Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 36
Filtrar
1.
Teach Learn Med ; 34(2): 178-186, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34348522

RESUMO

TheoryIn Medicine, arriving at the correct diagnosis is of paramount importance for patient health and safety, yet is a difficult task especially when a patient presents with symptoms that do not fit typical patterns of disease. This task can be further complicated by errors of judgment, with the failure to consider all possible diagnoses being the most common of such errors. In this study, we investigated the process of differential diagnosis generation within the growing evidence that diagnostic performance can be increased by activities such as walking as was previously shown in Oppezzo and Schwartz's 2014 study. Hypotheses: It was hypothesized that an increase in performance, as expressed by a greater number of plausible differential diagnoses, would be seen in the walking group. Method: Eighteen medical students in their last two months of pre-clerkship training and eighteen second year family medicine residents were shown four different lists of a constellation of signs and symptoms. Participants were asked to generate differential diagnoses over five minutes per each list. All participants sat when completing the first two lists (pretest phase), and then were equally and randomly assigned to sitting versus walking on a treadmill when completing the last two lists (post-test phase). The number of total and unique differential diagnoses generated was determined, before being submitted to a three-member expert panel who identified appropriate unique differential diagnoses. Results: Two-way mixed ANOVAs were conducted to investigate the impact of exercise on the number of total, unique, and appropriate unique ideas generated and compared between pretest and post-test phases. Conclusions: We conclude that there is neither an increase nor a decrease in the number or quality of differential diagnoses generated by the sitting and walking groups within a population that has acquired some level of expertise.


Assuntos
Estudantes de Medicina , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Humanos , Julgamento , Caminhada
2.
Br J Educ Psychol ; 92(2): e12455, 2022 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34427320

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: To date, studies that have investigated the bonds between students and their institution have emphasized the importance of student-staff relationships. Measuring the quality of those relationships (i.e., relationship quality) appears to help with investigating the relational ties students have with their higher education institutions. Growing interest has arisen in further investigating relationship quality in higher education, as it might predict students' involvement with the institution (e.g., student engagement and student loyalty). So far, most studies have used a cross-sectional design, so that causality could not be determined. AIMS: The aim of this longitudinal study was twofold. First, we investigated the temporal ordering of the relation between the relationship quality dimensions of trust (in benevolence and honesty) and affect (satisfaction, affective commitment, and affective conflict). Second, we examined the ordering of the paths between relationship quality, student engagement, and student loyalty. Our objectives were to gain a deeper understanding of the relationship quality construct in higher education and its later outcomes. SAMPLE: Participants (N = 1649) were students from three Dutch higher education institutions who were studying in a technology economics or social sciences program. METHODS: Longitudinal data from two time points were used to evaluate two types of cross-lagged panel models. In the first analysis, we could not assume measurement invariance for affective conflict over time. Therefore, we tested an alternative model without affective conflict, using the latent variables of trust and affect, the student engagement dimensions and student loyalty. In the second type of model, we investigated the manifest variables of relationship quality, student engagement, and student loyalty. The hypotheses were tested by evaluating simultaneous comparisons between estimates. RESULTS: Results indicated that the relation between relationship quality at Time 1 with student engagement and loyalty at Time 2 was stronger than the reverse ordering in the first model. In the second model, results indicated that cross-lagged relations between trust in benevolence and trust in honesty at Time 1 and affective commitment, affective conflict, and satisfaction at Time 2 were more likely than the reverse ordering. Furthermore, cross-lagged relations from relationship quality at Time 1 to student engagement and student loyalty at Time 2 also supported our hypothesis. CONCLUSIONS: This study contributes to the existing higher education literature, indicating that students' trust in the quality of their relationship with faculty/staff is essential for developing students' affective commitment and satisfaction and for avoiding conflict over time. Second, relationship quality factors positively influence students' engagement in their studies and their loyalty towards the institution. A relational approach to establishing (long-lasting) bonds with students appears to be fruitful as an approach for educational psychologists and for practitioners' guidance and strategies. Recommendations are made for future research to further examine relationship quality in higher education in Europe and beyond.


Assuntos
Instituições Acadêmicas , Estudantes , Estudos Transversais , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Satisfação Pessoal , Estudantes/psicologia
3.
Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract ; 26(2): 405-416, 2021 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32986221

RESUMO

The use of walking workstations in educational and work settings has been shown to improve cognitive abilities. At the same time, it has been repeatedly shown that medical residents around the world do not meet exercise guidelines, mainly due to a scarcity of available free time. Our study investigates the boundaries of the previously observed phenomenon of improved cognitive performance with physical activity using materials that represent real life tasks. Participants had different level of expertise and involved second year psychology students, medical students, and family medicine residents. We examined the effect of being physically inactive (i.e., sitting) or active (i.e., walking) while diagnosing multiple complex presentations of four skin conditions. We assumed that being physically active, irrespective of the level of expertise, will bolster diagnostic performance. Our findings show, however, that being physically active does not change the performance level of participants with different levels of medical expertise. Implications for medical education and suggestions for further research will be discussed.


Assuntos
Educação Médica , Estudantes de Medicina , Humanos , Caminhada
4.
BMC Psychol ; 4(1): 25, 2016 May 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27240421

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The testing effect is the finding that information that is retrieved during learning is more often correctly retrieved on a final test than information that is restudied. According to the semantic mediator hypothesis the testing effect arises because retrieval practice of cue-target pairs (mother-child) activates semantically related mediators (father) more than restudying. Hence, the mediator-target (father-child) association should be stronger for retrieved than restudied pairs. Indeed, Carpenter (2011) found a larger testing effect when participants received mediators (father) than when they received target-related words (birth) as final test cues. METHODS: The present study started as an attempt to test an alternative account of Carpenter's results. However, it turned into a series of conceptual (Experiment 1) and direct (Experiment 2 and 3) replications conducted with online samples. The results of these online replications were compared with those of similar existing laboratory experiments through small-scale meta-analyses. RESULTS: The results showed that (1) the magnitude of the raw mediator testing effect advantage is comparable for online and laboratory experiments, (2) in both online and laboratory experiments the magnitude of the raw mediator testing effect advantage is smaller than in Carpenter's original experiment, and (3) the testing effect for related cues varies considerably between online experiments. CONCLUSIONS: The variability in the testing effect for related cues in online experiments could point toward moderators of the related cue short-term testing effect. The raw mediator testing effect advantage is smaller than in Carpenter's original experiment.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação , Sinais (Psicologia) , Rememoração Mental , Prática Psicológica , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Retenção Psicológica , Semântica
5.
Med Teach ; 38(6): 585-93, 2016 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26313552

RESUMO

CONTENT: Medical schools aim to graduate medical doctors who are able to self-regulate their learning. It is therefore important to investigate whether medical students' self-regulated learning skills change during medical school. In addition, since these skills are expected to be helpful to learn more effectively, it is of interest to investigate whether these skills are related to academic performance. METHODS: In a cross-sectional design, the Self-Regulation of Learning Self-Report Scale (SRL-SRS) was used to investigate the change in students' self-regulated learning skills. First and third-year students (N = 949, 81.7%) SRL-SRS scores were compared with ANOVA. The relation with academic performance was investigated with multinomial regression analysis. RESULTS: Only one of the six skills, reflection, significantly, but positively, changed during medical school. In addition, a small, but positive relation of monitoring, reflection, and effort with first-year GPA was found, while only effort was related to third-year GPA. CONCLUSIONS: The change in self-regulated learning skills is minor as only the level of reflection differs between the first and third year. In addition, the relation between self-regulated learning skills and academic performance is limited. Medical schools are therefore encouraged to re-examine the curriculum and methods they use to enhance their students' self-regulated learning skills. Future research is required to understand the limited impact on performance.


Assuntos
Logro , Educação Médica/organização & administração , Aprendizagem , Autocontrole , Adolescente , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Autoeficácia , Adulto Jovem
6.
Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract ; 21(1): 51-61, 2016 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25935203

RESUMO

Medical schools all over the world select applicants using non-cognitive and cognitive criteria. The predictive value of these different types of selection criteria has however never been investigated within the same curriculum while using a control group. We therefore set up a study that enabled us to compare the academic performance of three different admission groups, all composed of school-leaver entry students, and all enrolled in the same Bachelor curriculum: students selected on non-cognitive criteria, students selected on cognitive criteria and students admitted by lottery. First-year GPA and number of course credits (ECTS) at 52 weeks after enrollment of non-cognitive selected students (N = 102), cognitive selected students (N = 92) and lottery-admitted students (N = 356) were analyzed. In addition, chances of dropping out, probability of passing the third-year OSCE, and completing the Bachelor program in 3 years were compared. Although there were no significant differences between the admission groups in first-year GPA, cognitive selected students had obtained significantly more ECTS at 52 weeks and dropped out less often than lottery-admitted students. Probabilities of passing the OSCE and completing the bachelor program in 3 years did not significantly differ between the groups. These findings indicate that the use of only non-cognitive selection criteria is not sufficient to select the best academically performing students, most probably because a minimal cognitive basis is needed to succeed in medical school.


Assuntos
Avaliação Educacional , Critérios de Admissão Escolar , Faculdades de Medicina , Humanos , Países Baixos , Estudantes de Medicina/psicologia
7.
Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract ; 21(1): 93-104, 2016 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26018998

RESUMO

Society expects physicians to always improve their competencies and to be up to date with developments in their field. Therefore, an important aim of medical schools is to educate future medical doctors to become self-regulated, lifelong learners. However, it is unclear if medical students become better self-regulated learners during the pre-clinical stage of medical school, and whether students develop self-regulated learning skills differently, dependent on the educational approach of their medical school. In a cross-sectional design, we investigated the development of 384 medical students' self-regulated learning skills with the use of the Self-Regulation of Learning Self-Report Scale. Next, we compared this development in students who enrolled in two distinct medical curricula: a problem-based curriculum and a lectured-based curriculum. Analysis showed that more skills decreased than increased during the pre-clinical stage of medical school, and that the difference between the curricula was mainly caused by a decrease in the skill evaluation in the lecture-based curriculum. These findings seem to suggest that, irrespective of the curriculum, self-regulated learning skills do not develop during medical school.


Assuntos
Currículo , Educação de Graduação em Medicina , Aprendizagem , Aprendizagem Baseada em Problemas , Estudantes de Medicina , Adolescente , Adulto , Brasil , Competência Clínica , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
8.
Acad Med ; 89(2): 285-91, 2014 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24362387

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Anecdotal evidence indicates that exposure to media-distributed disease information, such as news about an outbreak, can lead physicians to errors; influenced by an availability bias, they misdiagnose patients with similar-looking but different diseases. The authors investigated whether exposure to media-provided disease information causes diagnostic errors and whether reflection (systematic review of findings) counteracts bias. METHOD: In 2010, 38 internal medicine residents first read the Wikipedia entry about one or another of two diseases (Phase 1). Six hours later, in a seemingly unrelated study, they diagnosed eight clinical cases (Phase 2). Two cases superficially resembled the disease in the Wikipedia entry they had read (bias expected), two cases resembled the other disease they had not read about (bias not expected), and four were filler cases. In Phase 3, they diagnosed the bias-expected cases again, using reflective reasoning. RESULTS: Mean diagnostic accuracy scores (Phase 2; range: 0-1) were significantly lower on bias-expected cases than on bias-not-expected cases (0.56 versus 0.70, P = .016) because participants misdiagnosed cases that looked similar to a Wikipedia description of a disease more often when they had read the Wikipedia description (mean = 0.61) than when they had not (mean = 0.29). Deliberate reflection (Phase 3) restored performance on bias-expected cases to pre-bias levels (mean = 0.71). CONCLUSIONS: Availability bias may arise simply from exposure to media-provided information about a disease, causing diagnostic errors. The bias's effect can be substantial. It is apparently associated with nonanalytical reasoning and can be counteracted by reflection.


Assuntos
Erros de Diagnóstico , Internato e Residência , Médicos/psicologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Priming de Repetição , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
9.
Med Educ ; 47(11): 1109-16, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24117557

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: General guidelines for teaching clinical reasoning have received much attention, despite a paucity of instructional approaches with demonstrated effectiveness. As suggested in a recent experimental study, self-explanation while solving clinical cases may be an effective strategy to foster reasoning in clinical clerks dealing with less familiar cases. However, the mechanisms that mediate this benefit have not been specifically investigated. The aim of this study was to explore the types of knowledge used by students when solving familiar and less familiar clinical cases with self-explanation. METHODS: In a previous study, 36 third-year medical students diagnosed familiar and less familiar clinical cases either by engaging in self-explanation or not. Based on an analysis of previously collected data, the present study compared the content of self-explanation protocols generated by seven randomly selected students while solving four familiar and four less familiar cases. In total, 56 verbal protocols (28 familiar and 28 less familiar) were segmented and coded using the following categories: paraphrases, biomedical inferences, clinical inferences, monitoring statements and errors. RESULTS: Students provided more self-explanation segments from less familiar cases (M = 275.29) than from familiar cases (M = 248.71, p = 0.046). They provided significantly more paraphrases (p = 0.001) and made more errors (p = 0.008). A significant interaction was found between familiarity and the type of inferences (biomedical versus clinical, p = 0.016). When self-explaining less familiar cases, students provided significantly more biomedical inferences than familiar cases. CONCLUSIONS: Lack of familiarity with a case seems to stimulate medical students to engage in more extensive thinking during self-explanation. Less familiar cases seem to activate students' biomedical knowledge, which in turn helps them to create new links between biomedical and clinical knowledge, and eventually construct a more coherent mental representation of diseases. This may clarify the previously found positive effect that self-explanation has on the diagnosis of unfamiliar cases.


Assuntos
Educação de Graduação em Medicina/métodos , Conhecimento , Aprendizagem Baseada em Problemas/métodos , Estudantes de Medicina/psicologia , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Resolução de Problemas
10.
BMC Med Educ ; 13: 55, 2013 Apr 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23594455

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Learning physical examination skills is an essential element of medical education. Teaching strategies include practicing the skills either alone or in-group. It is unclear whether students benefit more from training these skills individually or in a group, as the latter allows them to observing their peers. The present study, conducted in a naturalistic setting, investigated the effects of peer observation on mastering psychomotor skills necessary for physical examination. METHODS: The study included 185 2nd-year medical students, participating in a regular head-to-toe physical examination learning activity. Students were assigned either to a single-student condition (n = 65), in which participants practiced alone with a patient instructor, or to a multiple-student condition (n = 120), in which participants practiced in triads under patient instructor supervision. The students subsequently carried out a complete examination that was videotaped and subsequently evaluated. Student's performance was used as a measure of learning. RESULTS: Students in the multiple-student condition learned more than those who practiced alone (81% vs 76%, p < 0.004). This result possibly derived from a positive effect of observing peers; students who had the possibility to observe a peer (the second and third students in the groups) performed better than students who did not have this possibility (84% vs 76%, p <. 001). There was no advantage of observing more than one peer (83.7% vs 84.1%, p > .05). CONCLUSIONS: The opportunity to observe a peer during practice seemed to improve the acquisition of physical examination skills. By using small groups instead of individual training to teach physical examination skills, health sciences educational programs may provide students with opportunities to improve their performance by learning from their peers through modelling.


Assuntos
Educação de Graduação em Medicina/métodos , Exame Físico , Competência Clínica/normas , Educação de Graduação em Medicina/normas , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Grupo Associado , Exame Físico/métodos , Exame Físico/normas , Estudantes de Medicina , Adulto Jovem
11.
Med Educ ; 46(5): 464-72, 2012 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22515754

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Developing diagnostic competence in students is a major goal of medical education, but there is little empirical evidence on instructional strategies that foster the acquisition of this competence. The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of structured reflection compared with the generation of immediate or differential diagnosis while practising with clinical cases on learning clinical diagnosis. METHODS: This was a three-phase experimental study. During a learning phase, 46 Year 4 students diagnosed six clinical cases under different experimental conditions: structured reflection, immediate diagnosis, or differential diagnosis. This was followed by an immediate test and a delayed test administered 1 week later. Each test consisted of diagnosing four different cases of diseases presented in the learning phase. Performance in diagnosing these new cases was used as a measure of learning. RESULTS: Repeated-measures analysis of variance on the mean diagnostic accuracy scores (range: 0-1) showed a significant interaction between performance moment (i.e. performance in the learning phase and on each test) and instructions followed during the learning phase (p=0.003). Follow-up analyses of this interaction showed that diagnostic performance did not differ between conditions in the learning phase. On the immediate test, scores in the reflection condition (mean=0.48, 95% confidence interval [CI] 0.38-0.58) were significantly lower than scores in the differential diagnosis condition (mean=0.62, 95% CI 0.54-0.70; p=0.012) and marginally lower than those in the immediate diagnosis condition (mean=0.61, 95% CI 0.52-0.70; p=0.04). One week later, however, scores in the reflection condition (mean=0.66, 95% CI 0.56-0.76) significantly outperformed those in the other conditions (differential diagnosis: mean=0.48, 95% CI 0.37-0.58 [p<0.01]; immediate diagnosis: mean=0.52, 95% CI 0.43-0.60 [p=0.01]). Comparisons within experimental conditions showed that performance from the immediate to the delayed test decreased in the immediate and differential diagnosis conditions (immediate diagnosis: p=0.042; differential diagnosis: p=0.012), but increased in the reflection condition (p=0.003). CONCLUSIONS: Structured reflection while practising with cases appears to foster the learning of clinical knowledge more effectively than the generation of immediate or differential diagnoses and therefore seems to be an effective instructional approach to developing diagnostic competence in students.


Assuntos
Competência Clínica/normas , Técnicas e Procedimentos Diagnósticos , Educação de Graduação em Medicina/métodos , Avaliação Educacional/métodos , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
12.
Teach Learn Med ; 24(2): 149-54, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22490096

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Psychological research has shown that people tend toward accepting rather than refuting hypotheses. Diagnostic suggestions may evoke such confirmatory tendencies in physicians, which may lead to diagnostic errors. PURPOSE: This study investigated the influence of a suggested diagnosis on physicians' diagnostic decisions on written clinical cases. It was hypothesized that physicians would tend to go along with the suggestions and therefore would have more difficulty rejecting incorrect suggestions than accepting correct suggestions. METHODS: Residents (N = 24) had to accept or reject suggested diagnoses on 6 cases. Three of those suggested diagnoses were correct, and 3 were incorrect. RESULTS: Results showed the mean correct evaluation score on cases with a correct suggested diagnosis (M = 2.21, SD = 0.88) was significantly higher than the score on cases with an incorrect suggested diagnosis (M = 1.42, SD = 0.97), meaning physicians indeed found it easier to accept correct diagnoses than to reject incorrect diagnoses, t(23) = 2.74, p < .05, d = .85, despite equal experience with the diagnoses. CONCLUSION: These findings indicate that suggested diagnoses may evoke confirmatory tendencies and consequently may lead to diagnostic errors.


Assuntos
Erros de Diagnóstico , Internato e Residência , Sugestão , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Feminino , Humanos , Medicina Interna , Masculino , Países Baixos
13.
J Contin Educ Health Prof ; 32(1): 4-9, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22447706

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Illness script theory offers explanations for expert-novice differences in clinical reasoning. However, it has mainly focused on diagnostic (Dx) performance, while patient management (Mx) has been largely ignored. The aim of the present study was to show the role of Mx knowledge in illness script development and how it relates to diagnostic knowledge in the course of development toward expertise. METHODS: The participants were 10 fourth-year and 10 sixth-year medical students, and 10 experienced physicians (ie, internists). Participants were asked to study 4 written clinical cases and provide management plans. Based on propositional analysis the management plans were examined for accuracy and elaborateness as well as the number of Mx and Dx items. RESULTS: Providing accurate Mx plans that concurrently pay attention to Dx and Mx was a characteristic of doctors' performance. The Mx plans of sixth-year students were as accurate as fourth-year students, but the format and the size of sixth-year students' plans was more similar to those of doctors. While sixth-year students generated plans with an Mx focus, the plans of fourth-year students were characterized by a Dx focus. DISCUSSION: The experienced physicians' accurate management plans are characterized by a high number of the Mx and Dx items. For sixth-year students the management plans are still incomplete, which leads to generic as well as inaccurate Mx orders. For fourth-year students, the Mx focus is lacking, and hence they seem to treat an Mx task as if it were a Dx task.


Assuntos
Competência Clínica , Técnicas e Procedimentos Diagnósticos/normas , Administração dos Cuidados ao Paciente/normas , Médicos/psicologia , Avaliação de Processos em Cuidados de Saúde/métodos , Estudantes de Medicina/psicologia , Tomada de Decisões , Prestação Integrada de Cuidados de Saúde , Educação Médica Continuada , Humanos , Irã (Geográfico) , Médicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Padrões de Prática Médica , Estudantes de Medicina/estatística & dados numéricos , Ensino/métodos
14.
BMJ Qual Saf ; 21(4): 295-300, 2012 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22389021

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Flaws in clinical reasoning are present in most diagnostic errors and occur even when physicians have enough knowledge to solve the problem. Deliberate reflection has been shown to improve diagnoses. The sources of faulty reasoning and how reflection counteracts them remain largely unknown. OBJECTIVE: To explore the causes of faulty reasoning and the mechanisms through which reflection neutralises them by investigating the influence of salient distracting clinical features on diagnostic decision-making. DESIGN AND SETTING: In a prior study, 34 internal medicine residents and 50 medical students of the Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, diagnosed four clinical cases by means of non-analytical reasoning and four by reflective reasoning. In the secondary analysis of the data presented here, five internists independently evaluated the diagnoses and examined the nature of the diagnostic errors in relation to case features that gave rise to these errors. MAIN OUTCOMES: Frequency of incorrect diagnoses caused by salient distracting features made through reflective and non-analytical reasoning. RESULTS: Among residents, reflective reasoning (Mean diagnostic accuracy score (M)=2.09, 95% CI 1.77 to 2.40) led to a significantly higher number of correct diagnoses than non-analytical reasoning (M=1.71, 95% CI 1.37 to 2.04; p=0.03). This higher diagnostic accuracy was associated with fewer incorrect diagnoses triggered by salient distracting clinical features (M=0.47, 95% CI 0.26 to 0.68) compared with non-analytical reasoning (M=0.85, 95% CI 0.59 to 1.11; p=0.02). Students did not benefit from reflection to improve diagnoses. CONCLUSION: Salient features in a case tend to attract physicians' attention and may misdirect diagnostic reasoning when they turn out to be unrelated to the problem, causing errors. Reflection helps by enabling physicians to overcome the influence of distracting features. The lack of effect for students suggests that this is only possible when there is enough knowledge to recognise which features discriminate between alternative diagnoses.


Assuntos
Competência Clínica , Tomada de Decisões , Erros de Diagnóstico , Humanos , Medicina Interna/educação
15.
Educ Health (Abingdon) ; 25(3): 153-9, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23823634

RESUMO

CONTEXT: Studies in medical expertise have shown that the medical knowledge of physicians is organized in a way that is easily retrievable when they encounter patients. These knowledge structures, called illness scripts, contain various pieces of information, including signs, symptoms, and enabling conditions, concerning a given disease. Illness script research has principally focused on understanding how physicians make diagnoses, while patient management has received much less attention. Although the work on diagnostics has taught us many things about the nature of medical expertise, focusing solely on this aspect provides only a narrow perspective on the subject, resulting in an incomplete depiction of medical experts. The goal of the present study was to experimentally determine how management knowledge plays a role in the development of illness scripts and developing expertise. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Medical students, interns, and residents were instructed to think aloud while reading a case with either a diagnostic or management focus. The recall protocols were examined in terms of illness script components, as well as diagnostic and management accuracy. FINDINGS: Both residents and interns were sensitive to the focus and generated significantly more management-items when thinking about management than when they were asked to diagnose a clinical case. They also provided more management items than medical students in management-focus. The performance of interns was superficially similar to that of residents in terms of management proportion, but with respect to both diagnostic and management accuracy they resembled medical students. Medical students, in contrast, were very persistent and insensitive to the focus. CONCLUSIONS: Medical expertise could be characterized by the emergence of illness scripts that are rich in terms of management knowledge. Illness scripts can generally be applied to any medical encounter that includes diagnosis and management, and expertise research should be extended to cover both domains.


Assuntos
Competência Clínica , Diagnóstico , Competência Clínica/normas , Educação Médica/normas , Humanos , Internato e Residência/normas , Estudantes de Medicina/psicologia
16.
JAMA ; 304(11): 1198-203, 2010 Sep 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20841533

RESUMO

CONTEXT: Diagnostic errors have been associated with bias in clinical reasoning. Empirical evidence on the cognitive mechanisms underlying biases and effectiveness of educational strategies to counteract them is lacking. OBJECTIVES: To investigate whether recent experience with clinical problems provokes availability bias (overestimation of the likelihood of a diagnosis based on the ease with which it comes to mind) resulting in diagnostic errors and whether reflection (structured reanalysis of the case findings) counteracts this bias. DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS: Experimental study conducted in 2009 at the Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, with 18 first-year and 18 second-year internal medicine residents. Participants first evaluated diagnoses of 6 clinical cases (phase 1). Subsequently, they diagnosed 8 different cases through nonanalytical reasoning, 4 of which had findings similar to previously evaluated cases but different diagnoses (phase 2). These 4 cases were subsequently diagnosed again through reflective reasoning (phase 3). MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: Mean diagnostic accuracy scores (perfect score, 4.0) on cases solved with or without previous exposure to similar problems through nonanalytical (phase 2) or reflective (phase 3) reasoning and frequency that a potentially biased (ie, phase 1) diagnosis was given. RESULTS: There were no main effects, but there was a significant interaction effect between "years of training" and "recent experiences with similar problems." Results consistent with an availability bias occurred for the second-year residents, who scored lower on the cases similar to those previously encountered (1.55; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.15-1.96) than on the other cases (2.19; 95% CI, 1.73-2.66; P =.03). This pattern was not seen among the first-year residents (2.03; 95% CI, 1.55-2.51 vs 1.42; 95% CI, 0.92-1.92; P =.046). Second-year residents provided the phase 1 diagnosis more frequently for phase 2 cases they had previously encountered than for those they had not (mean frequency per resident, 1.44; 95% CI, 0.93-1.96 vs 0.72; 95% CI, 0.28-1.17; P =.04). A significant main effect of reasoning mode was found: reflection improved the diagnoses of the similar cases compared with nonanalytical reasoning for the second-year residents (2.03; 95% CI, 1.49-2.57) and the first-year residents (2.31; 95% CI, 1.89-2.73; P =.006). CONCLUSION: When faced with cases similar to previous ones and using nonanalytic reasoning, second-year residents made errors consistent with the availability bias. Subsequent application of diagnostic reflection tended to counter this bias; it improved diagnostic accuracy in both first- and second-year residents.


Assuntos
Cognição , Erros de Diagnóstico , Medicina Interna/educação , Internato e Residência , Testes Diagnósticos de Rotina , Humanos , Países Baixos , Variações Dependentes do Observador , Exame Físico
17.
Psychol Res ; 74(6): 586-92, 2010 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20354726

RESUMO

Contrary to what common sense makes us believe, deliberation without attention has recently been suggested to produce better decisions in complex situations than deliberation with attention. Based on differences between cognitive processes of experts and novices, we hypothesized that experts make in fact better decisions after consciously thinking about complex problems whereas novices may benefit from deliberation-without-attention. These hypotheses were confirmed in a study among doctors and medical students. They diagnosed complex and routine problems under three conditions, an immediate-decision condition and two delayed conditions: conscious thought and deliberation-without-attention. Doctors did better with conscious deliberation when problems were complex, whereas reasoning mode did not matter in simple problems. In contrast, deliberation-without-attention improved novices' decisions, but only in simple problems. Experts benefit from consciously thinking about complex problems; for novices thinking does not help in those cases.


Assuntos
Atenção , Estado de Consciência , Competência Profissional , Pensamento , Erros de Diagnóstico , Humanos
18.
Br J Educ Psychol ; 79(Pt 3): 501-14, 2009 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19079953

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Constructivist views of learning have brought conceptions of learning to attention again. Conceptions are considered important determinants of effective learning. Students can differ in their conceptions depending on their educational experience. AIMS: The present study investigated students' conceptions of constructivist learning. Do students with greater experience in their academic programme differ in their conceptions of constructivist learning compared to students with less experience? In addition, to what extent are conceptions of constructivist learning different in a conventional, lecture-based curriculum compared to a constructivist, problem-based learning curriculum? SAMPLES: Three groups (i.e. first-year, second-year, and third-year students) in two different curricula (i.e. conventional, lecture-based and constructivist, problem-based) were tested. METHODS: A cross-sectional design was used. Students' conceptions of constructivist activities (i.e. knowledge construction, cooperative learning, self-regulation, use of authentic problems, self-perceived inability to learn, and motivation to learn) were measured by a questionnaire. Data were analyzed using a two-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). RESULTS: A significant difference in questionnaire's scores between year 1 and year 2 (but not between year 2 and 3) was found with respect to conceptions about knowledge construction, self-regulation, and the use of authentic problems, but not for cooperative learning and motivation to learn. For self-perceived inability, an interaction effect was found. Furthermore, results showed significant differences between both curriculum groups on all dependent measures. CONCLUSIONS: Differences in conceptions can be perceived between students who enter a new learning programme (i.e. higher education) and students who already have one year of experience in higher education. Among students with more than one year of educational experience, differences disappear. Furthermore, this study shows that the learning environment can make a difference with respect to students' conceptions of constructivist learning activities.


Assuntos
Atitude , Aprendizagem Baseada em Problemas , Estudantes/psicologia , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Estudos Transversais , Currículo , Feminino , Processos Grupais , Humanos , Masculino , Países Baixos , Resolução de Problemas , Psicologia/educação , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
19.
Br J Psychol ; 99(Pt 4): 473-97, 2008 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18433518

RESUMO

In this study, the longitudinal relation between deliberate practice and performance in chess was examined using a linear mixed models analysis. The practice activities and performance ratings of young elite chess players, who were either in, or had dropped out of the Dutch national chess training, were analysed since they had started playing chess seriously. The results revealed that deliberate practice (i.e. serious chess study alone and serious chess play) strongly contributed to chess performance. The influence of deliberate practice was not only observable in current performance, but also over chess players' careers. Moreover, although the drop-outs' chess ratings developed more slowly over time, both the persistent and drop-out chess players benefited to the same extent from investments in deliberate practice. Finally, the effect of gender on chess performance proved to be much smaller than the effect of deliberate practice. This study provides longitudinal support for the monotonic benefits assumption of deliberate practice, by showing that over chess players' careers, deliberate practice has a significant effect on performance, and to the same extent for chess players of different ultimate performance levels. The results of this study are not in line with critique raised against the deliberate practice theory that the factors deliberate practice and talent could be confounded.


Assuntos
Jogos Experimentais , Motivação , Prática Psicológica , Evasão Escolar/psicologia , Logro , Adolescente , Criança , Comportamento Competitivo , Feminino , Humanos , Modelos Lineares , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Países Baixos , Fatores Sexuais , Adulto Jovem
20.
Acad Med ; 83(12): 1210-6, 2008 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19202502

RESUMO

PURPOSE: While diagnosing problems, physicians frequently switch from nonanalytical to reflective reasoning. The conditions inducing doctors to reflect are largely unknown. The authors investigated whether a shift to reflection occurs when physicians perceive a case as problematic, and its effects on diagnostic accuracy. METHOD: The authors conducted two within-subjects experiments in Brazilian teaching hospitals in 2007. In Experiment 1, 20 medical residents diagnosed the same 10 clinical cases under two experimental conditions: a nonproblematic versus a problematic context. (The latter was created by informing participants that other physicians failed to diagnose the case previously.) In addition, participants judged whether a set of medical concepts were related to the case, and response time was measured. In Experiment 2, 18 residents diagnosed two cases while thinking aloud. The authors hypothesized that a case perceived as problematic would trigger reflection, leading to higher diagnostic accuracy, lower response times for recognizing concepts (Experiment 1), more time for diagnosing, and more elaborate think-aloud protocols (Experiment 2). RESULTS: Experiment 1: Accuracy of diagnosis was significantly higher within the problematic context, and participants were faster in deciding whether concepts were related to the case. The same cases were evaluated as more complex and less frequently seen. Experiment 2: Time spent on diagnosis, memory for case findings, and inferences derived from the cases were significantly higher within the problematic context. CONCLUSIONS: A context perceived as problematic induced reflection in the participating clinicians, as indicated by lower response times, more time spent on diagnosis, and more elaborate protocols. Reflective reasoning comprised more careful analysis of findings and alternative diagnoses, and increased diagnostic accuracy.


Assuntos
Competência Clínica , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Internato e Residência , Percepção Social , Pensamento , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Brasil , Competência Clínica/estatística & dados numéricos , Sistemas de Apoio a Decisões Clínicas , Feminino , Hospitais de Ensino/estatística & dados numéricos , Humanos , Internato e Residência/estatística & dados numéricos , Masculino
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA