Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 59.227
Filtrar
1.
J Pak Med Assoc ; 74(5 (Supple-5)): S8-S12, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39221789

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To assess the emotional development of school-aged children. METHODS: The descriptive, analytical study was carried out on children of elementary schools in the Semarang City area in January 2022. Included were children aged 6-12 years with active elementary school status. Data was collected using the 25-item Strength and Difficulties Rutter Questionnaire. The questionnaire was filled manually by the subject along with the guardian or teacher. Data was analysed using SPSS 20. RESULTS: Of the 326 children, 174(53.21%) were girls and 153(46.79%) were boys. Overall, 171(52.3%) subjects were aged <10 years, while 156(47.7%) were aged 10-12 years. There were 295(90.21%) children with normal prosocial behaviour, 206(63%) with normal emotional status, 264(80.73%) with normal conduct, 133(40.67%) with normal hyperactivity level, and 91(27.83%) with normal equation with peers. CONCLUSIONS: Different domains of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire showed varying degrees of pro-social behaviour, emotional status, conduct, hyperactivity level and peer interaction among the subjects.


Assuntos
Emoções , Saúde Mental , Humanos , Criança , Feminino , Masculino , Inquéritos e Questionários , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Comportamento Social , Paquistão , Grupo Associado , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia
2.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 153(9): 2239-2257, 2024 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39235888

RESUMO

Biases favoring the wealthy are ubiquitous, and they support and bolster vast resource inequalities across individuals and groups; yet, when these biases are acquired remains unknown. In Experiments 1 through 5 (Total N = 232), using multiple methods, we found that 14- to 18-month-old infants track individuals' wealth (Experiments 1-5), prefer and selectively help rich (vs. poor) individuals (Experiments 2 and 3), and negatively evaluate poor individuals (Experiments 4 and 5). In two subsequent experiments with 11- to 13-month-old infants (Total N = 65), however, we find no evidence of preferences for rich (vs. poor) individuals (Experiment 6) or differential evaluations of rich and poor people (Experiment 7). Together, these results demonstrate that in the second year of life, wealth emerges as a central and robust dimension of evaluation that guides social decision making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Comportamento Social , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Feminino , Tomada de Decisões , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Comportamento do Lactente/fisiologia , Comportamento do Lactente/psicologia
3.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 153(9): 2216-2229, 2024 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39235887

RESUMO

Third-party intervention is a cornerstone of cooperative societies, yet we know little about how children develop an understanding of this social behavior. The present work generates a cross-cultural and developmental picture of how 6-, 9-, and 12-year-olds (N = 447) across four societies (India, Germany, Uganda, and the United States) reason about third-party intervention. To do so, we measured children's obligation judgments and unstructured descriptions of third-party intervention. Although some cultural differences emerged, 6-year-olds largely considered bystanders as obligated to respond to wrongdoing, regardless of the bystander's social position. In contrast, 9- and 12-year-olds were more likely to exclusively ascribe this social responsibility to people in positions of authority. Despite these age differences, children of all ages generated role-specific descriptions of third-party intervention, with authority figures intervening in distinct ways from peers. For authority figures, children in India and Uganda described third-party intervention as involving corporal punishment or unspecified punishment, whereas children in the United States described such intervention as involving only verbal intervention (i.e., telling someone to stop). For peers, children in all societies described third-party intervention as involving reporting misdeeds to an authority. Collectively, these data show that early conceptualizations of third-party intervention are rooted in shared notions of obligation yet are also subject to cultural and contextual influences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Comparação Transcultural , Humanos , Criança , Masculino , Feminino , Uganda , Índia , Estados Unidos , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Alemanha , Comportamento Social , Punição , Comportamento Cooperativo
4.
Dev Psychobiol ; 66(7): e22545, 2024 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39236225

RESUMO

Temperamental characteristics and emerging cognitive control are meaningful predictors of children's development of adaptive and maladaptive social behaviors during the preschool period. However, knowledge of the interplay of these pathways, when examined concurrently to highlight their individual contributions, is limited. Using a cross-sectional sample of 3-year-old children, we examined parent-reported discrete traits of negative (anger, fear, sadness, and shyness) and positive (low- and high-intensity pleasure) temperamental reactivity as predictors of children's prosociality and physical aggression. Further, we tested whether the effects of discrete temperament were moderated by cognitive control, as indexed by the N2 event-related potential, during a go/no-go task. Analyses focus on a subsample of children with an observable N2 (n = 66). When controlling for other relative temperament traits, several significant main effects emerged. Moreover, at low cognitive control (smaller N2), fear was negatively associated with aggression, whereas at high cognitive control, sadness was positively associated with aggression. Heightened anger was linked to reduced prosocial behavior when cognitive control was low but linked to greater prosocial behavior when cognitive control was high. The results highlight that discrete temperament traits predict individual differences in child outcomes but that associations depend on concurrent levels of cognitive control.


Assuntos
Agressão , Comportamento Infantil , Comportamento Social , Temperamento , Humanos , Temperamento/fisiologia , Pré-Escolar , Masculino , Feminino , Comportamento Infantil/fisiologia , Agressão/fisiologia , Estudos Transversais , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Medo/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Ira/fisiologia , Timidez
6.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 45(13): e70017, 2024 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230055

RESUMO

Atypical social impairments (i.e., impaired social cognition and social communication) are vital manifestations of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) patients, and the incidence rate of ASD is significantly higher in males than in females. Characterizing the atypical brain patterns underlying social deficits of ASD is significant for understanding the pathogenesis. However, there are no robust imaging biomarkers that are specific to ASD, which may be due to neurobiological complexity and limitations of single-modality research. To describe the multimodal brain patterns related to social deficits in ASD, we highlighted the potential functional role of white matter (WM) and incorporated WM functional activity and gray matter structure into multimodal fusion. Gray matter volume (GMV) and fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations of WM (WM-fALFF) were combined by fusion analysis model adopting the social behavior. Our results revealed multimodal spatial patterns associated with Social Responsiveness Scale multiple scores in ASD. Specifically, GMV exhibited a consistent brain pattern, in which salience network and limbic system were commonly identified associated with all multiple social impairments. More divergent brain patterns in WM-fALFF were explored, suggesting that WM functional activity is more sensitive to ASD's complex social impairments. Moreover, brain regions related to social impairment may be potentially interconnected across modalities. Cross-site validation established the repeatability of our results. Our research findings contribute to understanding the neural mechanisms underlying social disorders in ASD and affirm the feasibility of identifying biomarkers from functional activity in WM.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Espectro Autista , Substância Cinzenta , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Imagem Multimodal , Substância Branca , Humanos , Transtorno do Espectro Autista/diagnóstico por imagem , Transtorno do Espectro Autista/fisiopatologia , Transtorno do Espectro Autista/patologia , Masculino , Substância Cinzenta/diagnóstico por imagem , Substância Cinzenta/patologia , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Substância Branca/diagnóstico por imagem , Substância Branca/patologia , Adolescente , Comportamento Social , Criança , Neuroimagem/métodos , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Encéfalo/patologia , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia
7.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 7742, 2024 Sep 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39231969

RESUMO

Social scientists from different disciplines have long argued that direct reciprocity plays an important role in regulating social interactions between unrelated individuals. Here, we examine whether 15-month-old infants (N = 160) already expect direct positive and negative reciprocity between strangers. In violation-of-expectation experiments, infants watch successive interactions between two strangers we refer to as agent1 and agent2. After agent1 acts positively toward agent2, infants are surprised if agent2 acts negatively toward agent1 in a new context. Similarly, after agent1 acts negatively toward agent2, infants are surprised if agent2 acts positively toward agent1 in a new context. Both responses are eliminated when agent2's actions are not knowingly directed at agent1. Additional results indicate that infants view it as acceptable for agent2 either to respond in kind to agent1 or to not engage with agent1 further. By 15 months of age, infants thus already expect a modicum of reciprocity between strangers: Initial positive or negative actions are expected to set broad limits on reciprocal actions. This research adds weight to long-standing claims that direct reciprocity helps regulate interactions between unrelated individuals and, as such, is likely to depend on psychological systems that have evolved to support reciprocal reasoning and behavior.


Assuntos
Interação Social , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Feminino , Comportamento Social
8.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220521, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230448

RESUMO

Human evolutionary ecology stands to benefit by integrating theory and methods developed in movement ecology, and in turn, to make contributions to the broader field of movement ecology by leveraging our species' distinct attributes. In this paper, we review data and evolutionary models suggesting that major changes in socio-spatial behaviour accompanied the evolution of language. To illustrate and explore these issues, we present a comparison of GPS measures of the socio-spatial behaviour of Hadza hunter-gatherers of northern Tanzania to those of olive baboons (Papio anubis), a comparatively small-brained primate that is also savanna-adapted. While standard spatial metrics show modest differences, measures of spatial diversity, landscape exploration and spatiotemporal displacement between individuals differ markedly. Groups of Hadza foragers rapidly accumulate a vast, diverse knowledge pool about places and things over the horizon, contrasting with the baboon's narrower and more homogeneous pool of ecological information. The larger and more complex socio-spatial world illustrated by the Hadza is one where heightened cognitive abilities for spatial and episodic memory, navigation, perspective taking and communication about things beyond the here and now all have clear value.This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Idioma , Papio anubis , Animais , Tanzânia , Humanos , Papio anubis/fisiologia , Papio anubis/psicologia , Comportamento Social , Comportamento Espacial
9.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220530, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230449

RESUMO

Social animals make behavioural decisions based on local habitat and conspecifics, as well as memorized past experience (i.e. 'familiarity') with habitat and conspecifics. Here, we develop a conceptual and empirical understanding of how spatial and social familiarity fit within the spatial-social interface-a novel framework integrating the spatial and social components of animal behaviour. We conducted a multi-scale analysis of the movements of GPS-collared plains bison (Bison bison, n = 66) residing in and around Yellowstone National Park, USA. We found that both spatial and social familiarity mediate how individuals respond to their spatial and social environments. For instance, individuals with high spatial familiarity rely on their own knowledge as opposed to their conspecifics, and individuals with high social familiarity rely more strongly on the movement of conspecifics to guide their own movement. We also found that fine-scale spatial and social phenotypes often scale up to broad-scale phenotypes. For instance, bison that select more strongly to align with their nearest neighbour have larger home ranges. By integrating spatial and social familiarity into the spatial-social interface, we demonstrate the utility of the interface for testing hypotheses, while also highlighting the pervasive importance of cognitive mechanisms in animal behaviour. This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Bison , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Comportamento Social , Animais , Bison/fisiologia , Feminino , Wyoming , Masculino , Ecossistema , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Comportamento de Retorno ao Território Vital
10.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220533, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230452

RESUMO

The spatial availability of social resources is speculated to structure animal movement decisions, but the effects of social resources on animal movements are difficult to identify because social resources are rarely measured. Here, we assessed whether varying availability of a key social resource-access to receptive mates-produces predictable changes in movement decisions among bighorn sheep in Nevada, the United States. We compared the probability that males made long-distance 'foray' movements, a critical driver of connectivity, across three ecoregions with varying temporal duration of a socially mediated factor, breeding season. We used a hidden Markov model to identify foray events and then quantified the effects of social covariates on the probability of foray using a discrete choice model. We found that males engaged in forays at higher rates when the breeding season was short, suggesting that males were most responsive to the social resource when its existence was short lived. During the breeding season, males altered their response to social covariates, relative to the non-breeding season, though patterns varied, and age was associated with increased foray probability. Our results suggest that animals respond to the temporal availability of social resources when making the long-distance movements that drive connectivity. This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Carneiro da Montanha , Animais , Carneiro da Montanha/fisiologia , Masculino , Nevada , Comportamento Social , Estações do Ano , Feminino , Comportamento Sexual Animal/fisiologia , Dinâmica Populacional , Movimento
11.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220531, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230446

RESUMO

Studying the spatial-social interface requires tools that distinguish between social and spatial drivers of interactions. Testing hypotheses about the factors determining animal interactions often involves comparing observed interactions with reference or 'null' models. One approach to accounting for spatial drivers of social interactions in reference models is randomizing animal movement paths to decouple spatial and social phenotypes while maintaining environmental effects on movements. Here, we update a reference model that detects social attraction above the effect of spatial constraints. We explore the use of our 'wrap-around' method and compare its performance to the previous approach using agent-based simulations. The wrap-around method provides reference models that are more similar to the original tracking data, while still distinguishing between social and spatial drivers. Furthermore, the wrap-around approach results in fewer false-positives than its predecessor, especially when animals do not return to one place each night but change movement foci, either locally or directionally. Finally, we show that interactions among GPS-tracked griffon vultures (Gyps fulvus) emerge from social attraction rather than from spatial constraints on their movements. We conclude by highlighting the biological situations in which the updated method might be most suitable for testing hypotheses about the underlying causes of social interactions. This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Comportamento Social , Animais , Falconiformes/fisiologia , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Modelos Biológicos , Movimento
12.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220532, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230447

RESUMO

Social and spatial structures of host populations play important roles in pathogen transmission. For environmentally transmitted pathogens, the host space use interacts with both the host social structure and the pathogen's environmental persistence (which determines the time-lag across which two hosts can transmit). Together, these factors shape the epidemiological dynamics of environmentally transmitted pathogens. While the importance of both social and spatial structures and environmental pathogen persistence has long been recognized in epidemiology, they are often considered separately. A better understanding of how these factors interact to determine disease dynamics is required for developing robust surveillance and management strategies. Here, we use a simple agent-based model where we vary host mobility (spatial), host gregariousness (social) and pathogen decay (environmental persistence), each from low to high levels to uncover how they affect epidemiological dynamics. By comparing epidemic peak, time to epidemic peak and final epidemic size, we show that longer infectious periods, higher group mobility, larger group size and longer pathogen persistence lead to larger, faster growing outbreaks, and explore how these processes interact to determine epidemiological outcomes such as the epidemic peak and the final epidemic size. We identify general principles that can be used for planning surveillance and control for wildlife host-pathogen systems with environmental transmission across a range of spatial behaviour, social structure and pathogen decay rates. This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Animais Selvagens , Animais , Comportamento Social , Modelos Biológicos , Interações Hospedeiro-Patógeno
13.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220534, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230454

RESUMO

All animals exhibit some combination of spatial and social behaviours. A diversity of interactions occurs between such behaviours, producing emergent phenomena at the spatial-social interface. Untangling and interrogating these complex, intertwined processes can be vital for identifying the mechanisms, causes and consequences of behavioural variation in animal ecology. Nevertheless, the integrated study of the interactions between spatial and social phenotypes and environments (at the spatial-social interface) is in its relative infancy. In this theme issue, we present a collection of papers chosen to expand the spatial-social interface along several theoretical, methodological and empirical dimensions. They detail new perspectives, methods, study systems and more, as well as offering roadmaps for applied outputs and detailing exciting new directions for the field to move in the future. In this Introduction, we outline the contents of these papers, placing them in the context of what comes before, and we synthesize a number of takeaways and future directions for the spatial-social interface. This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Comportamento Social , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Comportamento Espacial
14.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220523, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230455

RESUMO

Animals simultaneously navigate spatial and social environments, and their decision-making with respect to those environments constitutes their spatial (e.g. habitat selection) and social (e.g. conspecific associations) phenotypes. The spatial-social interface is a recently introduced conceptual framework linking these components of spatial and social ecology. The spatial-social interface is inherently scale-dependent, yet it has not been integrated with the rich body of literature on ecological scale. Here, we develop a conceptual connection between the spatial-social interface and ecological scale. We propose three key innovations that incrementally build upon each other. First, the use-availability framework that underpins a large body of literature in behavioural ecology can be used in analogy to the phenotype-environment nomenclature and is transferable across the spatial and social realms. Second, both spatial and social phenotypes are hierarchical, with nested components that are linked via constraints-from the top down-or emergent properties-from the bottom up. Finally, in both the spatial and social realms, the definitions of environment and phenotype depend on the focal scale of inquiry. These conceptual innovations cast our understanding of the relationships between social and spatial dimensions of animal ecology in a new light, allowing a more holistic understanding and clearer hypothesis development for animal behaviour. This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Ecossistema , Comportamento Social , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Fenótipo , Ecologia/métodos , Meio Social
15.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220528, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230456

RESUMO

How animals move and associate with conspecifics is rarely random, with a population's spatial structure forming the foundation on which the social behaviours of individuals form. Studies examining the spatial-social interface typically measure averaged behavioural differences between individuals; however, this neglects the inherent variation present within individuals and how it may impact the spatial-social interface. Here, we investigated differences in among-individual (co)variance in sociability, activity and site fidelity in a population of wild estuarine crocodiles, Crocodylus porosus, across a 10-year period. By monitoring 118 crocodiles using coded acoustic transmitters and an array of fixed underwater receivers, we discovered that not only did individual crocodiles repeatably differ (among-individual variation) in each behaviour measured but also in how consistently they expressed these behaviours through time (within-individual variation). As expected, crocodile activity and sociability formed a behavioural syndrome, with more active individuals being less sociable. Interestingly, we also found that individuals that were either more sociable or displayed greater site fidelity were also more specialized (lower within-individual variation) in these behaviours. Together, our results provide important empirical evidence for the interplay between spatial, temporal and social individual-level behavioural variation and how these contribute to forming behavioural niches. This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Jacarés e Crocodilos , Comportamento Social , Animais , Jacarés e Crocodilos/fisiologia , Comportamento Animal
16.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220525, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230451

RESUMO

Site fidelity-the tendency to reuse familiar spaces-is expected to improve fitness. Familiarity with the local environment is particularly crucial when resource demands or predation risk are high. Consequently, site fidelity often peaks during reproduction when energetic costs are high and offspring are vulnerable. For many species, the environment they experience is not solely a function of geography but also of the social environment. Social fidelity, the selection for familiar social environments, could constitute an independent or parallel strategy to spatial fidelity when considering behaviour at the spatial-social interface. Using global positioning system locations from caribou across Newfoundland, we tested whether females selected calving sites based on proximity to familiar conspecifics, in addition to geographical (spatial) fidelity. These strategies were synergistic, not alternative, and correlated across the population but more variable within individuals. We also tested whether either form of fidelity affected reproductive success. We failed to detect an effect of spatial or social fidelity on reproductive success in this population. Nevertheless, given the association between social and spatial fidelity and the demonstrated fitness consequences of site fidelity in other systems, familiar conspecifics and the potential benefits these social partners provide may be an underappreciated component driving site fidelity.This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Rena , Reprodução , Animais , Feminino , Rena/fisiologia , Terra Nova e Labrador , Comportamento Social , Sistemas de Informação Geográfica
17.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20240060, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230458

RESUMO

According to the information centre hypothesis (ICH), colonial species use social information in roosts to locate ephemeral resources. Validating the ICH necessitates showing that uninformed individuals follow informed ones to the new resource. However, following behaviour may not be essential when individuals have a good memory of the resources' locations. For instance, Egyptian fruit bats forage on spatially predictable trees, but some bear fruit at unpredictable times. These circumstances suggest an alternative ICH pathway in which bats learn when fruits emerge from social cues in the roost but then use spatial memory to locate them without following conspecifics. Here, using an unique field manipulation and high-frequency tracking data, we test for this alternative pathway: we introduced bats smeared with the fruit odour of the unpredictably fruiting Ficus sycomorus trees to the roost, when they bore no fruits, and then tracked the movement of conspecifics exposed to the manipulated social cue. As predicted, bats visited the F. sycomorus trees with significantly higher probabilities than during routine foraging trips (of >200 bats). Our results show how the integration of spatial memory and social cues leads to efficient resource tracking and highlight the value of using large movement datasets and field experiments in behavioural ecology. This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Quirópteros , Sinais (Psicologia) , Ficus , Frutas , Memória Espacial , Animais , Quirópteros/fisiologia , Memória Espacial/fisiologia , Ficus/fisiologia , Comportamento Social , Comportamento Alimentar , Odorantes/análise
18.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220522, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230453

RESUMO

Even in our highly interconnected modern world, geographic factors play an important role in human social connections. Similarly, social relationships influence how and where we travel, and how we think about our spatial world. Here, we review the growing body of neuroscience research that is revealing multiple interactions between social and spatial processes in both humans and non-human animals. We review research on the cognitive and neural representation of spatial and social information, and highlight recent findings suggesting that underlying mechanisms might be common to both. We discuss how spatial factors can influence social behaviour, and how social concepts modify representations of space. In so doing, this review elucidates not only how neural representations of social and spatial information interact but also similarities in how the brain represents and operates on analogous information about its social and spatial surroundings.This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Comportamento Social , Humanos , Animais , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Meio Social , Cognição
19.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20220527, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230457

RESUMO

Human disturbance is contributing to widespread, global changes in the distributions and densities of wild animals. These anthropogenic impacts on wildlife arise from multiple bottom-up and top-down pathways, including habitat loss, resource provisioning, climate change, pollution, infrastructure development, hunting and our direct presence. Animal behaviour is an important mechanism linking these disturbances to population outcomes, although these behavioural pathways are often complex and can remain obscured when different aspects of behaviour are studied in isolation from one another. The spatial-social interface provides a lens for understanding how an animal's spatial and social environments interact to determine its spatial and social phenotype (i.e. measurable characteristics of an individual), and how these phenotypes interact and feed back to reshape environments. Here, we review studies of animal behaviour at the spatial-social interface to understand and predict how human disturbance affects animal movement, distribution and intraspecific interactions, with consequences for the conservation of populations and ecosystems. By understanding the spatial-social mechanisms linking human disturbance to conservation outcomes, we can better design management interventions to mitigate undesired consequences of disturbance.This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Conservação dos Recursos Naturais , Ecossistema , Comportamento Social , Animais , Efeitos Antropogênicos , Humanos , Animais Selvagens , Comportamento Espacial
20.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1912): 20230374, 2024 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39230459

RESUMO

For prey, movement synchrony represents a potent antipredator strategy. Prey, however, must balance the costs and benefits of using conspecifics to mediate risk. Thus, the emergent patterns of risk-driven sociality depend on variation in space and in the predators and prey themselves. We applied the concept of predator-prey habitat domain, the space in which animals acquire food resources, to test the conditions under which individuals synchronize their movements relative to predator and prey habitat domains. We tested the response of movement synchrony of prey to predator-prey domains in two populations of ungulates that vary in their gregariousness and predator community: (i) elk, which are preyed on by wolves; and (ii) caribou, which are preyed on by coyotes and black bears. Prey in both communities responded to cursorial predators by increasing synchrony during seasons of greater predation pressure. Elk moved more synchronously in the wolf habitat domain during winter and caribou moved more synchronously in the coyote habitat domains during spring. In the winter, caribou increased movement synchrony when coyote and caribou domains overlapped. By integrating habitat domains with movement ecology, we provide a compelling argument for social behaviours and collective movement as an antipredator response. This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: A theoretical and empirical integration'.


Assuntos
Coiotes , Cervos , Comportamento Predatório , Rena , Lobos , Animais , Lobos/fisiologia , Cervos/fisiologia , Rena/fisiologia , Coiotes/fisiologia , Ursidae/fisiologia , Ecossistema , Cadeia Alimentar , Estações do Ano , Comportamento Social , Movimento
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA