Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 49
Filtrar
1.
J Psychiatry Neurosci ; 41(3): 203-13, 2016 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26599134

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Individuals with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11.2DS) have an elevated risk for schizophrenia, which increases with history of childhood anxiety. Altered hippocampal morphology is a common neuroanatomical feature of 22q11.2DS and idiopathic schizophrenia. Relating hippocampal structure in children with 22q11.2DS to anxiety and impaired cognitive ability could lead to hippocampus-based characterization of psychosis-proneness in this at-risk population. METHODS: We measured hippocampal volume using a semiautomated approach on MRIs collected from typically developing children and children with 22q11.2DS. We then analyzed hippocampal morphology with Localized Components Analysis. We tested the modulating roles of diagnostic group, hippocampal volume, sex and age on local hippocampal shape components. Lastly, volume and shape components were tested as covariates of IQ and anxiety. RESULTS: We included 48 typically developing children and 69 children with 22q11.2DS in our study. Hippocampal volume was reduced bilaterally in children with 22q11.2DS, and these children showed greater variation in the shape of the anterior hippocampus than typically developing children. Children with 22q11.2DS had greater inward deformation of the anterior hippocampus than typically developing children. Greater inward deformation of the anterior hippocampus was associated with greater severity of anxiety, specifically fear of physical injury, within the 22q11.2DS group. LIMITATIONS: Shape alterations are not specific to hippocampal subfields. CONCLUSION: Alterations in the structure of the anterior hippocampus likely affect function and may impact limbic circuitry. We suggest these alterations potentially contribute to anxiety symptoms in individuals with 22q11.2DS through modulatory pathways. Altered hippocampal morphology may be uniquely linked to anxiety risk factors for schizophrenia, which could be a powerful neuroanatomical marker of schizophrenia risk and hence protection.


Assuntos
Ansiedade/diagnóstico por imagem , Síndrome de DiGeorge/diagnóstico por imagem , Síndrome de DiGeorge/psicologia , Hipocampo/diagnóstico por imagem , Adolescente , Criança , Feminino , Hipocampo/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Inteligência , Testes de Inteligência , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Tamanho do Órgão , Prognóstico , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Caracteres Sexuais
2.
Hippocampus ; 24(7): 794-807, 2014 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24648155

RESUMO

Nonhuman primates are widely used models to investigate the neural substrates of human behavior, including the development of higher cognitive and affective function. Due to their neuroanatomical and behavioral homologies with humans, the rhesus macaque monkey (Macaca mulatta) provides an excellent animal model in which to characterize the maturation of brain structures from birth through adulthood and into senescence. To evaluate hippocampal development in rhesus macaques, structural magnetic resonance imaging scans were obtained longitudinally at 9 time points between 1 week and 260 weeks (5 years) of age on 24 rhesus macaque monkeys (12 males, 12 females). In our sample, the hippocampus reaches 50% of its adult volume by 13 weeks of age and reaches an adult volume by 52 weeks in both males and females. The hippocampus appears to be slightly larger at 3 years than at 5 years of age. Male rhesus macaques have larger hippocampi than females from 8 weeks onward by approximately 5%. Interestingly, there was increased variability in hemispheric asymmetry for hippocampus volumes at younger ages than at later ages. These data provide a comprehensive evaluation of the longitudinal development of male and female rhesus macaque hippocampus across development from 1 week to 5 years of age.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Macaca mulatta/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Neuroimagem , Animais , Feminino , Masculino , Tamanho do Órgão , Caracteres Sexuais
3.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 109: 160-8, 2014 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24462720

RESUMO

Environmental enrichment results in increased levels of Fmrp in brain and increased dendritic complexity. The present experiment evaluated activity-dependent increases in Fmrp levels in the motor cortex in response to training on a skilled forelimb reaching task in the CGG KI mouse model of the fragile X premutation. Fmrp, Arc, and c-Fos protein levels were quantified by Western blot in the contralateral motor cortex of mice following training to reach for sucrose pellets with a non-preferred paw and compared to levels in the ipsilateral motor cortex. After training, all mice showed increases in Fmrp, Arc, and c-Fos protein levels in the contralateral compared to the ipsilateral hemisphere; however, the increase in CGG KI mice was less than wildtype mice. Increases in Fmrp and Arc proteins scaled with learning, whereas this relationship was not observed with the c-Fos levels. These data suggest the possibility that reduced levels of activity-dependent proteins associated with synaptic plasticity such as Fmrp and Arc may contribute to the neurocognitive phenotype reported in the CGG KI mice and the fragile X premutation.


Assuntos
Proteína do X Frágil da Deficiência Intelectual/metabolismo , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/metabolismo , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Córtex Motor/metabolismo , Animais , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Proteína do X Frágil da Deficiência Intelectual/genética , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/genética , Técnicas de Introdução de Genes , Masculino , Camundongos , Mutação
4.
Hippocampus ; 23(5): 380-91, 2013 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23436324

RESUMO

It has been suggested that the role of the hippocampus for episodic memory is to selectively bind together item and contextual information. One such model, the Binding of Items and Context (BIC) model, proposed that the perirhinal cortex provides item and the postrhinal/parahippocampal cortex provides context to the hippocampus via the medial (MEC) and lateral entorhinal cortices (LEC) to be bound into an episodic representation. This model proposes that item and context information are stored and processed independently and in parallel before hippocampal processing. To evaluate this model, the present experiment evaluated the role of the MEC and LEC for item and contextual novelty detection. The present results suggest that excitotoxic lesions to the LEC primarily disrupt item novelty detection, whereas lesions to the MEC primarily disrupt contextual novelty detection. These data provide a functional double dissociation between the MEC and LEC across item and contextual processing. Despite this dissociation, the present results suggest that item and contextual information are not represented independently before hippocampal processing. These data support the basic assumptions of the BIC model, but suggest that item and context information may interact in the entorhinal cortex.


Assuntos
Córtex Entorrinal/fisiologia , Memória Episódica , Modelos Psicológicos , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Detecção de Sinal Psicológico/fisiologia , Animais , Córtex Entorrinal/anatomia & histologia , Córtex Entorrinal/lesões , Agonistas de Aminoácidos Excitatórios/toxicidade , Comportamento Exploratório/fisiologia , Hipocampo/lesões , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Ácido Ibotênico/toxicidade , Masculino , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans
5.
Hippocampus ; 22(12): 2260-75, 2012 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22707411

RESUMO

The fragile X premutation is a CGG repeat expansion on the FMR1 gene between 55 and 200 repeats in length. It has been proposed that impaired spatiotemporal function underlies cognitive deficits in genetic disorders, including the fragile X premutation. This study characterized the role of the premutation for cognitive function by demonstrating CGG KI mice with 70-198 CGG repeats show deficits across tasks requiring spatial and temporal pattern separation. To elucidate mechanisms whereby CGG repeats affect spatiotemporal processing, hippocampal slices were evaluated for LTP, LTD, and mGluR1/5 LTD. Increasing CGG repeat length modulated the induction of LTP, LTD, and mGluR1/5 LTD, as well as behavioral tasks emphasizing spatiotemporal processing. Despite the deficits in the induction of all forms of plasticity, there were no differences in expression of plasticity once evoked. These data provide evidence for a neurocognitive endophenotype in the CGG KI mouse model of the premutation in which CGG repeat length negatively modulates plasticity and spatiotemporal attention.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Proteína do X Frágil da Deficiência Intelectual/genética , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/genética , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Repetições de Trinucleotídeos/genética , Animais , Análise por Conglomerados , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/fisiopatologia , Genótipo , Humanos , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Transgênicos , Técnicas de Patch-Clamp , Máquina de Vetores de Suporte
6.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 97(2): 229-34, 2012 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22202169

RESUMO

The fragile X premutation is a tandem CGG trinucleotide repeat expansion in the fragile X mental retardation 1 (FMR1) gene between 55 and 200 repeats in length. A CGG knock-in (CGG KI) mouse has been developed that models the neuropathology and cognitive deficits reported in fragile X premutation carriers. Previous studies have demonstrated that CGG KI mice have spatiotemporal information processing deficits and impaired visuomotor function that worsen with increasing CGG repeat length. Since skilled forelimb reaching requires integration of information from the visual and motor systems, skilled reaching performance could identify potential visuomotor dysfunction in CGG KI mice. To characterize motor deficits associated with the fragile X premutation, 6 month old female CGG KI mice heterozygous for trinucleotide repeats ranging from 70-200 CGG in length were tested for their ability to learn a skilled forelimb reaching task. The results demonstrate that female CGG KI mice show deficits for learning a skilled forelimb reaching task compared to wildtype littermates, and that these deficits worsen with increasing CGG repeat lengths.


Assuntos
Proteína do X Frágil da Deficiência Intelectual/genética , Movimento/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Expansão das Repetições de Trinucleotídeos , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Feminino , Membro Anterior/fisiopatologia , Técnicas de Introdução de Genes , Camundongos , Camundongos Transgênicos
7.
Epilepsia ; 53 Suppl 1: 150-60, 2012 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22612820

RESUMO

The fragile X mental retardation 1 gene (Fmr1) is polymorphic for CGG trinucleotide repeat number in the 5'-untranslated region, with repeat lengths <45 associated with typical development and repeat lengths >200 resulting in hypermethylation and transcriptional silencing of the gene and mental retardation in the fragile X Syndrome (FXS). Individuals with CGG repeat expansions between 55 and 200 are carriers of the fragile X premutation (PM). PM carriers show a phenotype that can include anxiety, depression, social phobia, and memory deficits. They are also at risk for developing fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome (FXTAS), a late onset neurodegenerative disorder characterized by tremor, ataxia, cognitive impairment, and neuropathologic features including intranuclear inclusions in neurons and astrocytes, loss of Purkinje cells, and white matter disease. However, very little is known about dendritic morphology in PM or in FXTAS. Therefore, we carried out a Golgi study of dendritic complexity and dendritic spine morphology in layer II/III pyramidal neurons in primary visual cortex in a knock-in (KI) mouse model of the PM. These CGG KI mice carry an expanded CGG trinucleotide repeat on Fmr1, and model many features of the PM and FXTAS. Compared to wild-type (WT) mice, CGG KI mice showed fewer dendritic branches proximal to the soma, reduced total dendritic length, and a higher frequency of longer dendritic spines. The distribution of morphologic spine types (e.g., stubby, mushroom, filopodial) did not differ between WT and KI mice. These findings demonstrate that synaptic circuitry is abnormal in visual cortex of mice used to model the PM, and suggest that such changes may underlie neurologic features found in individuals carrying the PM as well as in individuals with FXTAS.


Assuntos
Dendritos/patologia , Espinhas Dendríticas/patologia , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/genética , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/patologia , Repetições de Trinucleotídeos/genética , Córtex Visual/patologia , Regiões 5' não Traduzidas/genética , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Ataxia/genética , Ataxia/patologia , Western Blotting , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Proteína do X Frágil da Deficiência Intelectual/genética , Genótipo , Complexo de Golgi/patologia , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Mutação/genética , Mutação/fisiologia , Células Piramidais/fisiologia , Células Piramidais/ultraestrutura , RNA Mensageiro/biossíntese , RNA Mensageiro/genética , Reação em Cadeia da Polimerase em Tempo Real , Sinapses/patologia , Sinapses/ultraestrutura
8.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 96(2): 361-6, 2011 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21742047

RESUMO

Olfactory working memory and pattern separation for odor information was assessed in male rats using a matching-to-sample for odors paradigm. The odor set consisted of a five aliphatic acids with unbranched carbon chains that varied from two- to six-carbons in length. Each trial consisted of a sample phase followed by a choice phase. During the sample phase, rats would receive one of five different odors. Fifteen seconds later during the choice phase one of the previous odors was presented simultaneously side by side with a different odor that was based on the number of aliphatic acids that varied in the carbon chains from two- to six-carbons in length and rats were allowed to choose between the two odors. The rule to be learned in order to receive a food reward was to always choose the odor that occurred during the study phase. Odor separations of 1, 2, 3 or 4 were selected for each choice phase and represented the carbon chain difference between the study phase odor and the test phase odor. Once an animal reached a criterion of 80-90% correct across all temporal separations based on 40 trials, rats received a control, dorsal hippocampal, or ventral hippocampal lesion and were retested on the task. On postoperative trials, only the ventral hippocampal lesion group was impaired relative to both control and dorsal hippocampal groups groups. There were no effects on odor pattern separation. All groups of rats could discriminate between the odors. The data suggest that the ventral hippocampus, but not dorsal hippocampus, supports working memory for odor information.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Percepção Olfatória/fisiologia , Animais , Masculino , Odorantes , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Olfato/fisiologia
9.
Acta Neuropathol ; 122(4): 467-79, 2011 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21785977

RESUMO

Fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome (FXTAS) is an adult-onset neurodegenerative disorder generally presenting with intention tremor and gait ataxia, but with a growing list of co-morbid medical conditions including hypothyroidism, hypertension, peripheral neuropathy, and cognitive decline. The pathological hallmark of FXTAS is the presence of intranuclear inclusions in both neurons and astroglia. However, it is unknown to what extent such inclusions are present outside the central nervous system (CNS). To address this issue, we surveyed non-CNS organs in ten human cases with FXTAS and in a CGG repeat knock-in (CGG KI) mouse model known to possess neuronal and astroglial inclusions. We find inclusions in multiple tissues from FXTAS cases and CGG KI mice, including pancreas, thyroid, adrenal gland, gastrointestinal, pituitary gland, pineal gland, heart, and mitral valve, as well as throughout the associated autonomic ganglia. Inclusions were observed in the testes, epididymis, and kidney of FXTAS cases, but were not observed in mice. These observations demonstrate extensive involvement of the peripheral nervous system and systemic organs. The finding of intranuclear inclusions in non-CNS somatic organ systems, throughout the PNS, and in the enteric nervous system of both FXTAS cases as well as CGG KI mice suggests that these tissues may serve as potential sites to evaluate early intervention strategies or be used as diagnostic factors.


Assuntos
Ataxia/patologia , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/genética , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/patologia , Triagem de Portadores Genéticos , Tremor/patologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Animais , Ataxia/genética , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Feminino , Proteína do X Frágil da Deficiência Intelectual/genética , Técnicas de Introdução de Genes , Humanos , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Mutantes , Especificidade de Órgãos/genética , Tremor/genética
10.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 93(1): 111-6, 2010 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19733676

RESUMO

Memory for the temporal order of a sequence of odors was assessed in male rats. A sequence of five odors mixed in sand was presented in digging cups one at a time to each rat in a sequence that varied on each trial. A reward was buried in each cup. Following the fifth odor, two of the previous five odors were presented simultaneously and the rat needed to choose the odor that occurred earliest in the sequence to receive a reward. Temporal separations of 1, 2, or 3 were used which represented the number of odors that occurred between the two odors in the sequence. Once pre-operative criterion was reached, rats received a control, dorsal CA1 (dCA1), or ventral CA1 (vCA1) lesion and were retested on the task. On post-operative trials, only the vCA1 group was impaired relative to both control and dCA1 groups. All groups of rats could discriminate between the odors. The data suggest that the vCA1, but not dorsal CA1, is involved in separating sensory events (odors) in time so that one odor can be remembered separate from another odor.


Assuntos
Região CA1 Hipocampal/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Odorantes , Percepção Olfatória/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Animais , Região CA1 Hipocampal/lesões , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reconhecimento Fisiológico de Modelo/fisiologia , Estimulação Física , Distribuição Aleatória , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Recompensa , Fatores de Tempo
11.
Behav Neurosci ; 123(3): 624-30, 2009 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19485569

RESUMO

It has been proposed that the hippocampus and subcortical structures interact during the processing of fear and anxiety-related information. It has been demonstrated that the subcortical efferents from CA3 and CA1 can be selectively disrupted without concomitant disruption to the afferents. The present experiment was designed to evaluate the role of CA3 efferents via the fimbria and the CA1 efferents via the dorsal fornix for encoding and consolidation/retrieval of classical fear conditioning. The present data suggest that the subcortical projections from CA3 and CA1 are differentially involved in the processing of classical fear conditioning, with CA3 subcortical efferents supporting acquisition of both cued and contextual fear but only supporting retention of contextual fear and CA1 subcortical efferents supporting the encoding and retrieval of both cued and contextual fear. These data further suggest that all hippocampal efferents are not homogeneous, and that the hippocampus and subcortex interact to process conditioned fear.


Assuntos
Condicionamento Clássico/fisiologia , Medo , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Análise de Variância , Animais , Aprendizagem por Associação/fisiologia , Vias Eferentes/fisiologia , Eletrochoque , Fórnice/fisiologia , Reação de Congelamento Cataléptica , Masculino , Memória/fisiologia , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Septo do Cérebro/fisiologia
12.
Acta Neuropathol Commun ; 7(1): 27, 2019 02 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30808398

RESUMO

The fragile X premutation is a CGG trinucleotide repeat expansion between 55 and 200 repeats in the 5'-untranslated region of the fragile X mental retardation 1 (FMR1) gene. Human carriers of the premutation allele are at risk of developing the late-onset neurodegenerative disorder, fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome (FXTAS). Characteristic neuropathology associated with FXTAS includes intranuclear inclusions in neurons and astroglia. Previous studies recapitulated these histopathological features in neurons in a knock-in mouse model, but without significant astroglial pathology. To determine the role of astroglia in FXTAS, we generated a transgenic mouse line (Gfa2-CGG99-eGFP) that selectively expresses a 99-CGG repeat expansion linked to an enhanced green fluorescent protein (eGFP) reporter in astroglia throughout the brain, including cerebellar Bergmann glia. Behaviorally these mice displayed impaired motor performance on the ladder-rung test, but paradoxically better performance on the rotarod. Immunocytochemical analysis revealed that CGG99-eGFP co-localized with GFAP and S-100ß, but not with NeuN, Iba1, or MBP, indicating that CGG99-eGFP expression is specific to astroglia. Ubiquitin-positive intranuclear inclusions were found in eGFP-expressing glia throughout the brain. In addition, intracytoplasmic ubiquitin-positive inclusions were found outside the nucleus in distal astrocyte processes. Intriguingly, intranuclear inclusions, in the absence of eGFP mRNA and eGFP fluorescence, were present in neurons of the hypothalamus and neocortex. Furthermore, intranuclear inclusions in both neurons and astrocytes displayed immunofluorescent labeling for the polyglycine peptide FMRpolyG, implicating FMRpolyG in the pathology found in Gfa2-CGG99 mice. Considered together, these results show that Gfa2-CGG99 expression in mice is sufficient to induce key features of FXTAS pathology, including formation of intranuclear inclusions, translation of FMRpolyG, and deficits in motor function.


Assuntos
Astrócitos/fisiologia , Ataxia/genética , Comunicação Celular/fisiologia , Proteína do X Frágil da Deficiência Intelectual/genética , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/genética , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/genética , Tremor/genética , Expansão das Repetições de Trinucleotídeos/genética , Animais , Astrócitos/metabolismo , Astrócitos/patologia , Ataxia/metabolismo , Ataxia/patologia , Sequência de Bases , Proteína do X Frágil da Deficiência Intelectual/biossíntese , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/metabolismo , Síndrome do Cromossomo X Frágil/patologia , Expressão Gênica , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Camundongos Transgênicos , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/metabolismo , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/patologia , Tremor/metabolismo , Tremor/patologia
13.
Hippocampus ; 18(9): 955-64, 2008.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18493930

RESUMO

It has been demonstrated that the dorsal CA1 subregion of the hippocampus mediates temporal processing of information, that dorsal CA3 participates in the spatiotemporal processing of memory, and the dorsal dentate gyrus (DG) mediates spatial pattern separation. A temporal ordering of spatial locations task was developed to test the role of the dorsal DG, CA3, and CA1 for the temporal processing of spatial information with either high or low levels of spatial interference. The results indicate that animals with DG lesions showed difficulty performing the task at high levels of spatial interference, but were able to perform the task well when there was low spatial interference. Animals with lesions to CA3 did not show a preference for either spatial location presented during the study phase during the preference test, suggesting impaired spatiotemporal processing. Animals with lesions to CA1 showed a preference for a later presented spatial location over the earlier, the opposite preference to that shown by control animals.


Assuntos
Giro Denteado/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Animais , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia
14.
Hippocampus ; 18(7): 699-709, 2008.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18493950

RESUMO

CA3 lesions impair encoding, whereas CA1 lesions impair retrieval during learning of a Hebb-Williams maze. CA3 efferents in the fimbria were transected, taking care to spare cholinergic and GABAergic afferents. CA1 efferents in the dorsal fornix were similarly transected. Fimbria transections, but not dorsal fornix transections, resulted in deficits for the encoding of spatial information during learning of a Hebb-Williams maze. Dorsal fornix, but not fimbria, transections resulted in deficits for retrieval of spatial memory during learning of a Hebb-Williams maze. These results reveal a double dissociation for the roles of CA3 and CA1 subcortical efferents in encoding and retrieval processes that mirror the double dissociation seen after excitotoxic lesions of CA1 and CA3. These data provide support for the theory that the cholinergic projections from the septal nuclei modulate the dynamics for encoding and consolidation/retrieval in the hippocampus.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/citologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Aprendizagem em Labirinto/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Denervação , Vias Eferentes/citologia , Vias Eferentes/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Fórnice/citologia , Fórnice/patologia , Fórnice/fisiologia , Hipocampo/patologia , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans
15.
Hippocampus ; 18(10): 1064-73, 2008.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18651615

RESUMO

It has been suggested that the dentate gyrus (DG) and CA3 cooperate to efficiently process spatial information. The DG has been proposed to be important for fine spatial discrimination, and the CA3 has been proposed to mediate larger scale spatial information processing. To evaluate the roles of the DG and CA3a,b for spatial processing, we developed a task that measures responses to either overall environmental novelty or a response to more subtle changes within the environment. Animals with lesions to the DG showed impaired novelty detection for both environment as well as smaller changes in the environment, whereas animals with lesions to CA3a,b showed no such deficits. A closer look at the lesions suggested that the CA3 lesions included only CA3a and CA3b, but spared CA3c. To test the role of the spared CA3c region, animals with selective lesions to CA3c that spared CA3a,b were run on the same task and showed an intermediate pattern of deficits. These results suggest that the DG is critical for spatial information processing. These data also suggest that CA3 is a heterogeneous structure, with CA3c lesioned animals showing greater spatial processing deficits than CA3a,b lesioned animals. These findings extend our knowledge of hippocampal function and need to be accounted for in future computational models.


Assuntos
Giro Denteado/fisiologia , Comportamento Exploratório/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia , Animais , Giro Denteado/patologia , Meio Ambiente , Hipocampo/patologia , Masculino , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia
16.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 90(2): 389-94, 2008 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18571941

RESUMO

Human research has shown that lesions of the parietal cortex disrupt spatial information processing, specifically topological information. Similar findings have been found in non-humans. It has been difficult to determine homologies between human and non-human mnemonic mechanisms for spatial information processing because methodologies and neuropathology differ. The first objective of the present study was to adapt a previously established human task for rats. The second objective was to better characterize the role of parietal cortex (PC) and dorsal hippocampus (dHPC) for topological spatial information processing. Rats had to distinguish whether a ball inside a ring or a ball outside a ring was the correct, rewarded object. After rats reached criterion on the task (>95%) they were randomly assigned to a lesion group (control, PC, and dHPC). Animals were then re-tested. Post-surgery data show that controls were 94% correct on average, dHPC rats were 89% correct on average, and PC rats were 56% correct on average. The results from the present study suggest that the parietal cortex, but not the dHPC processes topological spatial information. The present data are the first to support comparable topological spatial information processes of the parietal cortex in humans and rats.


Assuntos
Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Animais , Mapeamento Encefálico , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Especificidade da Espécie
17.
Behav Neurosci ; 122(1): 16-26, 2008 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18298245

RESUMO

Several studies have demonstrated the significance of a spatial cognitive map and its role for guided and accurate navigation through the environment. Learning and recalling spatial knowledge depends upon proper topological and metric spatial information processing. The present objectives are to better characterize the role of the hippocampus for processing topological and metric spatial information. Rats with dorsal hippocampal subregional lesions (dDG, dCA3, dCA1) were tested on a previously established metric task and topological task. The results of the present study suggest that dCA1, but not dDG or dCA3, mediates topological memory. Furthermore, dDG, dCA3, and dCA1 mediate metric memory. Dorsal DG is required for spatial information processing via pattern separation or orthogonalization of sensory inputs to generate metric representations. Dorsal CA3 and dCA1 then receive these metric representations transmitted from dDG along the trisynaptic loop. The present data add to a growing body of literature suggesting a diversity of function among the hippocampal subregions.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/anatomia & histologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Comportamento Exploratório , Hipocampo/lesões , Masculino , Distribuição Aleatória , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans
18.
Behav Neurosci ; 122(6): 1217-25, 2008 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19045941

RESUMO

This experiment tested the theory that the CA3 subregion of the hippocampus mediates episodic learning of arbitrary associations. The authors developed 2 tasks based on the episodic flavor-place paired-associate task described by M. Day, R. Langston, and R. G. Morris (2003): an object-cued spatial location recall task and a spatial location-cued object recall task. After rats were trained to a criterion of 80% correct on 1 of the 2 tasks, they received either a dorsal CA3 lesion or a vehicle control lesion. Control animals continued performing well on both tasks. Rats with lesions to dorsal CA3 were impaired on both tasks and performed at chance but were able to perform a nonepisodic version of the task as a control. These data suggest that CA3 mediates episodic learning of arbitrary associations as tested in the 1-trial object-cued spatial location recall and spatial location-cued object recall tasks.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Sinais (Psicologia) , Hipocampo/lesões , Masculino , Aprendizagem em Labirinto , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans
19.
Behav Neurosci ; 122(3): 643-50, 2008 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18513134

RESUMO

The differential contributions of the dorsal and ventral hippocampus for learning and memory have long been of interest. The present experiments were designed to evaluate the contributions of dorsal CA1 and ventral CA1 for temporal processing. Animals were run on three temporal ordering paradigms: one with visual objects, one with olfactory stimuli, and one with spatial locations. Animals with lesions to dorsal CA1 showed deficits for the temporal ordering of visual objects relative to control animals, and deficits for the temporal ordering of spatial locations relative to control and ventral CA1 lesioned animals. Animals with lesions to ventral CA1 showed deficits for the temporal ordering of olfactory information relative to control and dorsal CA1 lesioned animals, and a mild deficit for the temporal ordering of visual objects relative to control animals, but not as severe as those shown by the dorsal CA1 lesioned animals. These data suggest that dorsal CA1 and ventral CA1 contribute to temporal ordering processes, and that dorsal CA1 and ventral CA1 are dissociable for temporal ordering based upon the nature of the information that is processed.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/patologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Odorantes , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Mapeamento Encefálico , Comportamento Exploratório/fisiologia , Hipocampo/anatomia & histologia , Hipocampo/lesões , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia
20.
Behav Brain Res ; 188(2): 310-5, 2008 Apr 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18178264

RESUMO

It has been suggested that the hippocampus mediates episodic memory processing involving snapshot memory and temporal sequence learning. To test this theory, rats learned trial-unique sequences of spatial locations along a runway box and were tested on recall by removing one of the locations in the sequence and making the rat choose the correct location to be rewarded. Once animals were able to reliably perform this episodic memory task, they received lesions to either CA3 or CA1. Animals with lesions to either CA3 or CA1 had difficulty with episodic memory processing, although CA1 lesioned animals had a much greater deficit. However, when animals were trained on a non-episodic version of the same task, hippocampal lesions had no effect. These results suggest that CA3 and CA1 both contribute to episodic memory processing since lesions to CA3 or CA1 result in an inability to process spatial information episodically, whereas they have no effect on non-episodic information processing.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/anatomia & histologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Hipocampo/lesões , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA