Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Montrer: 20 | 50 | 100
Résultats 1 - 14 de 14
Filtrer
1.
Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol ; 162: 111281, 2022 Nov.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36001910

RÉSUMÉ

OBJECTIVES: The main goal of this study was to describe the expressive vocabulary of school-age children with mild to moderately severe hearing loss (CHL group) and to compare their performance with children with normal hearing (CNH group) of the same age. Another aim was to examine the interaction between nonword repetition and expressive vocabulary size. Furthermore, the interaction between results on vocabulary tests and background factors, such as the age of diagnosis, age of hearing aid fitting, and amount of hearing aid use, was explored. METHOD: School-aged children with mild to moderately severe, permanent bilateral hearing loss and children with normal hearing were included in this cross-sectional study. The children participated in assessments of naming pictures, defining words, and repetition of nonwords and sentences. Results of the CHL group and the CNH group were compared. The analysis also included the degree of hearing loss, the age of diagnosis, amount of hearing aid use, and level of parental education. RESULTS: The CHL group performed significantly below the CNH group on all measures: picture naming, defining words, nonword repetition, and repetition of sentences. The proportion of words pronounced with correct phonological structure when picture naming was more limited in the CHL group than in the CNH group. There was a significant positive correlation between the amount of hearing aid use and nonword repetition ability in the CHL group. Age of diagnosis and age of hearing aid fitting was not significantly correlated with the outcomes of the vocabulary assessments in this study. CONCLUSION: Despite the technological advancement of hearing aids, the expressive vocabulary in school-aged children with mild to moderately severe, permanent bilateral, hearing impairment does not reach the same level as for children with normal hearing, although there is a variation in performance within the group. The variation in the CHL group was not uniquely impacted by either age, degree of hearing loss, or the age of diagnosis. The amount of hearing aid use seems to impact the perception of new words. More studies of expressive vocabulary are needed, because they capture a dimension of word learning that seems particularly sensitive to hearing loss and hearing aid use.


Sujet(s)
Surdité , Aides auditives , Enfant , Études transversales , Surdité bilatérale partielle/diagnostic , Humains , Tests du langage , Vocabulaire
2.
ERJ Open Res ; 7(2)2021 Apr.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34109239

RÉSUMÉ

OBJECTIVES: This cross-sectional study aimed to investigate the prevalence of self-reported and clinically screened swallowing dysfunction (dysphagia) in COPD patients with severe exacerbations and to identify any associated factors. Findings were then compared to a control group. METHODS: Participants included 30 patients hospitalised due to a COPD exacerbation. The control group consisted of 30 adults hospitalised with acute cardiac symptoms. Data were derived from spirometry, the 150 mL timed water swallow test, a cookie swallow test and a dyspnoea questionnaire (modified Medical Research Council (mMRC)). Scores from the 10-item Eating Assessment Tool (EAT-10) were calculated to assess patient perception of swallowing dysfunction. RESULTS: Self-reported swallowing dysfunction and clinical signs thereof were more common in COPD patients than in the control group (67% versus 23% and 80% versus 37%, respectively; p≤0.001). Clinical signs of swallowing dysfunction in the group with acute exacerbation of COPD were associated with self-reported swallowing dysfunction (p=0.02) and xerostomia (p=0.04). Dyspnoea (mMRC ≥2) was more common among the COPD patients (90% versus 47%, p<0.001). There was a significant negative correlation between lung function and self-reported dysphagia (r=-0.39, p=0.03), but not between lung function and clinically screened dysphagia (r=-0.23, p=0.21). CONCLUSION: COPD patients hospitalised with an acute exacerbation experienced significantly more self-reported and clinically screened swallowing dysfunction compared to a control group of patients with cardiac symptoms. Both patient groups experienced dyspnoea, but it was twice as common in the group with acute exacerbation of COPD. Both groups also experienced xerostomia.

4.
Augment Altern Commun ; 34(1): 16-29, 2018 03.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29353508

RÉSUMÉ

Vocabulary learning reflects the language experiences of the child, both in typical and atypical development, although the vocabulary development of children who use aided communication may differ from children who use natural speech. This study compared the performance of children using aided communication with that of peers using natural speech on two measures of vocabulary knowledge: comprehension of graphic symbols and labeling of common objects. There were 92 participants not considered intellectually disabled in the aided group. The reference group consisted of 60 participants without known disorders. The comprehension task consisted of 63 items presented individually in each participant's graphic system, together with four colored line drawings. Participants were required to indicate which drawing corresponded to the symbol. In the expressive labelling task, 20 common objects presented in drawings had to be named. Both groups indicated the correct drawing for most of the items in the comprehension tasks, with a small advantage for the reference group. The reference group named most objects quickly and accurately, demonstrating that the objects were common and easily named. The aided language group named the majority correctly and in addition used a variety of naming strategies; they required more time than the reference group. The results give insights into lexical processing in aided communication and may have implications for aided language intervention.


Sujet(s)
Aides à la communication pour personnes handicapées , Troubles de la communication/rééducation et réadaptation , Compréhension , Vocabulaire , Adolescent , Études cas-témoins , Enfant , Enfant d'âge préscolaire , Femelle , Humains , Mâle
5.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28176891

RÉSUMÉ

BACKGROUND: COPD is a common problem associated with morbidity and mortality. COPD may also affect the dynamics and coordination of functions such as swallowing. A misdirected swallow may, in turn, result in the bolus entering the airway. A growing body of evidence suggests that a subgroup of people with COPD is prone to oropharyngeal dysphagia. The aim of this study was to evaluate swallowing dysfunction in patients with stable COPD and to determine the relation between signs and symptoms of swallowing dysfunction and lung function (forced expiratory volume in 1 second percent predicted). METHODS: Fifty-one patients with COPD in a stable phase participated in a questionnaire survey, swallowing tests, and spirometry. A post-bronchodilator ratio of the forced expiratory volume in 1 second/best of forced vital capacity and vital capacity <0.7 was used to define COPD. Swallowing function was assessed by a questionnaire and two swallowing tests (water and cookie swallow tests). RESULTS: Sixty-five percent of the patients reported subjective signs and symptoms of swallowing dysfunction in the questionnaire and 49% showed measurable ones in the swallowing tests. For the combined subjective and objective findings, 78% had a coexisting swallowing dysfunction. No significant difference was found between male and female patients. CONCLUSION: Swallowing function is affected in COPD patients with moderate to severe airflow limitation, and the signs and symptoms of this swallowing dysfunction were subjective, objective, or both.


Sujet(s)
Troubles de la déglutition/épidémiologie , Déglutition , Poumon/physiopathologie , Broncho-pneumopathie chronique obstructive/épidémiologie , Sujet âgé , Sujet âgé de 80 ans ou plus , Bronchodilatateurs/usage thérapeutique , Troubles de la déglutition/diagnostic , Troubles de la déglutition/physiopathologie , Femelle , Volume expiratoire maximal par seconde , Enquêtes de santé , Humains , Poumon/effets des médicaments et des substances chimiques , Mâle , Adulte d'âge moyen , Prévalence , Broncho-pneumopathie chronique obstructive/diagnostic , Broncho-pneumopathie chronique obstructive/traitement médicamenteux , Broncho-pneumopathie chronique obstructive/physiopathologie , Indice de gravité de la maladie , Spirométrie , Suède/épidémiologie , Capacité vitale
6.
Augment Altern Commun ; 31(2): 97-107, 2015 Jun.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25916273

RÉSUMÉ

Blissymbolics as a graphic symbol system has the potential to represent a large number of vocabulary items using a small number of basic Bliss-characters. The aim of this project was to investigate how children with typical development, aged 3 years to 7 years 11 months, interpreted Bliss-characters and compound Bliss-words and then constructed their own Bliss-words. Children participated in a teaching procedure that included explanations and feedback on the structure and meaning of Bliss-characters. Their spontaneous interpretations of Bliss-characters and their ability to construct new Bliss-words were explored. Results suggest that Bliss-characters, although not very transparent, evoked spontaneous linguistic activities and were retained after brief explanations. Children aged 5 years and older appeared to discover the logic of the structure of Bliss-words. Children of all ages used Bliss-characters to represent new ideas. Some chose Bliss-characters resembling adult representations of concepts, others chose Bliss-characters representing personal associations. In sum, children retained many of the Bliss-characters after a relatively brief exposure and demonstrated semantic creativity in interpretation and construction of Bliss-words.


Sujet(s)
Aides à la communication pour personnes handicapées , Communication , Développement du langage oral , Enfant , Développement de l'enfant , Enfant d'âge préscolaire , Femelle , Humains , Mâle , Vocabulaire
7.
Res Dev Disabil ; 34(11): 3924-35, 2013 Nov.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24021394

RÉSUMÉ

It has been proposed that an impairment of procedural memory underlies a range of linguistic, cognitive and motor impairments observed in developmental dyslexia (DD). However, studies designed to test this hypothesis using the implicit sequence learning paradigm have yielded inconsistent results. A fundamental aspect of procedural learning is that it takes place over an extended time-period that may be divided into distinct stages based on both behavioural characteristics and neural correlates of performance. Yet, no study of implicit sequence learning in children with DD has included learning stages beyond a single practice session. The present study was designed to fill this important gap by extending the investigation to include the effects of overnight consolidation as well as those of further practice on a subsequent day. The results suggest that the most pronounced procedural learning impairment in DD may emerge only after extended practice, in learning stages beyond a single practice session.


Sujet(s)
Dyslexie/physiopathologie , Mémoire/physiologie , Apprentissage sériel/physiologie , Études cas-témoins , Enfant , Dyslexie/psychologie , Femelle , Humains , Mâle
8.
PLoS One ; 8(5): e63998, 2013.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23717524

RÉSUMÉ

Developmental dyslexia (DD) has previously been associated with a number of cognitive deficits. Little attention has been directed to cognitive functions that remain intact in the disorder, though the investigation and identification of such strengths might be useful for developing new, and improving current, therapeutical interventions. In this study, an old/new recognition memory paradigm was used to examine previously untested aspects of declarative memory in children with DD and typically developing control children. The DD group was not only not impaired at the task, but actually showed superior recognition memory, as compared to the control children. These findings complement previous reports of enhanced cognition in other domains (e.g., visuo-spatial processing) in DD. Possible underlying mechanisms for the observed DD advantage in declarative memory, and the possibility of compensation by this system for reading deficits in dyslexia, are discussed.


Sujet(s)
Dyslexie/psychologie , Enfant , Femelle , Humains , Intelligence , Mâle , Rappel mnésique , Temps de réaction , Lecture ,
9.
Res Dev Disabil ; 32(6): 2362-75, 2011.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21840165

RÉSUMÉ

The Procedural Deficit Hypothesis (PDH) posits that Specific Language Impairment (SLI) can be largely explained by abnormalities of brain structures that subserve procedural memory. The PDH predicts impairments of procedural memory itself, and that such impairments underlie the grammatical deficits observed in the disorder. Previous studies have indeed reported procedural learning impairments in SLI, and have found that these are associated with grammatical difficulties. The present study extends this research by examining consolidation and longer-term procedural sequence learning in children with SLI. The Alternating Serial Reaction Time (ASRT) task was given to children with SLI and typically developing (TD) children in an initial learning session and an average of three days later to test for consolidation and longer-term learning. Although both groups showed evidence of initial sequence learning, only the TD children showed clear signs of consolidation, even though the two groups did not differ in longer-term learning. When the children were re-categorized on the basis of grammar deficits rather than broader language deficits, a clearer pattern emerged. Whereas both the grammar impaired and normal grammar groups showed evidence of initial sequence learning, only those with normal grammar showed consolidation and longer-term learning. Indeed, the grammar-impaired group appeared to lose any sequence knowledge gained during the initial testing session. These findings held even when controlling for vocabulary or a broad non-grammatical language measure, neither of which were associated with procedural memory. When grammar was examined as a continuous variable over all children, the same relationships between procedural memory and grammar, but not vocabulary or the broader language measure, were observed. Overall, the findings support and further specify the PDH. They suggest that consolidation and longer-term procedural learning are impaired in SLI, but that these impairments are specifically tied to the grammatical deficits in the disorder. The possibility that consolidation and longer-term learning are problematic in the disorder suggests a locus of potential study for therapeutic approaches. In sum, this study clarifies our understanding of the underlying deficits in SLI, and suggests avenues for further research.


Sujet(s)
Troubles du développement du langage/diagnostic , Linguistique , Troubles de la mémoire/diagnostic , Mémoire/physiologie , Enfant , Femelle , Humains , Troubles du développement du langage/physiopathologie , Tests du langage , Mâle , Troubles de la mémoire/physiopathologie , Valeur prédictive des tests , Différenciation sémantique , Apprentissage verbal/physiologie , Vocabulaire
10.
Epilepsy Behav ; 18(3): 267-75, 2010 Jul.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20493782

RÉSUMÉ

We reviewed the medical history, EEG recordings, and developmental milestones of 19 children with speech and language dysfunction and focal epileptiform activity. Speech, language, and neuropsychological assessments and EEG recordings were performed at follow-up, and prognostic indicators were analyzed. Three patterns of language development were observed: late start and slow development, late start and deterioration/regression, and normal start and later regression/deterioration. No differences in test results among these groups were seen, indicating a spectrum of related conditions including Landau-Kleffner syndrome and epileptic language disorder. More than half of the participants had speech and language dysfunction at follow-up. IQ levels, working memory, and processing speed were also affected. Dysfunction of auditory perception in noise was found in more than half of the participants, and dysfunction of auditory attention in all. Dysfunction of communication, oral motor ability, and stuttering were noted in a few. Family history of seizures and abundant epileptiform activity indicated a worse prognosis.


Sujet(s)
Troubles de la cognition/étiologie , Épilepsies partielles/complications , Développement du langage oral , Troubles du langage/étiologie , Parole/physiologie , Stimulation acoustique/méthodes , Adolescent , Adulte , Enfant , Enfant d'âge préscolaire , Communication , Électroencéphalographie/méthodes , Femelle , Études de suivi , Humains , Tests du langage , Mâle , Tests neuropsychologiques , Enquêtes et questionnaires , Jeune adulte
11.
J Pediatr Rehabil Med ; 3(4): 251-8, 2010.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21791859

RÉSUMÉ

OBJECTIVE: The purpose of this study was to focus on two linguistic abilities, word retrieval (expressive language) and comprehension of vocabulary and grammar (receptive language), as well as to investigate to what extent long-term problems exist in these areas in children following traumatic brain injury. METHODS: Two groups of children were studied retrospectively: twenty-four children with traumatic brain injury (TBI) and twenty-one children diagnosed with brain tumour. All children had been referred to the rehabilitation team for assessment. The children were between four and seventeen years old when assessed, with the assessments performed at least one year after the injury or brain tumour diagnosis. An established set of tests regarding word retrieval and comprehension of vocabulary and grammar was used, and the results were compared with normative test data. RESULTS: In both clinical groups, significantly more children scored lower than the designated "normal" score than in the normative sample on tests measuring confrontation naming and phonological word retrieval. In addition, in the brain tumour group, more children demonstrated significantly lower results than normal performance on a test for semantic word retrieval. In the TBI group, significantly more children scored below the normal value on tests of word and grammatical comprehension when compared to the normative sample. CONCLUSIONS: This study confirms that word retrieval is an area of deficit in many children with acquired brain injuries one year or more after the injury occurred. The study also indicates that children with TBI may have persistent deficits in comprehension of both vocabulary and grammar.

12.
Ups J Med Sci ; 114(2): 82-9, 2009.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19396694

RÉSUMÉ

In a previous study we reported difficulty with expressive language and visuoperceptual ability in preschool children with epilepsy and otherwise normal development. The present study analysed speech and language dysfunction for each individual in relation to epilepsy variables, ear preference, and intelligence in these children and described their auditory function. Twenty 6-year-old children with epilepsy (14 females, 6 males; mean age 6:5 y, range 6 y-6 y 11 mo) and 30 reference children without epilepsy (18 females, 12 males; mean age 6:5 y, range 6 y-6 y 11 mo) were assessed for language and auditory ability. Low scores for the children with epilepsy were analysed with respect to speech-language domains, type of epilepsy, site of epileptiform activity, intelligence, and language laterality. Auditory attention, perception, discrimination, and ear preference were measured with a dichotic listening test, and group comparisons were performed. Children with left-sided partial epilepsy had extensive language dysfunction. Most children with partial epilepsy had phonological dysfunction. Language dysfunction was also found in children with generalized and unclassified epilepsies. The children with epilepsy performed significantly worse than the reference children in auditory attention, perception of vowels and discrimination of consonants for the right ear and had more left ear advantage for vowels, indicating undeveloped language laterality.


Sujet(s)
Épilepsie/complications , Troubles de l'audition/complications , Langage , Enfant , Études de cohortes , Épilepsie/physiopathologie , Femelle , Troubles de l'audition/physiopathologie , Humains , Mâle , Activité motrice
13.
Acta Paediatr ; 95(12): 1586-93, 2006 Dec.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17129967

RÉSUMÉ

UNLABELLED: School level at age 10 was studied in two cohorts of children who had required neonatal intensive care (NIC): cohort 1, children born 1980-1985 (n=310); and cohort 2, children born 1986-1989 (n=245); and two control groups. More than 80% of all NIC children of both cohorts attended the appropriate mainstream grade 3 or 4; 12.9% of cohort 1 and 6.8% of cohort 2 were in mainstream grade 2. Six per cent of both cohorts received special education. Among very preterm children (23-31 gestational weeks), 73.5% of cohort 1 and 80.3% of cohort 2 attended grades 3 and 4, while 22.9% and 12.1%, respectively, were in grade 2. Assistance (remedial teaching, personal assistant or special teaching group) was given to 42.4% of cohort 1 in the mainstream (grades 2, 3 and 4) and to 38.2% of cohort 2 in the mainstream. In cohort 2, more very preterm girls than matched controls received assistance (p<0.05); no corresponding difference was found in very preterm boys. Most children with congenital malformations received assistance in mainstream education or received special education. CONCLUSION: Most NIC children are in mainstream school classes at age 10. Twelve to 23% of very preterm children are 1 y behind. Many NIC children in the school mainstream need assistance at school, but the proportion of children in mainstream education increases markedly with time.


Sujet(s)
Niveau d'instruction , Unités de soins intensifs néonatals , /statistiques et données numériques , Enfant , Études de cohortes , Femelle , Âge gestationnel , Humains , Nouveau-né , Mâle , Facteurs sexuels , Suède
14.
Acta Paediatr ; 94(4): 471-8, 2005 Apr.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16092463

RÉSUMÉ

AIM: To explore possible patterns of comorbidity in children with severe developmental language disorders (DLD). METHODS: A retrospective investigation of the clinical records of 28 children relating to oral motor and language problems, psychological profiles, medical history and EEG findings. RESULTS: 36% of all the children had pure expressive language problems, 64% had combined expressive-receptive language problems and 57% had additional oral motor problems. Girls predominated in expressive-receptive problems, while boys predominated in oral motor problems. Children with expressive-receptive disorders were over-represented at the lower end of normal full-scale IQ (p = 0.015). Lower verbal than non-verbal IQ levels were almost as common as equal levels, but a lower non-verbal IQ than verbal IQ was also found. Pre/perinatal problems were found in 21%, and heredity for developmental language problems or dyslexia in 39%. There was a higher proportion of attention and motor problems, EEG abnormalities, and epileptic syndromes than in the general population (p < 0.001). CONCLUSION: In severe DLD, both pure expressive and mixed expressive-receptive problems are found, and oral motor problems are common. Gender differences regarding symptoms are present. Receptive language ability is associated with the full-scale IQ level. A high proportion of EEG abnormalities and epilepsy is found. There is a complex pattern of comorbidity between speech-language symptoms, psychological characteristics, heredity and EEG findings.


Sujet(s)
Troubles de la prononciation et de l'articulation/complications , Intelligence , Troubles du développement du langage/étiologie , Adolescent , Enfant , Enfant d'âge préscolaire , Dyslexie/complications , Électroencéphalographie , Épilepsie/complications , Femelle , Humains , Troubles du développement du langage/génétique , Troubles du développement du langage/psychologie , Mâle , Facteurs sexuels
SÉLECTION CITATIONS
DÉTAIL DE RECHERCHE
...