Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Montrer: 20 | 50 | 100
Résultats 1 - 16 de 16
Filtrer
Plus de filtres











Base de données
Gamme d'année
1.
J Comp Psychol ; 137(3): 191-199, 2023 08.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37261734

RÉSUMÉ

Chunking is an important cognitive process allowing the compression of information in short-term memory. The aim of this study is to compare the dynamics of chunking during the learning of a visuomotor sequence in humans (Homo sapiens) and Guinea baboons (Papio papio). We duplicated in humans an experimental paradigm that has been used previously in baboons. On each trial, human participants had to point to a moving target on a touch screen. The experiment involved the repetition of the same sequence of nine items over a 1,000 trials. To reproduce as much as possible the conditions under which baboons performed the task, human participants were tested at their own pace. Results revealed that baboons and humans shared similar chunking dynamics: In both species, the sequence was initially parsed into small chunks that became longer and fewer with practice through two reorganization mechanisms (recombinations and concatenations). Differences were also observed regarding the global decrease in response times that was faster and more pronounced in humans compared with baboons. Analyses of these similarities and differences provide new empirical evidence for understanding the general properties of chunking mechanisms in sequence learning and its evolution across species. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Sujet(s)
Papio papio , Animaux , Humains , Papio papio/psychologie , Papio , Apprentissage , Mémoire à court terme , Temps de réaction
2.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 377(1843): 20200310, 2022 01 31.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34894743

RÉSUMÉ

Conventions form an essential part of human social and cultural behaviour and may also be important to other animal societies. Yet, despite the wealth of evidence that has accumulated for culture in non-human animals, we know surprisingly little about non-human conventions beyond a few rare examples. We follow the literature in behavioural ecology and evolution and define conventions as systematic behaviours that solve a coordination problem in which two or more individuals need to display complementary behaviour to obtain a mutually beneficial outcome. We start by discussing the literature on conventions in non-human primates from this perspective and conclude that all the ingredients for conventions to emerge are present and therefore that they ought to be more frequently observed. We then probe the emergence of conventions by using a unique novel experimental system in which pairs of Guinea baboons (Papio papio) can voluntarily participate together in touchscreen-based cognitive testing and we show that conventions readily emerge in our experimental set-up and that they share three fundamental properties of human conventions (arbitrariness, stability and efficiency). These results question the idea that observational learning, and imitation in particular, is necessary to establish conventions; they suggest that positive reinforcement is enough. This article is part of a discussion meeting issue 'The emergence of collective knowledge and cumulative culture in animals, humans and machines'.


Sujet(s)
Papio papio , Animaux , Langage , Apprentissage , Papio papio/psychologie , Primates , Comportement social
3.
Proc Biol Sci ; 286(1904): 20190729, 2019 06 12.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31161908

RÉSUMÉ

The unique cumulative nature of human culture has often been explained by high-fidelity copying mechanisms found only in human social learning. However, transmission chain experiments in human and non-human primates suggest that cumulative cultural evolution (CCE) might not necessarily depend on high-fidelity copying after all. In this study, we test whether defining properties of CCE can emerge in a non-copying task. We performed transmission chain experiments in Guinea baboons and human children where individuals observed and produced visual patterns composed of four squares on touchscreen devices. In order to be rewarded, participants had to avoid touching squares that were touched by a previous participant. In other words, they were rewarded for innovation rather than copying. Results nevertheless exhibited fundamental properties of CCE: an increase over generations in task performance and the emergence of systematic structure. However, these properties arose from different mechanisms across species: children, unlike baboons, converged in behaviour over generations by copying specific patterns in a different location, thus introducing alternative copying mechanisms into the non-copying task. In children, prior biases towards specific shapes led to convergence in behaviour across chains, while baboon chains showed signs of lineage specificity. We conclude that CCE can result from mechanisms with varying degrees of fidelity in transmission and thus that high-fidelity copying is not necessarily the key to CCE.


Sujet(s)
Évolution culturelle , Papio papio/psychologie , Apprentissage social , Animaux , Enfant , Enfant d'âge préscolaire , Femelle , Humains , Mâle , Comportement social
4.
J Hum Evol ; 125: 87-98, 2018 12.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30502900

RÉSUMÉ

Ritualized greetings, defined as exchanges of non-aggressive signals, are common among males living in multi-male groups and are thought to balance the trade-offs of male co-residence. While ritualized greetings are widespread in the animal kingdom, the behavioral repertoire described in the genus Papio is exceptional, as it involves potentially harmful behaviors such as genital fondling. Although greetings are one of the most striking male social interactions in baboons, their function is still disputed. We investigated the function of male-male ritualized greeting behavior in wild Guinea baboons. This species lives in multilevel societies where several 'units' comprising a primary male, females with young, and occasionally a secondary male form a 'party', and two to three parties form a gang. Adult males maintain affiliative relationships with preferred male partners whom they support in coalitions, regardless of kinship. We examined the social behavior of 24 adolescent and adult males (∼900 h focal observations) in the Niokolo-Koba National Park, Senegal, to test whether greetings reflect relationship quality or function to buffer tension. Greetings were ten times more frequent than aggression and twice as frequent as affiliation. Neither dyadic aggression nor tense context predicted greeting rate, discounting the buffering hypothesis. Greetings occurred almost exclusively between males of the same party, even when other parties were around. Within parties, spatially tolerant partners greeted more frequently but dyads did not greet due to proximity prior to the greeting. Although affiliation did not predict overall greeting rate, intense and potentially costly greetings were more likely between males with stronger affiliative relationships. Greetings in Guinea baboons appear to signal commitment among party members, test relationships among spatially tolerant partners, and accentuate relationship strength among highly affiliated males. Although ritualized baboon greetings lack the symbolic component of human rituals, they appear to serve similar functions, specifically to strengthen in-group affiliation and promote cooperation.


Sujet(s)
Comportement cérémoniel , Papio papio/psychologie , Comportement social , Animaux , Mâle , Sénégal
5.
PLoS One ; 12(8): e0183876, 2017.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28859134

RÉSUMÉ

The ability of Baboons (papio papio) to distinguish between English words and nonwords has been modeled using a deep learning convolutional network model that simulates a ventral pathway in which lexical representations of different granularity develop. However, given that pigeons (columba livia), whose brain morphology is drastically different, can also be trained to distinguish between English words and nonwords, it appears that a less species-specific learning algorithm may be required to explain this behavior. Accordingly, we examined whether the learning model of Rescorla and Wagner, which has proved to be amazingly fruitful in understanding animal and human learning could account for these data. We show that a discrimination learning network using gradient orientation features as input units and word and nonword units as outputs succeeds in predicting baboon lexical decision behavior-including key lexical similarity effects and the ups and downs in accuracy as learning unfolds-with surprising precision. The models performance, in which words are not explicitly represented, is remarkable because it is usually assumed that lexicality decisions, including the decisions made by baboons and pigeons, are mediated by explicit lexical representations. By contrast, our results suggest that in learning to perform lexical decision tasks, baboons and pigeons do not construct a hierarchy of lexical units. Rather, they make optimal use of low-level information obtained through the massively parallel processing of gradient orientation features. Accordingly, we suggest that reading in humans first involves initially learning a high-level system building on letter representations acquired from explicit instruction in literacy, which is then integrated into a conventionalized oral communication system, and that like the latter, fluent reading involves the massively parallel processing of the low-level features encoding semantic contrasts.


Sujet(s)
Columbidae/physiologie , Apprentissage discriminatif/physiologie , Papio papio/physiologie , Reconnaissance visuelle des formes/physiologie , Animaux , Humains , Langage , Réseau nerveux/physiologie , Papio papio/psychologie , Temps de réaction , Lecture , Sémantique , Spécificité d'espèce
6.
Anim Cogn ; 18(6): 1347-62, 2015 Nov.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26232908

RÉSUMÉ

Metacognition refers to the ability of an organism to evaluate its states of knowledge (metacognitive monitoring) and engage in appropriate information-seeking behaviors when a lack of knowledge is detected (metacognitive control). This study assessed metacognitive monitoring and control in three Guinea baboons (Papio papio). Monkeys were required to report on a touchscreen the location of two target stimuli that had previously appeared briefly on a grid. They could either respond directly or use a "Repeat" key providing an opportunity to repeat the target stimuli. In Experiment 1, the baboons used the Repeat key more frequently in difficult trials and transferred this use of the key to novel conditions. Two baboons showed higher accuracy when they declined using the key compared to Baseline trials in which the key was not available, suggesting accurate metacognitive monitoring judgments. The same two baboons were consistently faster at reporting the targets' locations after a repetition of the stimulus. In Experiment 2, the baboons had to choose between two Repeat keys, one for each target. Two baboons showed a preference for repeating the presentation of the less visible target, suggesting that they identified what information they lack. Overall, results support the hypothesis of metacognitive monitoring in baboons, and also provide limited evidence for metacognitive control. We propose that tests requiring subjects to choose between several metacognitive responses in computerized tasks provide a suitable new approach for studying targeted information-seeking behaviors in animals.


Sujet(s)
Jugement , Métacognition , Papio papio/psychologie , Animaux , Comportement de recherche d'information , Mâle
7.
Anim Cogn ; 18(6): 1339-46, 2015 Nov.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26194414

RÉSUMÉ

Cognitive set can be both helpful and harmful in problem solving. A large set of similar problems may be solved mechanically by applying a single-solution method. However, efficiency might be sacrificed if a better solution exists and is overlooked. Despite half a century of research on cognitive set, there have been no attempts to investigate whether it occurs in nonhuman species. The current study utilized a nonverbal, computer task to compare cognitive set between 104 humans and 15 baboons (Papio papio). A substantial difference was found between humans' and baboons' abilities to break cognitive set. Consistent with previous studies, the majority of humans were highly impaired by set, yet baboons were almost completely unaffected. Analysis of the human data revealed that children (aged 7-10) were significantly better able to break set than adolescents (11-18) and adults (19-68). Both the evolutionary and developmental implications of these findings are discussed.


Sujet(s)
Cognition , Papio papio/psychologie , Résolution de problème , Adolescent , Adulte , Sujet âgé , Animaux , Évolution biologique , Enfant , Femelle , Humains , Mâle , Adulte d'âge moyen
8.
Proc Biol Sci ; 281(1797)2014 12 22.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25377450

RÉSUMÉ

Culture pervades human life and is at the origin of the success of our species. A wide range of other animals have culture too, but often in a limited form that does not complexify through the gradual accumulation of innovations. We developed a new paradigm to study cultural evolution in primates in order to better evaluate our closest relatives' cultural capacities. Previous studies using transmission chain experimental paradigms, in which the behavioural output of one individual becomes the target behaviour for the next individual in the chain, show that cultural transmission can lead to the progressive emergence of systematically structured behaviours in humans. Inspired by this work, we combined a pattern reproduction task on touch screens with an iterated learning procedure to develop transmission chains of baboons (Papio papio). Using this procedure, we show that baboons can exhibit three fundamental aspects of human cultural evolution: a progressive increase in performance, the emergence of systematic structure and the presence of lineage specificity. Our results shed new light on human uniqueness: we share with our closest relatives essential capacities to produce human-like cultural evolution.


Sujet(s)
Évolution culturelle , Apprentissage , Papio papio/psychologie , Comportement social , Animaux , Femelle , Mâle , Papio papio/physiologie
9.
Anim Cogn ; 17(4): 911-24, 2014 Jul.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24352791

RÉSUMÉ

Relational processing involves learning about the relationship between or among stimuli, transcending the individual stimuli, so that abstract knowledge generalizable to novel situations is acquired. Relational processing has been studied in animals as well as in humans, but little attention has been paid to the contribution of specific items to relational thinking or to the factors that may affect that contribution. This study assessed the intertwined effects of item and relational processing in nonhuman primates. Using a procedure that entailed both expanding and contracting sets of pictorial items, we trained 13 baboons on a two-alternative forced-choice task, in which they had to distinguish horizontal from vertical relational patterns. In Experiment 1, monkeys engaged in item-based processing with a small training set size, and they progressively engaged in relation-based processing as training set size was increased. However, in Experiment 2, overtraining with a small stimulus set promoted the processing of item-based information. These findings underscore similarities in how humans and nonhuman primates process higher-order stimulus relations.


Sujet(s)
Apprentissage , Papio papio/psychologie , Animaux , Comportement de choix , Cognition , , Reconnaissance visuelle des formes , Stimulation lumineuse ,
10.
J Comp Psychol ; 127(3): 237-44, 2013 Aug.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23566028

RÉSUMÉ

[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported in Vol 127(3) of Journal of Comparative Psychology (see record 2013-30238-001). In the article, the link to the supplemental material was not included. Supplemental material for this article is available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031494.supp.] The perception and production of facial expressions have traditionally been used to infer hemispheric specialization for emotions in both human and nonhuman primates. The authors examined orofacial asymmetries in Olive baboons using 2 methodologies. First, objective measures were used to assess hemimouth length and area for screeching and eyebrow-raising. Right-hemisphere specialization was found only for screeching. Second, subjective measures were acquired via a human judgment of the emotional intensity of baboons' chimeric faces for the 2 previous emotional behaviors plus a neutral expression. They also addressed the question of hemispheric lateralization for emotions in human judges by using a chimeric task with human faces. The left-composite chimeric faces of baboons and the human chimeric faces were judged to be emotionally stronger than the right ones for the emotional behaviors, and no preference was found for the neutral non emotional category. Human participants, especially those who displayed a left-hemisphere specialization for processing emotions, demonstrated higher ability to distinguish the most emotionally communicative signals in baboons' facial expressions than right-hemispheric dominant participants. These results are discussed within the theoretical framework of the human and the nonhuman primate literature on hemispheric specialization for perception and production of facial expressions of emotions.


Sujet(s)
Expression faciale , Latéralité fonctionnelle , Papio papio/psychologie , Communication animale , Animaux , Encéphale/physiologie , Émotion exprimée , Femelle , Humains , Mâle , Communication non verbale/psychologie , Stimulation lumineuse , Jeune adulte
11.
Learn Behav ; 41(3): 229-37, 2013 Sep.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23354955

RÉSUMÉ

Analogical reasoning is a cornerstone of human cognition, but the extent and limits of analogical reasoning in animals remains unclear. Recent studies have demonstrated that apes and monkeys can match relations with relations, suggesting that these species have the basic abilities for analogical reasoning. However, analogical reasoning in humans entails two additional cognitive processes that remain unexplored in animals. These include the ability to (1) flexibly reencode the relations instantiated by the source domain as a function of the relational properties of the target domain, and (2) to match relations across different stimulus dimensions. Using a two-dimensional relational matching-to-sample task, the present study demonstrates that these two abilities are in the scope of baboons, given appropriate training. These findings unveil the richness of the cognitive processes implicated during analogical reasoning in nonhuman primates and further reduce the apparent gap between animal and human cognition.


Sujet(s)
Cognition , Formation de concepts , Papio papio/psychologie , Résolution de problème , Animaux , Mâle
12.
Anim Cogn ; 16(3): 519-24, 2013 May.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23334572

RÉSUMÉ

Reasoning by analogy is one of the most complex and highly adaptive cognitive processes in abstract thinking. For humans, analogical reasoning entails the judgment and conceptual mapping of relations-between-relations and is facilitated by language (Gentner in Cogn Sci 7:155-170, 1983; Premack in Thought without language, Oxford University Press, New York, 1986). Recent evidence, however, shows that monkeys like "language-trained" apes exhibit similar capacity to match relations-between-relations (Fagot and Thompson in Psychol Sci 22:1304-1309, 2011; Flemming et al. in J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Process 37:353-360, 2011; Truppa et al. in Plos One 6(8):e23809, 2011). Whether this behavior is driven by the abstraction of categorical relations or alternatively by direct perception of variability (entropy) is crucial to the debate as to whether nonhuman animals are capable of analogical reasoning. In the current study, we presented baboons (Papio papio) and humans (Homo sapiens) with a computerized same/different relational-matching task that in principle could be solved by either strategy. Both baboons and humans produced markedly similar patterns of responding. Both species responded different when the perceptual variability of a stimulus array fell exactly between or even closer to that of a same display. Overall, these results demonstrate that categorical abstraction trumped perceptual properties and, like humans, Old World monkeys can solve the analogical matching task by judging the categorical abstract equivalence of same/different relations-between-relations.


Sujet(s)
Formation de concepts , Papio papio/psychologie , Résolution de problème , Adulte , Animaux , Femelle , Humains , Mâle , Pensée (activité mentale) , Jeune adulte
13.
J Comp Psychol ; 127(2): 194-201, 2013 May.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22142038

RÉSUMÉ

A computerized visual search task was presented to 18 guinea baboons (Papio papio) ranging from 2.7 to 14.3 years of age. The task, inspired from Hick's (1952) task, required detection of a target among a variable number of distractors equidistant to a start button. The reaction times (RTs) and movement times both increased with the number of distractors expressed in bits of information. However, the slope of RT per bit function correlated positively with age, whereas a negative correlation was found for the movement time slopes. In Experiment 2, the same baboons were required to inhibit an ongoing manual pointing toward a target stimulus, to reengage in a new point as a consequence of a change in target location. Results revealed a more accurate performance in the adults, suggesting that differences in behavioral strategies in Experiment 1 can be accounted for by a greater inhibitory control of the adult participants. Implications of these results are discussed regarding the relation between attention, inhibitory control, and behavioral strategies in monkeys, and the general significance of RT slopes in visual search tasks.


Sujet(s)
Attention/physiologie , Comportement animal/physiologie , Fonction exécutive/physiologie , Papio papio/physiologie , Performance psychomotrice/physiologie , Facteurs âges , Animaux , Femelle , Inhibition psychologique , Mâle , Mouvement/physiologie , Papio papio/psychologie , Temps de réaction/physiologie , Perception de l'espace/physiologie
14.
Anim Cogn ; 14(3): 415-26, 2011 May.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21225440

RÉSUMÉ

Humans readily group component elements into a coherent perceptual whole and perceive the global form of visual patterns in priority over local features, which stands in contrast to at least some data from the animal literature, suggesting possible species differences in perceptual processes. In this study, chimpanzees and baboons were required to match intact and partially contour-deleted line-drawings in a computerized task in order to further explore the ability of nonhuman primates to group component elements into a coherent perceptual whole and to determine to what extent they use the global form or the local features. Experiment 1 showed that the baboons and chimpanzees matched intact continuous line-drawings (intact-intact) more easily than partially contour-deleted line-drawings (deleted-deleted). Both species could also match partially deleted line-drawings with their intact version (deleted-intact), but at a lower performance level. Experiment 2 further showed that subjects from the two species could match partially deleted coherent line-drawings (deleted-deleted) more easily than their scrambled incoherent versions (scrambled-scrambled). They could however match the coherent deleted forms with their scrambled version (deleted-scrambled). It is suggested that solutions of these tasks rely on the processing of both global and local cues, with no clear-cut species difference in that ability. Overall, the results show that contour completion of line-drawings was not easy. Implications on the processing of human-made two-dimensional representations such as line-drawings are discussed.


Sujet(s)
, Perception de la forme , Pan troglodytes/psychologie , Papio papio/psychologie , Animaux , /physiologie , Femelle , Perception de la forme/physiologie , Mâle , Pan troglodytes/physiologie , Papio papio/physiologie , Stimulation lumineuse
15.
Anim Cogn ; 12(2): 369-79, 2009 Mar.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18925421

RÉSUMÉ

How nonhuman primates process pictures of natural scenes or objects remains a matter of debates. This issue was addressed in the current research by questioning the processing of the canonical orientation of pictures in baboons. Two adult guinea baboons were trained to use an interactive key (IK) on a touch-screen to change the orientation of target pictures showing humans or quadruped mammals until upright. In experiment 1, both baboons successfully learned to use the IK when that key induced a 90 degrees rightward rotation of the picture, but post-training transfer of performance did not occur to novel pictures of natural scenes due to potential motor biases. In Experiment 2, a touch on IK randomly displayed the pictures in any of the four cardinal orientations. Baboons successfully learned the task, but transfer to novel pictures could only be demonstrated after they had been exposed to 360-480 pictures in that condition. Experiment 3 confirmed positive transfers to novel pictures, and showed that both the figure and background information controlled the behavior. Our research on baboons therefore demonstrates the development and use of an "upright" concept, and indicates that picture processing modes strongly depend on the subject's past experience with naturalistic pictorial stimuli.


Sujet(s)
Compréhension , Formation de concepts , Orientation , Papio papio/psychologie , Perception visuelle , Animaux , Mâle , Stimulation lumineuse , Comportement spatial ,
16.
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process ; 33(4): 381-91, 2007 Oct.
Article de Anglais | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17924787

RÉSUMÉ

Humans apply complex conceptual judgments to point-light displays (PLDs) representing biological motion (BM), but how animals process this kind of display remains uncertain. Four baboons (Papio papio) were trained to discriminate BM from nonbiological motion PLDs using an operant computerized test system. Transfer tests were given after training with novel BM stimuli representing humans or baboons (Experiment 1), with inverted PLDs (Experiment 2), and with BM stimuli in which body parts had been spatially disorganized (Experiment 3). Very limited transfer was obtained with the novel and inverted displays in Experiments 1 and 2, but transfer was much higher after spatial disorganization in Experiment 3. It is suggested that the baboons did not retrieve and interpret the articulated shape of the human or monkey body from the BM PLD stimuli, but rather focused their attention on the configural properties of subparts of the stimuli. Limits in perceptual grouping and restricted abilities in picture-object equivalence might explain why the baboons did not map BM PLD displays onto what they represent.


Sujet(s)
Formation de concepts , Apprentissage discriminatif , Perception du mouvement , Papio papio/psychologie , Reconnaissance visuelle des formes , Clôture perceptive , Animaux , Attention , Phénomènes biomécaniques , Conditionnement opérant , Signaux , Femelle , Mâle , Orientation , Résolution de problème ,
SÉLECTION CITATIONS
DÉTAIL DE RECHERCHE