated with their group. Studies show that activating stereotype threat can impair performance on cognitive tasks in various marginalized groups. Individuals with problematic alcohol use are subject to stigmatized views related to cognitive abilities and socialization skills; thus, we examine for the first time whether eliciting stereotype threat impairs performance on a memory and a theory of mind task in undergraduate students with varying drinking patterns. We randomized 205 students to a neutral or a stereotype threat condition, which informed participants that the purpose of the study was to assess memory performance and theory of mind skills in relation to different patterns of alcohol consumption. In the stereotype threat group, individuals with problematic drinking patterns demonstrated significantly worse memory performance than nonproblematic drinkers and nondrinkers. The same was not true in the neutral condition, where memory recall did not differ significantly as a function of drinking status. Experimental group and drinking status failed to reveal significant effects on cognitive and affective theory of mind performance. Problematic alcohol use patterns were only associated with poorer memory when stereotype threat was elicited, which indicates that assessments of neurocognitive profiles may be biased, at least for memory performance, if stereotype threat is inadvertently elicited in substance users. Broader implications support the imperative to avoid stigmatization of problematic substance use in scientific communication and clinical settings. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Estereotipo , Humanos , Memoria , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas
6.
Rev Esp Geriatr Gerontol ; 59(1): 101426, 2024.
Artículo en Español | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37922626

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Subjective memory complaints (SMC) might be an early sign of further deterioration in cognitive functions. However, no population studies have been published covering all Spain to determine the SMC prevalence. The objective of the present study was to determine the SMC prevalence in the general population residing in Spain >50 years of age and also which related actions were done. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Interviews were conducted with a nationally representative sample >50 years of age using an online/computer assisted web interview (CAWI) questionnaire. The survey consisted of 34 items divided into two blocks. Presence of SMC was considered when in question 1 the subject answered that "Yes" he had some memory problem. RESULTS: Two thousand three hundred people (53.7% women; 23.9% ≥75 years old) were interviewed. 31% answered that they considered they had a memory problem (no differences between sexes, mean time with memory problems of 3.0 years). A higher prevalence of SMC was observed in ≥75 years old (44%). 90% did not include any specific foods in their diet for their memory problems, neither were taking any pharmaceuticals, diet supplements or nutritional supplements (92%). 78% of those interviewed with SMC have not consulted health professionals for their memory problems. CONCLUSIONS: SMC are considerably prevalent in our environment affecting almost a third of people ≥50 years of age. Most of the interviewees with SMC did not go to health professionals to manage their SMC.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Memoria , Proyectos de Investigación , Masculino , Humanos , Femenino , Anciano , España/epidemiología , Prevalencia , Trastornos de la Memoria/epidemiología , Trastornos de la Memoria/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Cognición , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
7.
PeerJ ; 11: e16389, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38025740

RESUMEN

Background: Since 2020, information regarding COVID-19 has been a constant presence in the news, in our conversations and thoughts. Continuous exposure to this type of stimuli could have an impact on cognitive processes essential for our everyday activities, such as prospective memory (PM). PM is the ability to remember to perform an intention at a specific point in the future, like remembering to take prescribed medicines at a specific time or to turn off the stove after cooking. Do COVID-related stimuli affect our ability to perform a PM task? Methods: To answer this question, we proposed a novel version of the classical paradigm used to investigate PM. Namely, this paradigm includes a baseline condition, in which an ongoing task is presented alone, and a PM condition in which the same task is proposed again together with a second (prospective) task. In this study, a short video clip was presented between the baseline and the PM condition. The video clip displayed either neutral, negative, or COVID-related content. Additionally, participants were asked to respond to two questionnaires and a series of questions regarding their well-being and experience with the pandemic. Namely, the DASS-21 scale (evaluating depression, anxiety, and stress), and the COVID-19-PTSD questionnaire (a questionnaire evaluating post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms related to the pandemic experience) were administered. Participants' performance and responses were analyzed using a linear mixed effect (LME) model approach, and correlation analyses were run to highlight possible correlations between participants' scores in the DASS-21, the COVID-19-PTSD, and the additional questions on their personal experience with the pandemic. Results: The LME models revealed significant effects of the displayed video on performance: in line with previous studies, the clip displaying standard negative contents led to impaired accuracy in the ongoing task in the PM condition, compared to the Baseline. In contrast, participants who saw the COVID-related clip showed improved accuracy in the ongoing task compared to the other participants, selectively in the block performed after the video clip was displayed (PM condition). Furthermore, the explanatory power of the LME model calculated on accuracy to the ongoing trials was enhanced by the inclusion of the scores in the anxiety subscale of the DASS-21, suggesting a detrimental role of anxiety. Altogether, these results indicate a different effect of the exposure to classical negative contents (associated with a cost in terms of accuracy in the ongoing task between the baseline and the PM condition) and the pandemic-related one, which was instead characterized by a higher accuracy to ongoing trials compared to the other video clips. This counterintuitive finding seems to suggest that COVID-related stimuli are processed as "acute stressors" rather than negative stimuli, thus inducing a state of increased alertness and responsivity.


Asunto(s)
COVID-19 , Memoria Episódica , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático , Humanos , Atención/fisiología , Trastornos de Ansiedad , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología
8.
Behav Brain Sci ; 46: e365, 2023 11 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37961776

RESUMEN

Dementia syndromes offer a unique opportunity to clarify some of the component processes of spontaneous expressions of memory proposed by the Barzykowski and Moulin model. By considering the model through the lens of memory disorders, I outline several important extensions to progress our understanding of these spontaneous cognitive phenomena.


Asunto(s)
Demencia , Trastornos de la Memoria , Humanos , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Demencia/psicología
9.
BMC Geriatr ; 23(1): 670, 2023 10 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37848825

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Everyday memory problems are believed to increase with age, leading many researchers to focus on older ages when examining reports of memory lapses. However, real world memory lapses are ubiquitous across the adult lifespan, though less is known about the types of problems and their impacts at younger ages. The current study examined occurrence and impacts of memory lapses using daily diaries in a broad age range and whether characteristics of lapses varied across age, gender, or education level. METHODS: Using an 8-day daily diary protocol, 2,018 individuals (ages 25-91) provided reports of their experiences of two types of daily memory lapses (retrospective and prospective) as well as the impact those lapses had on their emotional and functional well-being that day. Using multilevel modeling, we examined the likelihood of reporting memory lapses and their impacts on daily life and whether these depended on age, gender, or education level. RESULTS: Participants reported lapses on approximately 40% of days; retrospective memory lapses were significantly more likely than prospective lapses. Older ages and higher education level were related to greater likelihood of reporting retrospective lapses. Women (compared to men) were more likely to report prospective memory lapses. Women also tended to report greater impacts of their memory lapses. Lower education levels were related to greater impacts of memory lapses compared to higher education levels. Interestingly, age was not related to impacts of lapses. DISCUSSION: Our results indicate that memory lapses are common across the lifespan and that those individuals more likely to report lapses are not necessarily those that experience the greatest impacts of those lapses on daily life. Additional work is needed to understand the daily experience of memory lapses and how they differentially affect individuals regardless of age, gender, and education. CONCLUSIONS: Memory lapses are an important aspect of daily life across the lifespan and require measurement in an individual's real-world environments. Better measurement of these experiences will allow the development of more sensitive measures of changes in cognitive functioning that may impact an individual's ability to live independently.


Asunto(s)
Individualidad , Longevidad , Masculino , Humanos , Femenino , Estudios Retrospectivos , Trastornos de la Memoria/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Memoria/epidemiología , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Cognición
10.
J Alzheimers Dis ; 96(2): 579-589, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37840488

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Subjective memory complaints and family history of dementia are possibly intertwined risk factors for the own subsequent dementia risk and Alzheimer's disease. However, their interaction has rarely been studied. OBJECTIVE: To study the association between subjective memory complaints and family history of dementia with regard to the own subsequent risk of dementia. METHODS: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses over a follow-up period of up to 13 years were conducted in a population sample of participants without dementia at baseline (n = 3,256, mean age = 79.62 years), using group comparisons and Cox proportional hazards models. RESULTS: Cross-sectionally, participants with subjective memory complaints were significantly more likely to report family history of dementia. Longitudinally, family history of dementia (FH) was significantly associated with subsequent dementia in the subjective memory complaints (SMC) group, but not in those without SMC. A relative excess risk due to interaction analysis confirmed a significant FHxSMC-interaction. CONCLUSIONS: Family history of dementia was a predictor of incident dementia in those with SMC, which can serve as an additional, clinically relevant criterion to gauge the risk of dementia in older-aged subjects with SMC with and without objective cognitive impairment.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva , Demencia , Humanos , Anciano , Demencia/diagnóstico , Demencia/epidemiología , Demencia/genética , Estudios Transversales , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Disfunción Cognitiva/epidemiología , Factores de Riesgo , Estudios de Cohortes , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
12.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 28(6): 424-436, 2023 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37889124

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Patients with functional disorders (FD) often experience cognitive problems such as forgetfulness and distractibility alongside physical symptoms that cannot be attributed to a known somatic disease. METHOD: Test scores of cognitive tests and psychiatric rating scales of 100 outpatients diagnosed with a functional disorder were compared to a control group (n = 300) of patients with other diagnoses and to test norms for the general population. RESULTS: Out of the 100 patients with functional disorders, 59 reported significant subjective cognitive symptoms. A moderate difference (d = 0.5-0.7) was found between the FD group mean and the population mean in processing speed tests, as well as in four psychiatric rating scales (depression, anxiety, phobias, somatisation) but there were no statistically significant differences in verbal and nonverbal reasoning or in logical memory. Somatisation and logical verbal memory scores were higher in the FD group compared to the control group. CONCLUSION: The findings of the study suggest that a decline in processing speed is a central feature in the cognitive profile of patients with functional disorders.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Conocimiento , Cognición , Humanos , Trastornos del Conocimiento/psicología , Memoria , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica
13.
Psychol Aging ; 38(7): 725-739, 2023 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37650797

RESUMEN

Older adults may be confronted with a variety of negative aging stereotypes (e.g., "forgetful," "physically frail," and "lonely") almost every day. While experimental studies have demonstrated the impact of negative aging stereotypes on older adults' cognitive performance, the relationship between multiple negative aging stereotype experiences and cognitive functioning in older people's daily lives is largely unknown. Using a 1-week daily diary study approach, the present studies examined the association between experiencing daily negative aging stereotypes and real-life cognitive functioning and the mediating role of daily negative affect. We also examined whether these associations differed in response to negative aging stereotype diversity, which refers to the breadth and evenness of negative aging stereotypes that older adults experience in 1 week. In Study 1 (n = 50), we explored the negative aging stereotypes that older adults often experience in their daily lives. These stereotypes can be seen as a measure of daily stereotype experiences. Then in Study 2, 100 older adults reported their everyday experiences of negative aging stereotypes, positive or negative affect, and cognitive failures for 1 week. The results showed that the more negative aging stereotype experiences older adults had in 1 day, the more cognitive failures occurred that day and that daily negative affect mediated this association. Further, the direct and indirect relationships between daily negative aging stereotype experiences and daily cognitive failures were significant in older adults with high negative aging stereotype diversity but not in those with low negative aging stereotype diversity. This study provides new insights for studies on aging stereotypes threat and offers theoretical guidance for future interventions for cognitive health in older adults and, in turn, contributes to promoting healthy aging in older adults. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Cognición , Humanos , Anciano , Envejecimiento/psicología , Estereotipo , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología
14.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 11782, 2023 07 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37479846

RESUMEN

Engagement in risky and impulsive behavior has long been associated with deficits in neurocognition. However, we have a limited understanding of how multiple subfunctions of neurocognition co-occur within individuals and which combinations of neurocognitive subfunctions are most relevant for risky and impulsive behavior. Using the neurotypical Nathan Kline Institute Rockland Sample (N = 673), we applied a Bayesian latent feature learning model-the Indian Buffet Process-to identify nuanced, individual-specific profiles of multiple neurocognitive subfunctions and examine their relationship to risky and impulsive behavior. All features were within a relatively normative range of neurocognition; however, there was subtle variability related to risky and impulsive behaviors. The relatively overall poorer neurocognition feature correlated with greater affective impulsivity and substance use patterns/problems. The poorer episodic memory and emotion feature correlated with greater trait externalizing and sensation-seeking. The poorer attention feature correlated with increased trait externalizing and negative urgency but decreased positive urgency and substance use. Finally, the average or mixed features negatively correlated with various risky and impulsive behaviors. Estimating nuanced patterns of co-occurring neurocognitive functions can inform our understanding of a continuum of risky and impulsive behaviors.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Impulsiva , Trastornos Neurocognitivos , Asunción de Riesgos , Humanos , Teorema de Bayes , Emociones , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Trastornos Neurocognitivos/psicología
15.
Cortex ; 166: 428-440, 2023 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37423786

RESUMEN

Unawareness of memory deficits is an early manifestation in patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD), which often delays diagnosis. This intriguing behavior constitutes a form of anosognosia, whose neural mechanisms remain largely unknown. We hypothesized that anosognosia may depend on a critical synaptic failure in the error-monitoring system, which would prevent AD patients from being aware of their own memory impairment. To investigate, we measured event-related potentials (ERPs) evoked by erroneous responses during a word memory recognition task in two groups of amyloid positive individuals with only subjective memory complaints at study entry: those who progressed to AD within the five-year study period (PROG group), and those who remained cognitively normal (CTRL group). A significant reduction in the amplitude of the positivity error (Pe), an ERP related to error awareness, was observed in the PROG group at the time of AD diagnosis (vs study entry) in intra-group analysis, as well as when compared with the CTRL group in inter-group analysis, based on the last EEG acquisition for all subjects. Importantly, at the time of AD diagnosis, the PROG group exhibited clinical signs of anosognosia, overestimating their cognitive abilities, as evidenced by the discrepancy scores obtained from caregiver/informant vs participant reports on the cognitive subscale of the Healthy Aging Brain Care Monitor. To our knowledge, this is the first study to reveal the emergence of a failure in the error-monitoring system during a word memory recognition task at the early stages of AD. This finding, along with the decline of awareness for cognitive impairment observed in the PROG group, strongly suggests that a synaptic dysfunction in the error-monitoring system may be the critical neural mechanism at the origin of unawareness of deficits in AD.


Asunto(s)
Agnosia , Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Trastornos de la Memoria , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/fisiopatología , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/psicología , Trastornos de la Memoria/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Memoria/fisiopatología , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Agnosia/diagnóstico , Agnosia/fisiopatología , Agnosia/psicología , Sinapsis , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
16.
J Alzheimers Dis ; 95(1): 69-74, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37483005

RESUMEN

Subjective memory is useful to detect cognitive deficits not apparent on objective tests. This research evaluates whether memory rated by an interviewer predicts incident dementia. Health and Retirement Study participants without cognitive impairment at baseline whose memory was rated by the interviewer were analyzed (N = 12,749). Worse interviewer-rated memory was associated with higher risk of incident dementia over 15 years (HR = 1.40, 95% CI = 1.27-1.54). The association was evident even among participants in the top quartile of objective memory performance (HR = 1.71, 95% CI = 1.26-2.32). Interviewer-rated memory may be a low-cost supplement to neuropsychological evaluation to predict dementia risk beyond self-reports and objective cognitive testing.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Conocimiento , Disfunción Cognitiva , Demencia , Humanos , Demencia/diagnóstico , Demencia/epidemiología , Demencia/psicología , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Disfunción Cognitiva/epidemiología , Cognición , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología
17.
J Glob Health ; 13: 04068, 2023 Jul 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37499129

RESUMEN

Background: Sensory impairments and eye diseases increase the risk of cognitive decline, but little is known regarding their influence on cognitive function in elderly Chinese and the underlying mechanisms. We aimed to explore these influence mechanism from the social participation perspective. Methods: We selected 2876 respondents aged ≥60 from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS) conducted in 2013, 2015, and 2018. We assessed sensory impairments and eye diseases based on self-reported responses, and evaluated its relation to social participation and cognitive function by fixed-effects regression and mediation effect analysis over a five-year period. Results: Respondents with visual impairment and cataracts had poor memory and mental status. Compared with near visual impairment, distance visual impairment was associated with a 1.7 times higher likelihood of cognitive decline (correlation coefficient (ß) = -0.051; 95% confidence interval (CI) = -0.065, -0.036)). Respondents with hearing impairment had bad memory (ß = -0.046; 95% CI = -0.065, -0.036), but not mental status. Social participation partially mediated the relationships of sensory impairments and cataracts with cognitive function in elderly Chinese. Individuals with sensory impairments affected by limited social participation reported a faster cognitive decline compared to those with eye disease. Conclusions: We found that sensory impairments and eye diseases were negatively associated with cognitive function. Furthermore, sensory impairments and cataracts influence cognitive function partly via social participation. Our results have important theoretical and practical implications and suggests that early interventions for sensory impairments and eye diseases may improve the cognitive function of elderly people.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Trastornos de la Audición , Participación Social , Trastornos de la Visión , Anciano , Humanos , Catarata/complicaciones , China/epidemiología , Cognición/fisiología , Pueblos del Este de Asia , Estudios Longitudinales , Trastornos de la Visión/complicaciones , Trastornos de la Visión/epidemiología , Trastornos de la Visión/psicología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Trastornos de la Audición/complicaciones , Trastornos de la Audición/epidemiología , Trastornos de la Audición/psicología , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología
18.
Drug Alcohol Rev ; 42(7): 1754-1763, 2023 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37469227

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Alcohol use has been linked to impairment in both short- and long-term measures of objective memory. However, limited research has investigated the association between alcohol use and subjective memory in everyday life. The study purpose was to investigate within- and between-person associations between daily alcohol use and prospective (i.e., forgetting an intended task) and retrospective (i.e., forgetting something learned in the past) memory lapses among middle-aged and older adults. METHODS: Participants (n = 925; Mage = 55.2) were non-abstaining adults from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study or the MIDUS Refresher who participated in an 8-day telephone diary asking about their daily experiences. RESULTS: Multilevel models revealed that within-individuals, heavier-than-usual alcohol use (i.e., having more drinks than one's daily average number of drinks) was associated with greater odds of reporting any memory lapses (odds ratio [OR] 1.06; 95% confidence interval [CI] 1.01, 1.12), while associations at the between-person level were nonsignificant (OR 1.07; 95% CI 0.99, 1.16). When assessing retrospective and prospective lapses separately, alcohol use was only associated with prospective lapses and only at the between-person level (OR 1.10; 95% CI 1.01, 1.19). Finally, alcohol use was unassociated with reported irritation or interference from memory lapses (p > 0.05). DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS: Heavier-than-usual alcohol use may have acute effects on daily memory functioning. Future studies should assess how alcohol use relates to an individual's ability to meet daily cognitive demands, as these findings may have critical implications for harm reduction efforts targeting daily functioning among older adults.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas , Trastornos de la Memoria , Persona de Mediana Edad , Humanos , Estados Unidos , Anciano , Estudios Retrospectivos , Estudios Prospectivos , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/epidemiología
19.
Contemp Clin Trials ; 131: 107249, 2023 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37268243

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Treatments that delay progression of cognitive impairment in older adults are of great public health significance. This manuscript outlines the protocol, recruitment, baseline characteristics, and retention for a randomized controlled trial of cognitive and aerobic physical training to improve cognition in individuals with subjective cognitive dysfunction, the "Cognitive and Aerobic Resilience for the Brain" (CARB) study. METHODS: Community-dwelling, older adults with self-reported memory loss were randomly assigned to receive either computer-based cognitive training, aerobic physical training, combined cognitive and physical training, or education control. Treatment was delivered 2- to 3-times per week in 45- to 90-min sessions for 12 weeks by trained facilitators videoconferencing into subject's home. Outcome assessments of were taken at the baseline, immediately following training, and 3-months after training. RESULTS: 191 subjects were randomized into the trial (mean age, 75.5 years; 68% female; 20% non-white; mean education, 15.1 years; 30% with 1+ APOE e4 allele). The sample was generally obese, hypertensive, and many were diabetic, while cognition, self-reported mood, and activities of daily living were in the normal range. There was excellent retention throughout the trial. Interventions were completed at high rates, participants found the treatments acceptable and enjoyable, and outcome assessments were completed at high rates. CONCLUSIONS: This study was designed to determine the feasibility of recruiting, intervening, and documenting response to treatment in a population at risk for progressive cognitive decline. Older adults with self-reported memory loss were enrolled in high numbers and were well engaged with the intervention and outcome assessments.


Asunto(s)
Actividades Cotidianas , Disfunción Cognitiva , Humanos , Femenino , Anciano , Masculino , Encéfalo , Cognición , Disfunción Cognitiva/terapia , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Trastornos de la Memoria/terapia , Resultado del Tratamiento
20.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 190: 1-7, 2023 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37270144

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Cognitive impairment in depression can present as subjective or objective, but the intensity of the former is higher and is not correlated with the deficits measured in neuropsychological tests. We hypothesised that rumination would be associated with subjective cognitive impairment. METHODS: The study was performed through the online PsyToolkit platform. It included 168 healthy people and 93 people with depression. To examine memory, a recognition-type task was used with emotionally charged words as the stimulus. Depression symptoms were measured using the Beck Depression Inventory-II, subjective cognitive impairment with the Perceived Deficits Questionnaire-20, and intensity of rumination with the Polish Questionnaire of Rumination. RESULTS: MDD patients had significantly higher levels of depression symptoms, rumination, and subjective cognitive deficits than the control group. In the memory task, the MDD group had a higher error rate than the control group. In hierarchical regression analysis, depression and rumination were found to be significant predictors of subjective cognitive impairment, whereas objective memory performance was not. Exploratory analyses revealed that rumination mediates the association between depression and subjective cognitive complaints. CONCLUSION: Cognitive problems are common in depression and affect the quality of life. The results suggest that patients with depression have higher levels of rumination and subjective memory impairment, and that there is no direct link between subjective and objective cognitive decline. The findings may have implications for the development of effective treatment strategies for depression and cognitive impairment.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Conocimiento , Disfunción Cognitiva , Humanos , Calidad de Vida , Cognición , Disfunción Cognitiva/complicaciones , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Depresión/diagnóstico
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...