Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Montrer: 20 | 50 | 100
Résultats 1 - 12 de 12
Filtrer
Plus de filtres










Base de données
Gamme d'année
2.
Neuroinformatics ; 20(1): 25-36, 2022 01.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33506383

RÉSUMÉ

There is great need for coordination around standards and best practices in neuroscience to support efforts to make neuroscience a data-centric discipline. Major brain initiatives launched around the world are poised to generate huge stores of neuroscience data. At the same time, neuroscience, like many domains in biomedicine, is confronting the issues of transparency, rigor, and reproducibility. Widely used, validated standards and best practices are key to addressing the challenges in both big and small data science, as they are essential for integrating diverse data and for developing a robust, effective, and sustainable infrastructure to support open and reproducible neuroscience. However, developing community standards and gaining their adoption is difficult. The current landscape is characterized both by a lack of robust, validated standards and a plethora of overlapping, underdeveloped, untested and underutilized standards and best practices. The International Neuroinformatics Coordinating Facility (INCF), an independent organization dedicated to promoting data sharing through the coordination of infrastructure and standards, has recently implemented a formal procedure for evaluating and endorsing community standards and best practices in support of the FAIR principles. By formally serving as a standards organization dedicated to open and FAIR neuroscience, INCF helps evaluate, promulgate, and coordinate standards and best practices across neuroscience. Here, we provide an overview of the process and discuss how neuroscience can benefit from having a dedicated standards body.


Sujet(s)
Neurosciences , Reproductibilité des résultats
3.
PLoS One ; 8(2): e54919, 2013.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23390506

RÉSUMÉ

Hemianopia patients have lost vision from the contralateral hemifield, but make behavioural adjustments to compensate for this field loss. As a result, their visual performance and behaviour contrast with those of hemineglect patients who fail to attend to objects contralateral to their lesion. These conditions differ in their ocular fixations and perceptual judgments. During visual search, hemianopic patients make more fixations in contralesional space while hemineglect patients make fewer. During line bisection, hemianopic patients fixate the contralesional line segment more and make a small contralesional bisection error, while hemineglect patients make few contralesional fixations and a larger ipsilesional bisection error. Hence, there is an attentional failure for contralesional space in hemineglect but a compensatory adaptation to attend more to the blind side in hemianopia. A challenge for models of visual attentional processes is to show how compensation is achieved in hemianopia, and why such processes are hindered or inaccessible in hemineglect. We used a neurophysiology-derived computational model to examine possible cortical compensatory processes in simulated hemianopia from a V1 lesion and compared results with those obtained with the same processes under conditions of simulated hemineglect from a parietal lesion. A spatial compensatory bias to increase attention contralesionally replicated hemianopic scanning patterns during visual search but not during line bisection. To reproduce the latter required a second process, an extrastriate lateral connectivity facilitating form completion into the blind field: this allowed accurate placement of fixations on contralesional stimuli and reproduced fixation patterns and the contralesional bisection error of hemianopia. Neither of these two cortical compensatory processes was effective in ameliorating the ipsilesional bias in the hemineglect model. Our results replicate normal and pathological patterns of visual scanning, line bisection, and differences between hemianopia and hemineglect, and may explain why compensatory processes that counter the effects of hemianopia are ineffective in hemineglect.


Sujet(s)
Adaptation physiologique , Hémianopsie/physiopathologie , Modèles neurologiques , Troubles de la perception/physiopathologie , Cortex visuel/physiopathologie , Perception visuelle , Attention/physiologie , Simulation numérique , Latéralité fonctionnelle , Hémianopsie/anatomopathologie , Humains , Tests neuropsychologiques , Troubles de la perception/anatomopathologie , Performance psychomotrice , Vision , Cortex visuel/anatomopathologie , Champs visuels/physiologie
4.
PLoS One ; 7(4): e33460, 2012.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22493669

RÉSUMÉ

BACKGROUND: There are few clinical tools that assess decision-making under risk. Tests that characterize sensitivity and bias in decisions between prospects varying in magnitude and probability of gain may provide insights in conditions with anomalous reward-related behaviour. OBJECTIVE: We designed a simple test of how subjects integrate information about the magnitude and the probability of reward, which can determine discriminative thresholds and choice bias in decisions under risk. DESIGN/METHODS: Twenty subjects were required to choose between two explicitly described prospects, one with higher probability but lower magnitude of reward than the other, with the difference in expected value between the two prospects varying from 3 to 23%. RESULTS: Subjects showed a mean threshold sensitivity of 43% difference in expected value. Regarding choice bias, there was a 'risk premium' of 38%, indicating a tendency to choose higher probability over higher reward. An analysis using prospect theory showed that this risk premium is the predicted outcome of hypothesized non-linearities in the subjective perception of reward value and probability. CONCLUSIONS: This simple test provides a robust measure of discriminative value thresholds and biases in decisions under risk. Prospect theory can also make predictions about decisions when subjective perception of reward or probability is anomalous, as may occur in populations with dopaminergic or striatal dysfunction, such as Parkinson's disease and schizophrenia.


Sujet(s)
Comportement de choix/physiologie , Prise de décision/physiologie , Perception/physiologie , Probabilité , Adulte , Femelle , Humains , Mâle , Adulte d'âge moyen , Modèles psychologiques , Récompense , Risque , Sensibilité et spécificité
5.
Neuropsychologia ; 50(6): 1190-201, 2012 May.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22118912

RÉSUMÉ

Whether an attentional gradient favouring the ipsilesional side is responsible for the line bisection errors in visual neglect is uncertain. We explored this by using a conjunction-search task on the right side of a computer screen to bias attention while healthy subjects performed line bisection. The first experiment used a probe detection task to confirm that the conjunction-search task created a rightward attentional gradient, as manifest in response times, detection rates, and fixation patterns. In the second experiment subjects performed line bisection with or without a simultaneous conjunction-search task. Fixation patterns in the latter condition were biased rightwards as in visual neglect, and bisection also showed a rightward bias, though modest. A third experiment using the probe detection task again showed that the attentional gradient induced by the conjunction-search task was reduced when subjects also performed line bisection, perhaps explaining the modest effects on bisection bias. Finally, an experiment with briefly viewed pre-bisected lines produced similar results, showing that the small size of the bisection bias was not due to an unlimited view allowing deployment of attentional resources to counteract the conjunction-search task's attentional gradient. These results show that an attentional gradient induced in healthy subjects can produce visual neglect-like visual scanning and a rightward shift of perceived line midpoint, but the modest size of this shift points to limitations of this physiological model in simulating the pathologic effects of visual neglect.


Sujet(s)
Attention , Latéralité fonctionnelle/physiologie , Troubles de la perception/physiopathologie , Performance psychomotrice/physiologie , Perception visuelle/physiologie , Adolescent , Adulte , Femelle , Fixation oculaire , Humains , Mâle , Modèles biologiques , Stimulation lumineuse/méthodes , Temps de réaction/physiologie , Champs visuels
6.
PLoS One ; 5(6): e11128, 2010 Jun 15.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20559559

RÉSUMÉ

BACKGROUND: Visual neglect is an attentional deficit typically resulting from parietal cortex lesion and sometimes frontal lesion. Patients fail to attend to objects and events in the visual hemifield contralateral to their lesion during visual search. METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDING: The aim of this work was to examine the effects of parietal and frontal lesion in an existing computational model of visual attention and search and simulate visual search behaviour under lesion conditions. We find that unilateral parietal lesion in this model leads to symptoms of visual neglect in simulated search scan paths, including an inhibition of return (IOR) deficit, while frontal lesion leads to milder neglect and to more severe deficits in IOR and perseveration in the scan path. During simulations of search under unilateral parietal lesion, the model's extrastriate ventral stream area exhibits lower activity for stimuli in the neglected hemifield compared to that for stimuli in the normally perceived hemifield. This could represent a computational correlate of differences observed in neuroimaging for unconscious versus conscious perception following parietal lesion. CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE: Our results lead to the prediction, supported by effective connectivity evidence, that connections between the dorsal and ventral visual streams may be an important factor in the explanation of perceptual deficits in parietal lesion patients and of conscious perception in general.


Sujet(s)
Attention , Vision , Lobe frontal/physiopathologie , Humains , Lobe pariétal/physiopathologie , Stimulation lumineuse
7.
Mol Genet Metab ; 99(3): 291-5, 2010 Mar.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19939718

RÉSUMÉ

New treatment options for Niemann-Pick Type C (NPC) have recently become available. To assess the efficiency and efficacy of these new treatment markers for disease status and progression are needed. Both the diagnosis and the monitoring of disease progression are challenging and mostly rely on clinical impression and functional testing of horizontal eye movements. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) provides information about the microintegrity especially of white matter. We show here in a case report how DTI and measures derived from this imaging method can serve as adjunct quantitative markers for disease management in Niemann-Pick Type C. Two approaches are taken--first, we compare the fractional anisotropy (FA) in the white matter globally between a 29-year-old NPC patient and 18 healthy age-matched controls and show the remarkable difference in FA relatively early in the course of the disease. Second, a voxelwise comparison of FA values reveals where white matter integrity is compromised locally and demonstrate an individualized analysis of FA changes before and after 1year of treatment with Miglustat. This method might be useful in future treatment trials for NPC to assess treatment effects.


Sujet(s)
1-Désoxynojirimycine/analogues et dérivés , Imagerie par tenseur de diffusion/méthodes , Antienzymes , Mouvements oculaires/effets des médicaments et des substances chimiques , Maladie de Niemann-Pick de type C/traitement médicamenteux , 1-Désoxynojirimycine/administration et posologie , 1-Désoxynojirimycine/usage thérapeutique , Adulte , Anisotropie , Antienzymes/administration et posologie , Antienzymes/usage thérapeutique , Mouvements oculaires/physiologie , Humains , Mâle , Résultat thérapeutique , Jeune adulte
8.
J Neuroophthalmol ; 29(2): 96-103, 2009 Jun.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19491631

RÉSUMÉ

BACKGROUND: Motion perception may be preserved after damage to striate cortex (primary visual cortex, area V1). Awareness and normal discrimination of fast-moving stimuli have been observed even in the complete absence of V1. These facts suggest that motion-sensitive cortex (the V5/MT complex or V5/MT+) may be activated by direct thalamic or collicular inputs that bypass V1. Such projections have been identified previously in monkeys but have not been shown in humans using neuroimaging techniques. METHODS: We used diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) tractography to visualize white matter fiber tracts connecting with V5/MT+ in 10 healthy volunteers. V5/MT+ was localized for each subject using functional MRI (fMRI). Functional activity maps were overlaid on high-resolution anatomical images and registered with the diffusion-weighted images to define V5/MT+ as the region of interest (ROI) for DTI tractography analysis. Fibers connecting to V1 were excluded from the analysis. RESULTS: Using conservative tractography parameters, we found connections between the V5/MT+ region and the posterior thalamus and/or superior colliculus in 4 of 10 subjects. CONCLUSIONS: Connections between the V5/MT+ region and the posterior thalamus and/or superior colliculus may explain visual motion awareness in the absence of a functioning V1.


Sujet(s)
Perception du mouvement/physiologie , Cortex visuel/vascularisation , Cortex visuel/physiologie , Voies optiques/vascularisation , Voies optiques/physiologie , Adulte , Cartographie cérébrale , Imagerie par résonance magnétique de diffusion , Femelle , Humains , Traitement d'image par ordinateur/méthodes , Imagerie par résonance magnétique , Mâle , Oxygène/sang , Stimulation lumineuse , Colliculus supérieurs/vascularisation , Colliculus supérieurs/physiologie , Thalamus/vascularisation , Thalamus/physiologie , Jeune adulte
9.
Cogn Neurodyn ; 3(3): 223-42, 2009 Sep.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19125356

RÉSUMÉ

Single cell recordings in monkey inferior temporal cortex (IT) and area V4 during visual search tasks indicate that modulation of responses by the search target object occurs in the late portion of the cell's sensory response (Chelazzi et al. in J Neurophysiol 80:2918-2940, 1998; Cereb Cortex 11:761-772, 2001) whereas attention to a spatial location influences earlier responses (Luck et al. in J Neurophysiol 77:24-42, 1997). Previous computational models have not captured differences in the latency of these attentional effects and yet the more protracted development of the object-based effect could have implications for behaviour. We present a neurodynamic biased competition model of visual attention in which we aimed to model the timecourse of spatial and object-based attention in order to simulate cellular responses and saccade onset times observed in monkey recordings. In common with other models, a top-down prefrontal signal, related to the search target, biases activity in the ventral visual stream. However, we conclude that this bias signal is more complex than modelled elsewhere: the latency of object-based effects in V4 and IT, and saccade onset, can be accurately simulated when the target object feedback bias consists of a sensory response component in addition to a mnemonic response. These attentional effects in V4 and IT cellular responses lead to a system that is able to produce search scan paths similar to those observed in monkeys and humans, with attention being guided to locations containing behaviourally relevant stimuli. This work demonstrates that accurate modelling of the timecourse of single cell responses can lead to biologically realistic behaviours being demonstrated by the system as a whole.

10.
J Vis ; 8(8): 2.1-9, 2008 Jun 02.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18831625

RÉSUMÉ

Perceptual studies suggest that processing facial identity emphasizes upper-face information, whereas processing expressions of anger or happiness emphasizes the lower-face. The two goals of the present study were to determine (a) if the distributions of eye fixations reflect these upper/lower-face biases, and (b) whether this bias is task- or stimulus-driven. We presented a target face followed by a probe pair of morphed faces, neither of which was identical to the target. Subjects judged which of the pair was more similar to the target face while eye movements were recorded. In Experiment 1 the probe pair always differed from each other in both identity and expression on each trial. In one block subjects judged which probe face was more similar to the target face in identity, and in a second block subjects judged which probe face was more similar to the target face in expression. In Experiment 2 the two probe faces differed in either expression or identity, but not both. Subjects were not informed which dimension differed, but simply asked to judge which probe face was more similar to the target face. We found that subjects scanned the upper-face more than the lower-face during the identity task but the lower-face more than the upper-face during the expression task in Experiment 1 (task-driven effects), with significantly less variation in bias in Experiment 2 (stimulus-driven effects). We conclude that fixations correlate with regional variations of diagnostic information in different processing tasks, but that these reflect top-down task-driven guidance of information acquisition more than stimulus-driven effects.


Sujet(s)
Face , Expression faciale , Processus mentaux/physiologie , /physiologie , Adulte , Émotions , Femelle , Fixation oculaire , Humains , Jugement , Mâle , Stimulation lumineuse , Pensée (activité mentale)/physiologie
11.
Hippocampus ; 18(4): 335-9, 2008.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18172895

RÉSUMÉ

Individuals vary widely in their ability to orient within the environment. We used diffusion tensor imaging to investigate whether this ability, as measured by navigational performance in a virtual environment, correlates with the anatomic structural properties of the hippocampus, i.e., fractional anisotropy. We found that individuals with high fractional anisotropy in the right hippocampus are (a) faster in forming a cognitive map of the environment, and (b) more efficient in using this map for the purpose of orientation, than individuals with low fractional anisotropy. These results are consistent with the role of the hippocampus in navigation, and suggest that its microstructural properties may contribute to the intersubject variability observed in spatial orientation.


Sujet(s)
Cognition/physiologie , Hippocampe/anatomie et histologie , Hippocampe/physiologie , Orientation/physiologie , Performance psychomotrice/physiologie , Perception de l'espace/physiologie , Adulte , Facteurs âges , Anisotropie , Cartographie cérébrale/méthodes , Imagerie par résonance magnétique de diffusion/méthodes , Femelle , Latéralité fonctionnelle/physiologie , Humains , Mâle , Neurofibres myélinisées/physiologie , Neurofibres myélinisées/ultrastructure , Voies nerveuses/anatomie et histologie , Voies nerveuses/physiologie , Biais de l'observateur , Caractères sexuels
12.
Neural Netw ; 17(5-6): 873-97, 2004.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15288904

RÉSUMÉ

When a monkey searches for a colour and orientation feature conjunction target, the scan path is guided to target coloured locations in preference to locations containing the target orientation [Vision Res. 38 (1998b) 1805]. An active vision model, using biased competition, is able to replicate this behaviour. As object-based attention develops in extrastriate cortex, featural information is passed to posterior parietal cortex (LIP), enabling it to represent behaviourally relevant locations [J. Neurophysiol. 76 (1996) 2841] and guide the scan path. Attention evolves from an early spatial effect to being object-based later in the response of the model neurons, as has been observed in monkey single cell recordings. This is the first model to reproduce these effects with temporal precision and is reported here at the systems level allowing the replication of psychophysical scan paths.


Sujet(s)
Comportement appétitif/physiologie , Attention/physiologie , , Vision/physiologie , Voies optiques/physiologie , Perception visuelle/physiologie , Animaux , Rétroaction biologique (psychologie)/physiologie , Apprentissage discriminatif , Fixation oculaire/physiologie , Haplorhini , Humains , Traitement d'image par ordinateur , Inhibition nerveuse/physiologie , Neurones/physiologie , Stimulation lumineuse/méthodes , Performance psychomotrice/physiologie , Psychophysique , Temps de réaction , Rétine/physiologie , Saccades/physiologie , Facteurs temps , Voies optiques/cytologie
SÉLECTION CITATIONS
DÉTAIL DE RECHERCHE