Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 47
Filtrar
1.
PLoS Biol ; 16(2): e2003113, 2018 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29438378

RESUMO

It is widely accepted for humans and higher animals that vision is an active process in which the organism interprets the stimulus. To find out whether this also holds for lower animals, we designed an ambiguous motion stimulus, which serves as something like a multi-stable perception paradigm in Drosophila behavior. Confronted with a uniform panoramic texture in a closed-loop situation in stationary flight, the flies adjust their yaw torque to stabilize their virtual self-rotation. To make the visual input ambiguous, we added a second texture. Both textures got a rotatory bias to move into opposite directions at a constant relative angular velocity. The results indicate that the fly now had three possible frames of reference for self-rotation: either of the two motion components as well as the integrated motion vector of the two. In this ambiguous stimulus situation, the flies generated a continuous sequence of behaviors, each one adjusted to one or another of the three references.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Voo Animal/fisiologia , Orientação Espacial , Estimulação Luminosa , Animais , Feminino , Processos Estocásticos , Percepção Visual
2.
J Neurogenet ; 34(1): 9-20, 2020 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32233838

RESUMO

We present here our reflections on the scientific work of the late Troy D. Zars (1967 - 2018), on what it was like to work with him, and what it means to us. A common theme running through his work is that memory systems are not for replaying the past. Rather, they are forward-looking systems, providing whatever guidance past experience has to offer for anticipating the outcome of future actions. And in situations where no such guidance is available trying things out is the best option. Working with Troy was inspiring precisely because of the optimism inherent in this concept and that he himself embodied. Our reflections highlight what this means to us as his former mentors, colleagues, and mentees, respectively, and what it might mean for the future of neurogenetics.


Assuntos
Genética/história , Neurologia/história , Animais , Antecipação Psicológica/fisiologia , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , História do Século XX , História do Século XXI , Humanos , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Mentores
3.
Learn Mem ; 24(7): 318-321, 2017 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28620079

RESUMO

Starvation causes a motivational state that facilitates diverse behaviors such as feeding, walking, and search. Starved Drosophila can form odor/feeding-time associations but the role of starvation in encoding of "time" is poorly understood. Here we show that the extent of starvation is correlated with the fly's ability to establish odor/feeding-time memories. Prolonged starvation promotes odor/feeding-time associations after just a single cycle of reciprocal training. We also show that starvation is required for acquisition but is dispensable for retrieval of odor/feeding-time memory. Finally, even with extended starvation, a functional circadian oscillator is indispensable for establishing odor/feeding-time memories.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação/fisiologia , Comportamento Alimentar/fisiologia , Privação de Alimentos/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Odorantes , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster , Feminino , Masculino , Mutação/genética , Proteínas Circadianas Period/genética , Proteínas Circadianas Period/metabolismo , Fatores de Tempo
4.
Learn Mem ; 22(6): 294-8, 2015 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25979991

RESUMO

An animal generates behavioral actions because of the effects of these actions in the future. Occasionally, the animal may generate an action in response to a certain event or situation. If the outcome of the action is adaptive, the animal may keep this stimulus-response link in its behavioral repertoire, in case the event or situation occurs again. If a responsive action is innate but the outcome happens to be less adaptive than it had been before, the link may be loosened. This adjustment of outcome expectations involves a particular kind of learning, which will be called "outcome learning." The present article discusses several examples of outcome learning in Drosophila. Learning and memory are intensely studied in flies, but the focus is on classical conditioning. Outcome learning, a particular form of operant learning, is of special significance, because it modulates outcome expectations that are operational components of action selection and intentionality.


Assuntos
Condicionamento Operante , Drosophila , Intenção , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Desempenho Psicomotor
5.
J Neurogenet ; 29(1): 30-7, 2015 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25585638

RESUMO

Novelty choice, a visual paired-comparison task, for the fly Drosophila melanogaster is studied with severely restrained single animals in a flight simulator. The virtual environment simulates free flight for rotation in the horizontal plane. The behavior has three functional components: visual azimuth orientation, working memory, and pattern discrimination (perception). Here we study novelty choice in relation to its neural substrate in the brain and show that it requires the central complex and, in particular, the ring neurons of the ellipsoid body. Surprisingly, it also involves the mushroom bodies which are needed specifically in the comparison of patterns of different sizes.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Corpos Pedunculados/citologia , Corpos Pedunculados/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Drosophila , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Inibidores Enzimáticos/farmacologia , Hidroxiureia/farmacologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Neurônios/efeitos dos fármacos , Orientação/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(17): 7230-5, 2011 Apr 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21482795

RESUMO

Organisms with complex visual systems rarely respond to just the sum of all visual stimuli impinging on their eyes. Often, they restrict their responses to stimuli in a temporarily selected region of the visual field (selective visual attention). Here, we investigate visual attention in the fly Drosophila during tethered flight at a torque meter. Flies can actively shift their attention; however, their attention can be guided to a certain location by external cues. Using visual cues, we can direct the attention of the fly to one or the other of the two visual half-fields. The cue can precede the test stimulus by several seconds and may also be spatially separated from the test by at least 20° and yet attract attention. This kind of external guidance of attention is found only in the lower visual field.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Voo Animal/fisiologia , Animais , Drosophila , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 107(12): 5634-9, 2010 Mar 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20212139

RESUMO

The visual systems of most species contain photoreceptors with distinct spectral sensitivities that allow animals to distinguish lights by their spectral composition. In Drosophila, photoreceptors R1-R6 have the same spectral sensitivity throughout the eye and are responsible for motion detection. In contrast, photoreceptors R7 and R8 exhibit heterogeneity and are important for color vision. We investigated how photoreceptor types contribute to the attractiveness of light by blocking the function of certain subsets and by measuring differential phototaxis between spectrally different lights. In a "UV vs. blue" choice, flies with only R1-R6, as well as flies with only R7/R8 photoreceptors, preferred blue, suggesting a nonadditive interaction between the two major subsystems. Flies defective for UV-sensitive R7 function preferred blue, whereas flies defective for either type of R8 (blue- or green-sensitive) preferred UV. In a "blue vs. green" choice, flies defective for R8 (blue) preferred green, whereas those defective for R8 (green) preferred blue. Involvement of all photoreceptors [R1-R6, R7, R8 (blue), R8 (green)] distinguishes phototaxis from motion detection that is mediated exclusively by R1-R6.


Assuntos
Drosophila/fisiologia , Células Fotorreceptoras de Invertebrados/fisiologia , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Comportamento Animal , Visão de Cores/genética , Visão de Cores/fisiologia , Drosophila/genética , Genes de Insetos , Modelos Biológicos , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Mutação , Dinâmica não Linear , Estimulação Luminosa , Células Fotorreceptoras de Invertebrados/classificação , Células Fotorreceptoras de Invertebrados/efeitos da radiação , Raios Ultravioleta
8.
Nature ; 439(7076): 551-6, 2006 Feb 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16452971

RESUMO

The fly Drosophila melanogaster can discriminate and remember visual landmarks. It analyses selected parts of its visual environment according to a small number of pattern parameters such as size, colour or contour orientation, and stores particular parameter values. Like humans, flies recognize patterns independently of the retinal position during acquisition of the pattern (translation invariance). Here we show that the central-most part of the fly brain, the fan-shaped body, contains parts of a network mediating visual pattern recognition. We have identified short-term memory traces of two pattern parameters--elevation in the panorama and contour orientation. These can be localized to two groups of neurons extending branches as parallel, horizontal strata in the fan-shaped body. The central location of this memory store is well suited to mediate translational invariance.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Drosophila/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adenilil Ciclases/genética , Adenilil Ciclases/metabolismo , Animais , Sinais (Psicologia) , Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Mutação/genética , Estimulação Luminosa , Torque
9.
Neuron ; 56(1): 155-70, 2007 Oct 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17920022

RESUMO

In the eye, visual information is segregated into modalities such as color and motion, these being transferred to the central brain through separate channels. Here, we genetically dissect the achromatic motion channel in the fly Drosophila melanogaster at the level of the first relay station in the brain, the lamina, where it is split into four parallel pathways (L1-L3, amc/T1). The functional relevance of this divergence is little understood. We now show that the two most prominent pathways, L1 and L2, together are necessary and largely sufficient for motion-dependent behavior. At high pattern contrast, the two pathways are redundant. At intermediate contrast, they mediate motion stimuli of opposite polarity, L2 front-to-back, L1 back-to-front motion. At low contrast, L1 and L2 depend upon each other for motion processing. Of the two minor pathways, amc/T1 specifically enhances the L1 pathway at intermediate contrast. L3 appears not to contribute to motion but to orientation behavior.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Sistema Nervoso , Vias Visuais/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Proteínas de Drosophila , Drosophila melanogaster , Voo Animal/fisiologia , Interneurônios/classificação , Interneurônios/fisiologia , Movimento (Física) , Sistema Nervoso/citologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Transdução de Sinais/fisiologia
10.
Curr Biol ; 18(3): 159-67, 2008 Feb 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18249112

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: In mammals and humans, noradrenaline is a key modulator of aggression. Octopamine, a closely related biogenic amine, has been proposed to have a similar function in arthropods. However, the effect of octopamine on aggressive behavior is little understood. RESULTS: An automated video analysis of aggression in male Drosophila has been developed, rendering aggression accessible to high-throughput studies. The software detects the lunge, a conspicuous behavioral act unique to aggression. In lunging, the aggressor rears up on his hind legs and snaps down on his opponent. By using the software to eliminate confounding effects, we now show that aggression is almost abolished in mutant males lacking octopamine. This suppression is independent of whether tyramine, the precursor of octopamine, is increased or also depleted. Restoring octopamine synthesis in the brain either throughout life or in adulthood leads to a partial rescue of aggression. Finally, neuronal silencing of octopaminergic and tyraminergic neurons almost completely abolishes lunges. CONCLUSIONS: Octopamine modulates Drosophila aggression. Genetically depleting the animal of octopamine downregulates lunge frequency without a sizable effect on the lunge motor program. This study provides access to the neuronal circuitry mediating this modulation.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Drosophila melanogaster/metabolismo , Octopamina/metabolismo , Animais , Tamanho Corporal/fisiologia , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Inativação Gênica , Masculino , Mutação , Neurônios/metabolismo , Octopamina/biossíntese , Tiramina/metabolismo , Caminhada/fisiologia
11.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 105(41): 15985-90, 2008 Oct 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18824685

RESUMO

Even in a simple Pavlovian memory task an animal may form several associations that can be independently assessed by the appropriate tests. Studying conditioned odor discrimination of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster we found that animals store quality and intensity of an odor as separate memory traces. The trace of odor intensity is short-lived, decaying in <3 h. Only the last intensity value is stored. In contrast to odor-quality memory, odor-intensity memory does not require the rutabaga-dependent cAMP signaling pathway. Flies rely on their memory of intensity in a narrow concentration range in which they can generalize intensity. Larger concentration differences they treat like different qualities. This study shows that the perceptual identity of an odor is based on at least three lines of processing in the brain: (i) a memory of odor quality, (ii) a memory of odor intensity, and (iii) a range of intensities (and qualities), in which the odor is generalized.


Assuntos
Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Odorantes , Animais , AMP Cíclico , Discriminação Psicológica
12.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 105(12): 4910-5, 2008 Mar 25.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18353989

RESUMO

Whether motion vision uses color contrast is a controversial issue that has been investigated in several species, from insects to humans. We used Drosophila to answer this question, monitoring the optomotor response to moving color stimuli in WT and genetic variants. In the fly eye, a motion channel (outer photoreceptors R1-R6) and a color channel (inner photoreceptors R7 and R8) have been distinguished. With moving bars of alternating colors and high color contrast, a brightness ratio of the two colors can be found, at which the optomotor response is largely missing (point of equiluminance). Under these conditions, mutant flies lacking functional rhodopsin in R1-R6 cells do not respond at all. Furthermore, genetically eliminating the function of photoreceptors R7 and R8 neither alters the strength of the optomotor response nor shifts the point of equiluminance. We conclude that the color channel (R7/R8) does not contribute to motion detection as monitored by the optomotor response.


Assuntos
Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Movimento (Física) , Visão Ocular , Animais , Cegueira , Cor , Sensibilidades de Contraste , Drosophila melanogaster/citologia , Mutação/genética , Células Fotorreceptoras de Invertebrados/citologia
13.
BMC Bioinformatics ; 11: 274, 2010 May 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20492697

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Current imaging methods such as Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI), Confocal microscopy, Electron Microscopy (EM) or Selective Plane Illumination Microscopy (SPIM) yield three-dimensional (3D) data sets in need of appropriate computational methods for their analysis. The reconstruction, segmentation and registration are best approached from the 3D representation of the data set. RESULTS: Here we present a platform-independent framework based on Java and Java 3D for accelerated rendering of biological images. Our framework is seamlessly integrated into ImageJ, a free image processing package with a vast collection of community-developed biological image analysis tools. Our framework enriches the ImageJ software libraries with methods that greatly reduce the complexity of developing image analysis tools in an interactive 3D visualization environment. In particular, we provide high-level access to volume rendering, volume editing, surface extraction, and image annotation. The ability to rely on a library that removes the low-level details enables concentrating software development efforts on the algorithm implementation parts. CONCLUSIONS: Our framework enables biomedical image software development to be built with 3D visualization capabilities with very little effort. We offer the source code and convenient binary packages along with extensive documentation at http://3dviewer.neurofly.de.


Assuntos
Interpretação de Imagem Assistida por Computador/métodos , Imageamento Tridimensional/métodos , Linguagens de Programação , Gráficos por Computador , Microscopia Confocal/métodos , Reconhecimento Automatizado de Padrão/métodos , Software
15.
Nature ; 430(7003): 983, 2004 Aug 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15329711

RESUMO

Can relief from pain be a pleasure? If so, noxious events should--despite their typically aversive effects--also have a 'rewarding' after-effect. Through training fruitflies by using an electric shock paired with an odour, we show here that the shock can condition either avoidance of this odour or approach to it. These opposing behaviours depend on the relative timing of the shock and odour presentations during training, and indicate that a shock can act as either an aversive reinforcer or an appetitive one.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem da Esquiva/fisiologia , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Odorantes , Punição , Recompensa , Animais , Condicionamento Psicológico/fisiologia , Eletrochoque , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Sinapses/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
16.
Mol Cell Neurosci ; 42(2): 134-41, 2009 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19555761

RESUMO

Mutations in Ribosomal s6 kinase 2 (Rsk2) are associated with severe neuronal dysfunction in Coffin-Lowry syndrome (CLS) patients, flies and mice. So far, the mechanisms of how Rsk2 regulates development, maintenance and activity of neurons are not understood. We have investigated the consequences of Rsk2 deficiency in mouse spinal motoneurons. Survival of isolated Rsk2 deficient motoneurons is not reduced, but these cells grow significantly longer neurites. Conversely, overexpression of a constitutively active form of Rsk2 leads to reduced axon growth. Increased axon growth in Rsk2 deficient neurons was accompanied by higher Erk 1/2 phosphorylation, and the knockout phenotype could be rescued by pharmacological inhibition of MAPK/Erk kinase (Mek). These data indicate that Rsk2 negatively regulates axon elongation via the MAPK pathway. Thus, the functional defects observed in the nervous system of CLS patients and animal models with Rsk2 deficiency might be caused by dysregulated neurite growth rather than primary neurodegeneration.


Assuntos
Axônios/fisiologia , Neurônios Motores , Proteínas Quinases S6 Ribossômicas 90-kDa/metabolismo , Animais , Sobrevivência Celular , Feminino , Humanos , Sistema de Sinalização das MAP Quinases/fisiologia , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Camundongos Knockout , Neurônios Motores/citologia , Neurônios Motores/fisiologia , Proteínas Quinases S6 Ribossômicas 90-kDa/genética , Medula Espinal/citologia
17.
Elife ; 92020 02 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32066523

RESUMO

Experience alters brain structure, but the underlying mechanism remained unknown. Structural plasticity reveals that brain function is encoded in generative changes to cells that compete with destructive processes driving neurodegeneration. At an adult critical period, experience increases fiber number and brain size in Drosophila. Here, we asked if Toll receptors are involved. Tolls demarcate a map of brain anatomical domains. Focusing on Toll-2, loss of function caused apoptosis, neurite atrophy and impaired behaviour. Toll-2 gain of function and neuronal activity at the critical period increased cell number. Toll-2 induced cycling of adult progenitor cells via a novel pathway, that antagonized MyD88-dependent quiescence, and engaged Weckle and Yorkie downstream. Constant knock-down of multiple Tolls synergistically reduced brain size. Conditional over-expression of Toll-2 and wek at the adult critical period increased brain size. Through their topographic distribution, Toll receptors regulate neuronal number and brain size, modulating structural plasticity in the adult brain.


Everything that you experience leaves its mark on your brain. When you learn something new, the neurons involved in the learning episode grow new projections and form new connections. Your brain may even produce new neurons. Physical exercise can induce similar changes, as can taking antidepressants. By contrast, stress, depression, ageing and disease can have the opposite effect, triggering neurons to break down and even die. The ability of the brain to change in response to experience is known as structural plasticity, and it is in a tug-of-war with processes that drive neurodegeneration. Structural plasticity occurs in other species too: for example, it was described in the fruit fly more than a quarter of a century ago. Yet, the molecular mechanisms underlying structural plasticity remain unclear. Li et al. now show that, in fruit flies, this plasticity involves Toll receptors, a family of proteins present in the brain but best known for their role in the immune system. Fruit flies have nine different Toll receptors, the most abundant being Toll-2. When activated, these proteins can trigger a series of molecular events in a cell. Li et al. show that increasing the amount of Toll-2 in the fly brain makes the brain produce new neurons. Activating neurons in a brain region has the same effect, and this increase in neuron number also depends on Toll-2. By contrast, reducing the amount of Toll-2 causes neurons to lose their projections and connections, and to die, and impairs fly behaviour. Li et al. also show that each Toll receptor has a unique distribution across the fly brain. Different types of experiences activate different brain regions, and therefore different Toll receptors. These go on to trigger a common molecular cascade, but they modulate it such as to result in distinct outcomes. By working together in different combinations, Toll receptors can promote either the death or survival of neurons, and they can also drive specific brain cells to remain dormant or to produce new neurons. By revealing how experience changes the brain, Li et al. provide clues to the way neurons work and form; these findings may also help to find new treatments for disorders that change brain structure, such as certain psychiatric conditions. Toll-like receptors in humans could thus represent a promising new target for drug discovery.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Receptores Toll-Like/metabolismo , Animais , Encéfalo/citologia , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Neurônios/fisiologia
18.
Neuron ; 35(5): 951-60, 2002 Aug 29.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12372288

RESUMO

Memory loss occurs by diverse mechanisms, as different time constants of performance decrement and sensitivities to experimental manipulations suggest. While the phenomena of memory decay, interference, and extinction are well established behaviorally, little is known about them at the circuit or molecular level. In Drosophila, odorant memories lasting up to 3 hr can be localized to mushroom body Kenyon cells, a single neuronal level in the olfactory pathway. The plasticity underlying this memory trace can be induced without Kenyon cell synaptic output. Experimental extinction, i.e., presentation of the conditioned stimulus without the reinforcer, reduces memory performance and does so at the same circuit level as memory formation. Thus, unreinforced presentation of learned odorants antagonizes intracellularly the signaling cascade underlying memory formation.


Assuntos
Extinção Psicológica/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Corpos Pedunculados/fisiologia , Condutos Olfatórios/citologia , Olfato/fisiologia , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados/fisiologia , Condicionamento Psicológico/fisiologia , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Corpos Pedunculados/citologia , Condutos Olfatórios/fisiologia , Olfato/genética , Frações Subcelulares/fisiologia , Sinapses/genética , Sinapses/fisiologia
19.
J Neurosci ; 27(41): 11132-8, 2007 Oct 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17928455

RESUMO

Physical traces underlying simple memories can be confined to a single group of cells in the brain. In the fly Drosophila melanogaster, the Kenyon cells of the mushroom bodies house traces for both appetitive and aversive odor memories. The adenylate cyclase protein, Rutabaga, has been shown to mediate both traces. Here, we show that, for appetitive learning, another group of cells can additionally accommodate a Rutabaga-dependent memory trace. Localized expression of rutabaga in either projection neurons, the first-order olfactory interneurons, or in Kenyon cells, the second-order interneurons, is sufficient for rescuing the mutant defect in appetitive short-term memory. Thus, appetitive learning may induce multiple memory traces in the first- and second-order olfactory interneurons using the same plasticity mechanism. In contrast, aversive odor memory of rutabaga is rescued selectively in the Kenyon cells, but not in the projection neurons. This difference in the organization of memory traces is consistent with the internal representation of reward and punishment.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Condutos Olfatórios/fisiologia , Recompensa , Olfato/fisiologia , Animais , Drosophila melanogaster , Feminino , Masculino
20.
Mol Biol Cell ; 16(5): 2433-42, 2005 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15772149

RESUMO

The elaboration of neuronal axons and dendrites is dependent on a functional cytoskeleton. Cytoskeletal components have been shown to play a major role in the maintenance of the nervous system through adulthood, and changes in neurofilaments and microtubule-associated proteins (MAPs) have been linked to a variety of neurodegenerative diseases. Here we show that Futsch, the fly homolog of MAP1B, is involved in progressive neurodegeneration. Although Futsch is widely expressed throughout the CNS, degeneration in futsch(olk) primarily occurs in the olfactory system and mushroom bodies. Consistent with the predicted function of Futsch, we find abnormalities in the microtubule network and defects in axonal transport. Degeneration in the adult brain is preceded by learning deficits, revealing a neuronal dysfunction before detectable levels of cell death. Futsch is negatively regulated by the Drosophila Fragile X mental retardation gene, and a mutation in this gene delays the onset of neurodegeneration in futsch(olk). A similar effect is obtained by expression of either fly or bovine tau, suggesting a certain degree of functional redundancy of MAPs. The futsch(olk) mutants exhibit several characteristics of human neurodegenerative diseases, providing an opportunity to study the role of MAPs in progressive neurodegeneration within an experimentally accessible, in vivo model system.


Assuntos
Proteínas de Drosophila/fisiologia , Drosophila/fisiologia , Proteínas Associadas aos Microtúbulos/fisiologia , Fatores de Crescimento Neural/fisiologia , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Transporte Axonal , Bovinos , Citoesqueleto/fisiologia , Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Feminino , Proteína do X Frágil da Deficiência Intelectual , Genes de Insetos , Masculino , Proteínas Associadas aos Microtúbulos/genética , Mitocôndrias/metabolismo , Mutação , Degeneração Neural/genética , Degeneração Neural/fisiopatologia , Fatores de Crescimento Neural/genética , Neurônios/fisiologia , Proteínas de Ligação a RNA/genética , Proteínas de Ligação a RNA/fisiologia , Olfato/genética , Olfato/fisiologia , Proteínas tau/genética , Proteínas tau/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA