Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
: 20 | 50 | 100
1 - 20 de 123
1.
Pain ; 2024 May 07.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38713801

ABSTRACT: Pain perception and its modulation are fundamental to human learning and adaptive behavior. This study investigated the hypothesis that pain perception is tied to pain's learning function. Thirty-one participants performed a threat conditioning task where certain cues were associated with a possibility of receiving a painful electric shock. The cues that signaled potential pain or safety were regularly changed, requiring participants to continually establish new associations. Using computational models, we quantified participants' pain expectations and prediction errors throughout the task and assessed their relationship with pain perception and electrophysiological responses. Our findings suggest that subjective pain perception increases with prediction error, that is, when pain was unexpected. Prediction errors were also related to physiological nociceptive responses, including the amplitude of nociceptive flexion reflex and electroencephalography markers of cortical nociceptive processing (N1-P2-evoked potential and gamma-band power). In addition, higher pain expectations were related to increased late event-related potential responses and alpha/beta decreases in amplitude during cue presentation. These results further strengthen the idea of a crucial link between pain and learning and suggest that understanding the influence of learning mechanisms in pain modulation could help us understand when and why pain perception is modulated in health and disease.

2.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 76(6): 1245-1263, 2023 Jun.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35899896

In electroencephalography (EEG) studies of visual search, task-irrelevant fillers are included in displays to balance bottom-up stimulation across the visual field and generally considered as inconsequential for performance or EEG results. We examined the impact of fillers on target and distractor processing using lateralised event-related potentials (ERPs). Two task-relevant items (TRIs) were presented, with or without fillers. One TRI (target or target-colour distractor) was on the vertical midline and the other in a lateral position (left or right visual field) on an imaginary circle around fixation. An N2pc was elicited by lateral targets and task-relevant distractors, suggesting that attention was allocated to the lateral TRI because it possessed a target defining feature (colour). A Ptc was only elicited by lateral task-relevant distractors, in line with previous research suggesting that this component is associated with distractor processing. When fillers were also in the circular arrangement, alterations in performance and neural activity occurred. Fillers enhanced and delayed attentional deployment (N2pc) and delayed distractor processing (Ptc). Critically, we observed no difference in Ptc amplitude according to filler presence. Thus, if the Ptc reflects active suppression (or attentional disengagement), it was not required for fillers. ERPs were also modulated by the distance between TRIs (which could be separated by one or four filler positions), but differently according to the colour scheme (blue TRIs with grey fillers or vice versa). Our results suggest that fillers affect lateralised electrophysiological activity at multiple loci during visual search and should not be considered inconsequential.


Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials , Humans , Electroencephalography/methods , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Attention/physiology , Brain/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Photic Stimulation
3.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 476, 2022 01 10.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35013361

Verbal fluency (VF) is a heterogeneous cognitive function that requires executive as well as language abilities. The purpose of this study was to elucidate the specificity of the resting state MEG correlates of the executive and language components. To this end, we administered a VF test, another verbal test (Vocabulary), and another executive test (Trail Making Test), and we recorded 5-min eyes-open resting-state MEG data in 28 healthy participants. We used source-reconstructed spectral power estimates to compute correlation/anticorrelation MEG clusters with the performance at each test, as well as with the advantage in performance between tests, across individuals using cluster-level statistics in the standard frequency bands. By obtaining conjunction clusters between verbal fluency scores and factor loading obtained for verbal fluency and each of the two other tests, we showed a core of slow clusters (delta to beta) localized in the right hemisphere, in adjacent parts of the premotor, pre-central and post-central cortex in the mid-lateral regions related to executive monitoring. We also found slow parietal clusters bilaterally and a cluster in the gamma 2 and 3 bands in the left inferior frontal gyrus likely associated with phonological processing involved in verbal fluency.


Brain/physiology , Language , Verbal Behavior , Adult , Cognition , Female , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Vocabulary , Young Adult
4.
Psychophysiology ; 59(1): e13943, 2022 01.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34536021

Previous event-related potential (ERP) studies show that a salient lateral sound activates the visual cortex more strongly contralateral to the sound, observed as an auditory-evoked contralateral occipital positivity (ACOP). Studies showed that this activation enhances the early cortical processing of co-localized visual stimuli presented after, reflected by better detection rates, better discrimination, and sharper perceived contrast. We replicated the ACOP, using earphones, and tested whether auditory cuing can influence temporal order judgments (TOJ) for two visual stimuli (horizontal arrangement) as well as if the ACOP would predict the amplitude of this influence. A lateral salient sound was followed, after 150 or 630 ms, by the visual presentation of a pair of disks, one in left and one in right hemifield, with variable SOA. The TOJ task was to indicate which disk appeared first or which disk appeared second (controlling for response bias). We observed an ACOP at posterior electrode sites and confirmed our hypothesis that the lateral sound influenced TOJ by accelerating the perception of the disk presented on the cued side, even though the sound was irrelevant to the task. Furthermore, the ACOP amplitude was correlated to this visual perceptual change, indicating that a larger change in brain activity was associated with a faster processing of co-localized visual stimuli.


Attention/physiology , Cues , Electrophysiology , Judgment/physiology , Sound , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
5.
Psychophysiology ; 58(3): e13751, 2021 03.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33347633

We studied the effects of mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) in an aging population. We examined visual search with event-related potentials (ERPs) and event-related fields (ERF) for a lateral color singleton focusing on the P1 and N1 in each hemisphere. Forty participants (19 mTBI and 21 controls) aged 50 to 72 performed a visual search task, while we recorded their magnetoencephalogram (MEG) with simultaneous electroencephalogram (EEG). We compared visual ERPs and ERFs and associated cortical activity estimated using MEG source localization. Relative to matched controls, participants with an mTBI had a smaller P1 in the left hemisphere and a smaller N1 in the right hemisphere. Also, mTBI participants showed inversed activation patterns across the hemispheres during the N1 in MEG compared with controls. This is the first study to investigate the impact of mTBI on neuronal source activations during early visual processing in an aging population. Results showed that when aging individuals suffer from an mTBI, there are perturbations in the amplitude and hemispheric dominance patterns in the visual P1 and N1 responses that are visible for months to years following the injury. Our findings indicate that mTBI can lead to modifications of sensory and/or perceptual responses, suggesting possible adaptive functional reorganization following mTBI.


Aging/physiology , Attention/physiology , Brain Concussion/physiopathology , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Magnetoencephalography , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Aged , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
6.
Psychophysiology ; 57(11): e13651, 2020 11.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32797636

A contralateral posterior negativity elicited by lateral oddballs (N2pc) and a bilateral posterior negativity elicited by vertical midline oddballs (bilateral N2) are ERP components reflecting attentional deployment that have been rarely compared. In different tasks, we explored to what extent they reflect similar underlying mechanisms of attention. We used a multiple-frame procedure to present pop-out color oddballs among distractors. A homogeneous condition contained only distractors (0 oddballs) and served as a control condition that was subtracted from oddball-present conditions to isolate attention effects. The number of oddballs and the vertical hemifield containing them (upper vs. lower) were two critical factors. For the lower hemifield, the signal amplitude increased with the number of oddballs, otherwise had similar effects and scalp distributions, suggesting the bilateral N2 acted as a bilateral N2pc and likely reflected similar underlying generators. For the upper hemifield, component amplitude also increased with the number of oddballs, but the scalp distributions were positive and more centered, suggesting inverted generators across the two vertical hemifields. An ipsilateral positivity occurred about 50 ms after a contralateral positivity, similar in magnitude, producing a biphasic contra-minus-ipsi difference wave. Previously reported smaller negative N2pc components for upper hemifield oddballs likely reflected a negative lobe artificially created by the subtraction of a lagged positive ipsilateral response. The results compel us to argue for a systematic separation of data for upper versus lower hemifields in studies of visuo-spatial attention, and the use of an experimental design permitting the separate estimation of contralateral and ipsilateral responses.


Attention/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Young Adult
7.
Psychophysiology ; 57(11): e13644, 2020 11.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32710667

Many visual search paradigms use color to distinguish task-relevant items from those considered fillers (e.g., blue task-relevant items and grey fillers). Hilimire and colleagues suggested that the N2pc, a lateralized electrophysiological component typically observed in visual attention, is a neural correlate for localized attentional interference, which postulates that target selection is degraded by nearby competing stimuli. In their study, N2pc amplitude decreased with decreasing distance between task-relevant items presented among fillers. With an increase in distance, however, there was also an increase in the number of fillers between task-relevant items. We tested whether this distance effect could be explained by the presence of fillers near task-relevant items rather than their proximity per se. We manipulated the distance between task-relevant items (adjacent, separated by two, or by four positions) and the presence/absence of fillers orthogonally. We used two color schemes: blue task-relevant items and grey fillers or grey task-relevant items and blue fillers (manipulated between-subjects) to control for color interactions. N2pc amplitude increased with increasing distance, but only when fillers were present, suggesting that the results of Hilimire et al. may be due to increasing fillers interference. Exploratory analyses also suggested that the colors selected to be task-relevant and task-irrelevant could play a role in our ability to filter task-irrelevant information. Our results suggest that fillers are not as inconsequential as sometimes assumed and generally support the Ambiguity Resolution Theory, where nearby items increase N2pc amplitude because of a greater need for focused attention.


Attention/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Electroencephalography , Feedback, Sensory/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
8.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 155: 105-117, 2020 09.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32553753

We studied the cognitive processes involved in pure acoustic memory retrieval using ERPs and independent component analysis (ICA) of the ERPs. We used a modified Sternberg task with pure tones as memory items, with 3 memory set size conditions (2, 4, or 6 tones), and a control condition in which no task-relevant tone was presented (tones replaced by white-noise segments). Participants reported the presence or absence of a probe tone in the memory set. EEG signals were recorded using a BioSemi ActiveTwo system, with 64 active electrodes following the 10-10 system and average-mastoids reference. A P3 component, at electrode POz, was elicited by the probe when items were to be held in memory, no P3 was observed in control trials. With an increase in memory load, P3 amplitude decreased, accuracy was lower, and response time (RT) longer. There was a clear recency effect, in the electrophysiological and behavioral results reflected in a larger P3, shorter RT, and higher accuracy, when the probe matched the last tone of a sequence than when it matched any of the other presented tones, regardless of memory load. P3 amplitude and behavioral results were similar across all the other serial positions and loads. The results suggest that acoustic memory for pure tones is mediated by two subsystems: one encoding the last item with high probability, and another for all other serial positions. ICA of the ERPs isolated parieto-occipital ICA components with larger activations for probes that matched the last serial position than for earlier ones, and an anterior component that had little activation for the last serial positions and large activations for all earlier ones, corroborating the two-systems theory.


Evoked Potentials , Memory, Short-Term , Acoustic Stimulation , Acoustics , Electroencephalography , Humans , Reaction Time
9.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 82(4): 1835-1846, 2020 May.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31898070

We studied negative priming (NP) in auditory attention switching. In a cued variant of dichotic listening, two spoken number words were presented, one to each ear, one spoken by a female, and one spoken by a male voice. A visual cue indicated whether the male or female voice was the target. A numerical magnitude judgement of the target number was required. The selection criterion could either switch or repeat across trials, so there were attention switch and repetition trials. Two experiments examined NP (distractor becomes target) and also included a "competitor priming" (CP) condition (target becomes distractor), relative to a "no priming" condition (target and distractor not related to previous trial). In Experiment 1, we investigated the basic priming effects. In Experiment 2, we additionally varied the response-cue interval (RCI; 100 ms vs. 1,900 ms) to examine time-related changes in priming. We found longer response times (RT) for switch trials compared with repetition trials (attention switch costs)-that is, when the internal processing context changed. In addition, we found longer RT for NP trials as well as reduced switch costs in long RCI, suggesting that previously relevant attentional settings dissipate over longer time. However, NP was not influenced by attention switches, and it was also not affected by RCI. Hence, NP in auditory attention switching does not seem strongly context or time sensitive.


Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Attention/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Intention , Voice/physiology , Adult , Cues , Female , Humans , Judgment/physiology , Male , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
10.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 32(4): 703-721, 2020 04.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31765600

We bisected the sequence of processing into operations taking place before or after the engagement of visual-spatial attention during a difficult search task using event-related potentials. We were able to assign variance in RTs associated with experimental factor effects to phases of processing by examining stimulus-locked (SLpcN) and response-locked (RLpcN) posterior contralateral negativity. Participants searched for a gray square with one gap among gray squares with two gaps. The number of displayed items (set size) and the number of response alternatives were varied. Both experimental manipulations affected the onset latency of the RLpcN, whereas the SLpcN showed small or no latency effects, suggesting they had effects after the initial deployment of attention. Moreover, amplitude effects in the RLpcN and SLpcN behaved similarly. Most importantly, different aspects of the RLpcN dissociated the experimental manipulations: Set size primarily affected processing between RLpcN onset and peak amplitude of the RLpcN, whereas the number of response alternatives affected the onset latency and the latency of peak amplitude of RLpcN. These results show how RLpcN activity can dissociate factor effects that are not separable with SLpcN activity during difficult search.


Attention/physiology , Brain/physiology , Evoked Potentials , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Psychomotor Performance , Reaction Time , Young Adult
11.
Restor Neurol Neurosci ; 37(6): 553-569, 2019.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31839615

BACKGROUND: The cholinergic system is a potent neuromodulator system that plays a critical role in cortical plasticity, attention, and learning. Recently, it was found that boosting this system during perceptual learning robustly enhances sensory perception in rodents. In particular, pairing cholinergic activation with visual stimulation increases neuronal responses, cue detection ability, and long-term facilitation in the primary visual cortex. The mechanisms of cholinergic enhancement are closely linked to attentional processes, long-term potentiation, and modulation of the excitatory/inhibitory balance. Some studies currently examine this effect in humans. OBJECTIVE: The present article reviews the research from our laboratory, examining whether potentiating the central cholinergic system could help visual perception and restoration. METHODS: Electrophysiological or pharmacological enhancement of the cholinergic system are administered during a visual training. Electrophysiological responses and perceptual learning performance are investigated before and after the training in rats and humans. This approach's ability to restore visual capacities following a visual deficit induced by a partial optic nerve crush is also investigated in rats. RESULTS: The coupling of visual training to cholinergic stimulation improved visual discrimination and visual acuity in rats, and improved residual vision after a deficit. These changes were due to muscarinic and nicotinic transmissions and were associated with a functional improvement of evoked potentials. In humans, potentiation of cholinergic transmission with 5 mg of donepezil showed improved learning and ocular dominance plasticity, although this treatment was ineffective in augmenting the perceptual threshold and electroencephalography. CONCLUSIONS: Potential therapeutic outcomes ought to facilitate vision restoration using commercially available cholinergic agents combined with visual stimulation in order to prevent irreversible vision loss in patients. This approach has the potential to help a large population of visually impaired individuals.


Cholinergic Agents/therapeutic use , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Vision Disorders/drug therapy , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Acetylcholine/pharmacology , Acetylcholine/therapeutic use , Animals , Cholinergic Agents/pharmacology , Donepezil/pharmacology , Donepezil/therapeutic use , Evoked Potentials, Visual/drug effects , Humans , Neuronal Plasticity/drug effects , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Rats , Rodentia , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Visual Cortex/drug effects , Visual Perception/drug effects
12.
Brain Sci ; 9(12)2019 Nov 27.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31783501

OBJECTIVE: Attentional problems are amongst the most commonly reported complaints following mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI), including difficulties orienting and disengaging attention, sustaining it over time, and dividing attentional resources across multiple simultaneous demands. The objective of this study was to track, using a single novel electrophysiological task, various components associated with the deployment of visuospatial selective attention. METHODS: A paradigm was designed to evoke earlier visual evoked potentials (VEPs), as well as attention-related and visuocognitive ERPs. Data from 36 individuals with mTBI (19 subacute, 17 chronic) and 22 uninjured controls are presented. Postconcussion symptoms (PCS), anxiety (BAI), depression (BDI-II) and visual attention (TEA Map Search, DKEFS Trail Making Test) were also assessed. RESULTS: Earlier VEPs (P1, N1), as well as processes related to visuospatial orientation (N2pc) and encoding in visual short-term memory (SPCN), appear comparable in mTBI and control participants. However, there appears to be a disruption in the spatiotemporal dynamics of attention (N2pc-Ptc, P2) in subacute mTBI, which recovers within six months. This is also reflected in altered neuropsychological performance (information processing speed, attentional shifting). Furthermore, orientation of attention (P3a) and working memory processes (P3b) are also affected and remain as such in the chronic post-mTBI period, in co-occurrence with persisting postconcussion symptomatology. CONCLUSIONS: This study adds original findings indicating that such a sensitive and rigorous ERP task implemented at diagnostic and follow-up levels could allow for the identification of subtle but complex brain activation and connectivity deficits that can occur following mTBI.

13.
Front Neurosci ; 13: 875, 2019.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31507359

The inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) is a white matter tract that connects the occipital and the temporal lobes. ILF abnormalities have been associated with deficits in visual processing and language comprehension in dementia patients, thus suggesting that its integrity is important for semantic processing. However, it remains elusive whether ILF microstructural organization per se impacts the visual semantic processing efficiency in the healthy brain. The present study aims to investigate whether there is an association between ILF's microstructural organization and visual semantic processing at the individual level. We hypothesized that the efficiency of visual semantic processing positively correlates with the degree of anisotropy of the ILF. We studied 10 healthy right-handed subjects. We determined fractional anisotropy (FA) of the ILF using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). We extracted N400m latency and amplitude from magnetoencephalography (MEG) signals during a visual semantic decision task. N400m and mean FA of the ILF were left lateralized with the higher FA value in the left hemisphere. Inter-individual analysis showed that FA of the ILF negatively correlated with the N400m latency and amplitude, which suggests that high ILF anisotropy is associated with more efficient semantic processing. In summary, our findings provide supporting evidence for a role of the ILF in language comprehension.

14.
Cancer ; 125(20): 3639-3648, 2019 10 15.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31310324

BACKGROUND: Acute lymphoblastic leukemia (ALL) is the most common childhood cancer. Treatments against ALL might lead to later cognitive effects and alterations in brain structure in survivors but to the authors' knowledge the observed variability in the severity of neurocognitive deficits is not fully understood. The objective of the current study was to investigate abnormalities in visual short-term memory (VSTM) brain activation in survivors of childhood ALL using magnetoencephalography. METHODS: A VSTM task was completed by 40 survivors of ALL and 26 controls. VSTM capacity (Cowan K) and brain activation were assessed during the retention period of the task (400-1400 milliseconds) using a standard minimum norm source localization method. RESULTS: Performance (Cowan K) was found to be similar between survivors of ALL and controls. Atypical brain activation was found in survivors of ALL during the task, including overactivation of regions usually involved in VSTM (lateral occipital, precentral gyrus, and postcentral gyrus), recruitment of regions that typically are not involved in VSTM (superior/middle temporal gyrus and supramarginal gyrus), and lower activation of frontal brain regions (inferior frontal gyrus). These patterns of activation were modulated by the age at the time of cancer onset (P = .01) because activity was found to be reduced in participants who were younger at diagnosis. CONCLUSIONS: The results of the current study suggest a pattern of neural inefficiency and compensatory activity during VSTM in survivors of ALL.


Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Memory, Short-Term , Ocular Physiological Phenomena , Precursor Cell Lymphoblastic Leukemia-Lymphoma/physiopathology , Adult , Cancer Survivors , Child , Female , Frontal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Precursor Cell Lymphoblastic Leukemia-Lymphoma/complications , Precursor Cell Lymphoblastic Leukemia-Lymphoma/diagnostic imaging , Young Adult
15.
Neuroimage Clin ; 23: 101907, 2019.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31233955

Mounting evidence suggests that mild traumatic brain injuries (mTBI) have long-term effects that interact with the aging process to precipitate cognitive decline. This line of research predicts that early exposure to brain trauma is particularly detrimental to long-term brain integrity. However, a second line of research into the effects of age at trauma onset predict that older brains are more vulnerable to the effects of mTBI than younger brains. We sought to determine whether patients who sustain a mTBI earlier in life fare better than patients who sustain a mTBI at an older age. We conducted a multi-cohort, case-control study, with participants randomly sampled from a population of patients with a history of mTBI. We recruited two cohorts of aging participants (N = 74, mean [SD] = 61.16 [6.41]) matched in age and education levels that differed in only one respect: age at mTBI onset. One cohort sustained their concussion in their early twenties (24.60 [6.34] y/o), the other in their early sixties (61.05 [4.90] y/o). Each mTBI cohort had its own matched control group. Participants underwent high-resolution MRI at 3 Tesla for T1 and diffusion-weighted images (DWI) acquisition. Images were processed and analyzed using Deformation-Based Morphometry and DWI Tract-Based Spatial Statistics to identify group differences in a 2 × 2 ANOVA design. Results showed a significant interaction on DWI measures of white matter integrity indicating larger anomalies in participants who sustained a mTBI at a younger age (F1,70, P < .05, FDR corrected). These findings suggest that mTBI initiates a lifelong neurodegeneration process that outweighs the risks associated with sustaining a mTBI at an older age. Implications are important for young athletes' populations exposed to the risk of mTBI in the practice of their sports and for retired athletes aging with a history of concussions sustained at a younger age.


Aging , Brain Concussion/pathology , Brain Concussion/physiopathology , Disease Progression , Age Factors , Age of Onset , Aged , Aging/pathology , Aging/physiology , Brain Concussion/diagnostic imaging , Case-Control Studies , Cohort Studies , Diffusion Tensor Imaging , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
16.
Neuroreport ; 29(17): 1437-1442, 2018 12 05.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30273223

Curve tracing occurs when a line is followed covertly to accomplish a task, for example, to determine whether two landmarks are on the same line or not (could Highway 61 take Robert Johnson from New Orleans to St Louis?). Previous work suggests that attention either moves along the curve, momentarily activating local representations of the curve during this process, leaving little or no trace of this activation once attention has passed, or attention spreads along the curve, resulting in an activated state along the entire portion of the curve that was traced. We re-examined this issue using event-related potentials. Curves to be traced were presented briefly to encourage a rapid deployment of attention. The curves started on the vertical midline and passed into the left or right visual field and terminated either on the vertical midline or at a lateral position. We measured a posterior contralateral negativity (relative to the visual field of the traced curve) that offset more rapidly when the curve was traced back to the midline than when it remained lateral. The results suggest that attention travels along the curve like fire on a fuse, with activation returning to baseline once the flame has passed.


Attention/physiology , Brain/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials , Humans , Photic Stimulation
17.
Neuropsychology ; 32(4): 417-435, 2018 05.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29809032

OBJECTIVE: Semantic memory impairment has been documented in individuals with amnestic Mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), who are at risk of developing Alzheimer's disease (AD), yet little is known about the neural basis of this breakdown. The aim of this study was to investigate the brain mechanisms associated with semantic performance in aMCI patients. METHOD: A group of aMCI patients and a group of healthy controls carried out a semantic categorization task while their brain activity was recorded using magnetoencephalography (MEG). During the task, participants were shown famous faces and had to determine whether each famous person matched a given occupation. The main hypotheses were that (a) semantic processing should be compromised for aMCI patients, and (b) these deficits should be associated with cortical dysfunctions within specific areas of the semantic network. RESULTS: Behavioral results showed that aMCI participants were significantly slower and less accurate than controls at the semantic task. Additionally, relative to controls, a significant pattern of hyperactivation was found in the aMCI group within specific regions of the extended semantic network, including the right anterior temporal lobe (ATL) and fusiform gyrus. CONCLUSIONS: Abnormal functional activation within key areas of the semantic network suggests that it is compromised early in the disease process. Moreover, this pattern of right ATL and fusiform gyrus hyperactivation was positively associated with gray matter integrity in specific areas, but was not associated with any pattern of atrophy, suggesting that this pattern of hyperactivation may precede structural alteration of the semantic network in aMCI. (PsycINFO Database Record


Amnesia/diagnostic imaging , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Cognitive Dysfunction/diagnostic imaging , Nerve Net/diagnostic imaging , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Amnesia/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Brain Mapping , Cognitive Dysfunction/physiopathology , Female , Gray Matter/diagnostic imaging , Gray Matter/physiopathology , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Memory/physiology , Nerve Net/physiopathology , Neuropsychological Tests
18.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 127: 52-61, 2018 05.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29524444

To improve our understanding of the mechanisms of target selection, we examined how the spatial separation of salient items and their similarity to a pre-defined target interact using lateralised electrophysiological correlates of visual spatial attention (N2pc component) and visual short-term memory (VSTM; SPCN component). Using these features of target selection, we sought to expand on previous work proposing a model of early and late selection, where the N2pc is suggested to reflect the selection probability of visual stimuli (Aubin and Jolicoeur, 2016). The authors suggested that early-selection processes could be enhanced when items are adjacent. In the present work, the stimuli were short oriented lines, all of which were grey except for two that were blue and hence salient. A decrease in N2pc amplitude with decreasing spatial separation between salient items was observed. The N2pc increased in amplitude with increasing similarity of salient distractors to the target template, but only in target-absent trials. There was no interaction between these two factors, suggesting that separable attentional mechanisms influenced the N2pc. The findings suggest that selection is initially based on easily-distinguished attributes (i.e., both blue items) followed by a later identification-based process (if necessary), which depends on feature similarity to a target template. For the SPCN component, the results were in line with previous work: for target-present trials, an increase in similarity of salient distractors was associated with an increase in SPCN amplitude, suggesting more information was maintained in VSTM. In sum, results suggest there is a need for further inspection of salient distractors when they are similar to the target, increasing the need for focal attention, demonstrated by an increase in N2pc amplitude, followed by a higher probability of transfer to VSTM, demonstrated by an increase in SPCN amplitude.


Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Choice Behavior/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
19.
Psychol Res ; 82(2): 255-271, 2018 Mar.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27778123

We hypothesize that a shared spatial attention mechanism is used for both perception and action. To this end we created a new dual-task version of the classical Simon task. In one task, the spatial-input task, associated with input spatial attention, participants named one shape out of two bilaterally presented colored shapes. In a second task, the spatial-output task, associated with output spatial attention, participants discriminated between high and low pitch tones by pressing either a left or a right key. In Experiment 1, input for both tasks appeared simultaneously, and participants were instructed not to prioritize either task. A between tasks Simon-like effect was found for responses to both tasks. Reaction times were shorter when the side of the relevant shape in the spatial-input task and the side of the correct response in the spatial-output task were congruent. In Experiment 2, we manipulated the stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) between the inputs for the two tasks and showed that the Simon-like effect remained intact at all SOAs. Experiment 3 was similar to Experiment 1 except that the vocal response for the spatial-input task was not speeded. A Simon-like effect was still observed. Experiment 4 was the same as Experiment 3 except that the non-speeded response for the spatial-input task was manual rather than vocal. No Simon-like effect was observed in this experiment. Our results support a shared spatial attention mechanism involved in the Simon effect and indicate that this spatial attention mechanism is shared by perception and action.


Attention/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception , Visual Perception , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
20.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 25(5): 1840-1846, 2018 Oct.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29110247

It is well established that processes of perception and action interact. A key question concerns the role of attention in the interaction between perception-action processes. We tested the hypothesis that spatial attention is shared by perception and action. We created a dual-task paradigm: In one task, spatial information is relevant for perception (spatial-input task) but not for action, and in a second task, spatial information is relevant for action (spatial-output task) but not for perception. We used endogenous pre-cueing, with two between-subjects conditions: In one condition the cue was predictive only for the target location in the spatial-input task; in a second condition the cue was predictive only for the location of the response in the spatial-output task. In both conditions, the cueing equally affected both tasks, regardless of the information conveyed by the cue. This finding directly supports the shared input-output attention hypothesis.


Attention/physiology , Cues , Space Perception/physiology , Humans , Reaction Time/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
...