Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 25
Filtrar
1.
Child Dev ; 94(4): 1049-1067, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37016553

RESUMO

In Tajikistan, infants are bound supine in a "gahvora" cradle that severely restricts movement. Does cradling affect motor development and body growth? In three studies (2013-2018), we investigated associations between time in the gahvora (within days and across age) and motor skills and flattened head dimensions in 8-24-month-old Tajik infants (N = 269, 133 girls, 136 boys)) and 4.3-5.1-year-old children (N = 91, 53 girls, 38 boys). Infants had later motor onset ages relative to World Health Organization standards and pronounced brachycephaly; cradling predicted walk onset age and the proficiency of sitting, crawling, and walking. By 4-5 years, children's motor skills were comparable with US norms. Cultural differences in early experiences offer a unique lens onto developmental processes and equifinality in development.


Assuntos
Destreza Motora , Caminhada , Lactente , Masculino , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Pré-Escolar , Tadjiquistão , Relações Pais-Filho , Desenvolvimento Infantil
2.
Dev Psychobiol ; 61(1): 29-42, 2019 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30447002

RESUMO

What happens to early acquired but later abandoned motor skills? To investigate effects of disuse on early-developing motor skills, we examined crawling in two groups of habitual crawlers (34 6-12-month-old infants and five adults with Uner Tan Syndrome) and two groups of rusty crawlers (27 11-12-year-old children and 13 college-aged adults). Habitual crawlers showed striking similarities in gait patterns, limbs supporting the body, and crawling speed, despite dramatic differences in crawling practice, posture, and body size. Habitual crawlers trotted predominantly, whereas rusty crawlers showed a variety of gait patterns. Within sequences, habitual crawlers and children showed more switches in gait patterns than young adults. Children crawled faster and kept fewer limbs on the grounds than the other groups. Old crawling patterns were retained despite disuse, but new ones were also added. Surprisingly, results indicate that nothing was lost with disuse, but some features of crawling were gained or altered.


Assuntos
Fenômenos Biomecânicos/fisiologia , Ataxia Cerebelar/fisiopatologia , Desenvolvimento Humano/fisiologia , Locomoção/fisiologia , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Adulto , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
3.
Dev Psychobiol ; 60(3): 265-277, 2018 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29442370

RESUMO

Previous research has revealed that fetuses detect and respond to extrauterine stimuli such as maternal movement and speech, but little attention has been cast on how fetuses may directly influence and respond to each other in the womb. This study investigated whether motor activity of E20 rat fetuses influenced the behavior of siblings in utero. Three experiments showed that; (a) contiguous siblings expressed a higher frequency of synchronized movement than noncontiguous siblings; (b) fetuses that lay between two siblings immobilized with curare showed less movement relative to fetuses between saline or uninjected controls; and (c) fetuses between two siblings behaviorally activated by the opioid agonist U50,488 also showed less activity and specific behavioral changes compared to controls. Our findings suggest that rat fetuses are directly impacted by sibling motor activity, and thus that a rudimentary form of communication between siblings may influence the development of fetuses in utero.


Assuntos
Comunicação Animal , Movimento Fetal/fisiologia , Relações entre Irmãos , Irmãos , (trans)-Isômero de 3,4-dicloro-N-metil-N-(2-(1-pirrolidinil)-ciclo-hexil)-benzenoacetamida/farmacologia , Analgésicos não Narcóticos/farmacologia , Animais , Feminino , Gravidez , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley
4.
Dev Psychobiol ; 58(3): 341-54, 2016 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26497472

RESUMO

Adults primarily walk to reach a new location, but why do infants walk? Do infants, like adults, walk to travel to a distant goal? We observed 30 13-month-old and 30 19-month-old infants during natural walking in a laboratory playroom. We characterized the bout structure of walking-when infants start and stop walking-to examine why infants start and stop walking. Locomotor activity was composed largely of brief spurts of walking. Half of 13-month-olds' bouts and 41% of 19-month-olds' bouts consisted of three or fewer steps-too few to carry infants to a distant goal. Most bouts ended in the middle of the floor, not at a recognizable goal. Survival analyses of the distribution of steps per bout indicated that the probability of continuing to walk was independent of the length of the ongoing bout; infants were just as likely to stop walking after five steps as after 50 steps and they showed no bias toward bouts long enough to carry them across the room to a goal. However, 13-month-olds showed an increased probability of stopping after 1-3 steps, and they did not initiate walking more frequently to compensate for their surfeit of short bouts. We propose that infants' natural walking is not intentionally directed at distant goals; rather, it is a stochastic process that serves exploratory functions. Relations between the bout structure of walking and other measures of walking suggest that locomotor exploration is constrained by walking skill in younger infants, but not in older infants.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Objetivos , Comportamento do Lactente/fisiologia , Caminhada/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino
5.
Dev Sci ; 18(2): 206-18, 2015 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25041056

RESUMO

Traditionally, crawling and sitting are considered distinct motor behaviors with different postures and functions. Ten- to 12-month-old infants were observed in the laboratory or in their homes while being coaxed to crawl continuously over long, straight walkways (Study 1; N = 20) and during spontaneous crawling during free play (Study 2; N = 20). In every context, infants stopped crawling to sit 3-6 times per minute. Transitions from crawling to sitting frequently turned infants' bodies away from the direction of heading; subsequent transitions back to crawling were offset by as much as 180° from the original direction of heading. Apparently, body reorientations result from the biomechanics of transitioning between crawling and sitting. Findings indicate that sustained, linear crawling is likely an epiphenomenon of how gait is studied in standard paradigms. Postural transitions between crawling and sitting are ubiquitous and can represent a functional unit of action. These transitions and the accompanying body reorientations likely have cascading effects for infants' exploration, visual perception, and spatial cognition.


Assuntos
Comportamento do Lactente/fisiologia , Locomoção/fisiologia , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Postura/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino
6.
Dev Psychobiol ; 57(4): 421-34, 2015 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25735558

RESUMO

Fetal rats can alter patterns of interlimb coordination after experience with a yoke that links two legs together. Yoke training results in a pronounced increase in conjugate limb movements (CLM). To determine whether yoke motor learning is mediated by spinal cord circuitry, fetal subjects at embryonic Day 20 (E20) received yoke training after mid-thoracic spinal cord transection or sham surgery. Both spinal and sham-treated fetuses exhibited an increase in CLM during training. In a second experiment, fetuses received initial yoke training, then were transected or sham treated before a 2nd training. Spinal and sham fetuses that were yoked during both training sessions exhibited a more rapid rise in CLM than those yoked only in the later session. These findings indicate that motor learning in fetal rats can be supported by spinal cord circuitry alone, and that savings implies a form of motor memory localized in the spinal cord.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Medula Espinal/cirurgia , Análise de Variância , Animais , Feminino , Gravidez , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley
7.
Dev Psychobiol ; 56(3): 327-39, 2014 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23389881

RESUMO

Early locomotion in the neonatal rat previously has been reported 3 days after birth during exposure to an odor of biological relevance (nest material). The current study explores if other ecologically relevant stimuli-amniotic fluid (AF) and milk-could evoke a similar locomotor response in the newborn rat and whether the endogenous opioid system mediates the response. Newborn rats tested 24 hr after birth were presented with the odors of AF or milk while placed in a runway. Pups expressed crawling and moved along the runway in response to direct exposure to the odors of AF and milk (Exp. 1). However, there was no evidence that this crawling response was altered after pretreatment with the opioid antagonist naloxone (Exp. 2). This study provides evidence of the capacity of AF and milk to evoke coordinated motor behavior, suggesting that they may play a role in the development of fundamental motor patterns.


Assuntos
Líquido Amniótico , Locomoção/fisiologia , Leite , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Odorantes , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Feminino , Locomoção/efeitos dos fármacos , Masculino , Atividade Motora/efeitos dos fármacos , Naloxona/farmacologia , Antagonistas de Entorpecentes/farmacologia , Ratos , Olfato/fisiologia
8.
Adv Child Dev Behav ; 66: 197-232, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39074922

RESUMO

What is natural behavior and how does it differ from laboratory-based behavior? The "natural" in natural behavior implies the everyday, complex, ever-changing, yet predictable environment in which children grow up. "Behavior" is motor action and is foundational to psychology, as it includes all things to function in everyday environments. Is behavior demonstrated in the laboratory un-natural? Suppose behavior emerges spontaneously, in a context that is most common to the animal but an observer is there to document it using particular research tools. Is that behavior natural or natural-ish? Methods can powerfully affect conclusions about infant experiences and learning. In the lab, tasks are typically narrowly constrained where infants and children have little opportunity to display the variety of behaviors in their repertoire. Data from naturalistic observations may paint a very different picture of learning and development from those based on structured tasks, exposing striking variability in the environment and behavior and new relations between the organism and its environment. Using motor development as a model system, in this chapter we compare frameworks, methods, and findings originating in the lab and in the field, applied and adapted in different settings. Specifically, we recount our journey of pursuing the study of cultural influences on motor development in Tajikistan, and the challenges, surprises, and lessons learned.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil , Humanos , Lactente , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Comportamento do Lactente , Meio Social , Cultura , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Aprendizagem
9.
Dev Psychobiol ; 55(4): 323-33, 2013 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22488203

RESUMO

Perinatal mammals show spontaneous movements that often appear random and uncoordinated. Here, we examined if spontaneous limb movements are responsive to a proprioceptive manipulation by applying a weight unilaterally to a forelimb of postnatal day 0 (P0; day of birth) and P1 rats. Weights were calibrated to approximate 0%, 25%, 50%, or 100% of the average mass of a forelimb, and were attached at the wrist. P0 and P1 pups showed different levels of activity during the period of limb weighting, in response to weight removal, and during the period after weighting. Pups exposed to 50% and 100% weights showed proportionately more activity in the nonweighted forelimb during the period of weighting, suggesting a threshold for evoking proprioceptive changes. Findings suggest that newborn rats use movement-related feedback to modulate spontaneous motor activity, and corroborate studies of human infants that have suggested a role for proprioception during early motor development.


Assuntos
Animais Recém-Nascidos/fisiologia , Retroalimentação Sensorial/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Propriocepção/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Feminino , Membro Anterior/fisiologia , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia
10.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 152(11): 3243-3265, 2023 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37535540

RESUMO

Researchers routinely infer learning and other unobservable psychological functions based on observable behavior. But what behavioral changes constitute evidence of learning? The standard approach is to infer learning based on a single behavior across individuals, including assumptions about the direction and magnitude of change (e.g., everyone should avoid falling repeatedly on a treacherous obstacle). Here we illustrate the benefits of an alternative "multiexpression, relativist, agnostic, individualized" approach. We assessed infant learning from falling based on multiple behaviors relative to each individual's baseline, agnostic about the direction and magnitude of behavioral change. We tested infants longitudinally (10.5-15 months of age) over the transition from crawling to walking. At each session, infants were repeatedly encouraged to crawl or walk over a fall-inducing foam pit interspersed with no-fall baseline trials on a rigid platform. Our approach revealed two learning profiles. Like adults in previous work, "pit-avoid" infants consistently avoided falling. In contrast, "pit-go" infants fell repeatedly across trials and sessions. However, individualized comparisons to baseline across multiple locomotor, exploratory, and social-emotional behaviors showed that pit-go infants also learned at every session. But they treated falling as an unimpactful "pratfall" rather than an aversive "pitfall." Pit-avoid infants displayed enhanced learning across sessions and partial transfer of learning from crawling to walking, whereas pit-go infants displayed neither. Thus, reliance on a predetermined, "one-size-fits-all" behavioral expression of a psychological function can obscure different behavioral profiles and lead to erroneous inferences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Acidentes por Quedas , Locomoção , Adulto , Humanos , Lactente , Caminhada , Afeto , Comportamento do Lactente
11.
Dev Psychobiol ; 52(8): 740-54, 2010 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21117244

RESUMO

The present study investigated if oral exposure to milk or amniotic fluid (AF) alters responsiveness to sensory stimulation in the neonatal rat, and whether these effects are mediated by the opioid system. Facial wiping evoked by intraoral lemon infusion was used as a measure of sensory responsiveness. Pups were tested in a supine posture, because they showed more paw-face strokes during facial wiping than pups tested prone (Experiment 1). Moreover, pups orally exposed to milk (Experiment 2) or AF (Experiment 3) showed a diminished wiping response to lemon compared to controls exposed to water. Blockade of opioid receptors with the nonselective antagonist naltrexone (Experiment 4) or the kappa antagonist nor-binaltorphimine (Experiment 5) reinstated higher levels of facial wiping after AF exposure. These findings confirm developmental continuity between fetal and neonatal behavioral responses to AF and the ability of AF to induce activity at kappa receptors of the endogenous opioid system.


Assuntos
Líquido Amniótico , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Atividade Motora/efeitos dos fármacos , Peptídeos Opioides/efeitos dos fármacos , Receptores Opioides/efeitos dos fármacos , Administração Oral , Análise de Variância , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Animais Lactentes , Feminino , Masculino , Leite , Naltrexona/análogos & derivados , Naltrexona/farmacologia , Antagonistas de Entorpecentes/farmacologia , Peptídeos Opioides/antagonistas & inibidores , Gravidez , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Estimulação Química
12.
Psychol Rev ; 115(3): 527-43, 2008 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18729590

RESUMO

Developmental trajectories provide the empirical foundation for theories about change processes during development. However, the ability to distinguish among alternative trajectories depends on how frequently observations are sampled. This study used real behavioral data, with real patterns of variability, to examine the effects of sampling at different intervals on characterization of the underlying trajectory. Data were derived from a set of 32 infant motor skills indexed daily during the first 18 months. Larger sampling intervals (2-31 days) were simulated by systematically removing observations from the daily data and interpolating over the gaps. Infrequent sampling caused decreasing sensitivity to fluctuations in the daily data: Variable trajectories erroneously appeared as step functions, and estimates of onset ages were increasingly off target. Sensitivity to variation decreased as an inverse power function of sampling interval, resulting in severe degradation of the trajectory with intervals longer than 7 days. These findings suggest that sampling rates typically used by developmental researchers may be inadequate to accurately depict patterns of variability and the shape of developmental change. Inadequate sampling regimes therefore may seriously compromise theories of development.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Destreza Motora , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino
13.
Child Dev ; 79(6): 1648-53, 2008.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19037939

RESUMO

Nativist and constructivist approaches to the study of development share a common emphasis on characterizing beginning and end states in development. This focus has highlighted the question of preservation and transformation-whether core aspects of the adult end state are present in the earliest manifestations during infancy. In contrast, a developmental systems approach emphasizes the process of developmental change. This perspective eschews the notions of objective starting and ending points in a developmental progression and rejects the idea that any particular factor should enjoy a privileged status in explaining developmental change. Using examples from motor development and animal behavior, we show how a developmental systems framework can avoid the pitfalls of the long and contentious debate about continuity versus qualitative change.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil , Teoria Psicológica , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Criança , Comportamento Infantil , Humanos , Lactente , Destreza Motora/fisiologia
14.
Behav Neurosci ; 119(3): 821-33, 2005 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15998204

RESUMO

The effects of serotonergic agonists were examined in intact and spinal fetuses, using an in vivo fetal rat preparation. On Gestational Day 20, fetuses were prepared with a midthoracic or sham spinal transection. Dose-response curves were obtained for quipazine (nonselective 5-hydroxytryptamine [5-HT] agonist; 1.0-10.0 mg/kg), CGS-12066A (5-HT-sub(1B) agonist; 1.0-30.0 mg/kg), and alpha-methylserotonin (alpha-Me-5-HT; 5-HT-sub-2 agonist; 0.2-15.0 mg/kg). During a 10-min test, each of the agonists (delivered via intraperitoneal injection) influenced fetal behavior: They increased the occurrence of head movements, mouthing, and hindlimb stepping. Quipazine and alpha-Me-5-HT also promoted hindlimb activity in spinal fetuses. Thus, stimulation of the fetal 5-HT system modulates motor activity at multiple levels of the developing central nervous system.


Assuntos
Membro Posterior/efeitos dos fármacos , Atividade Motora/efeitos dos fármacos , Desempenho Psicomotor/efeitos dos fármacos , Agonistas do Receptor de Serotonina/farmacologia , Traumatismos da Medula Espinal/tratamento farmacológico , Traumatismos da Medula Espinal/fisiopatologia , Análise de Variância , Animais , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Embrião de Mamíferos , Feminino , Membro Posterior/fisiopatologia , Masculino , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Gravidez , Quinoxalinas/farmacologia , Quipazina/farmacologia , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Serotonina/análogos & derivados , Serotonina/farmacologia
15.
Behav Neurosci ; 118(4): 835-44, 2004 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15301609

RESUMO

In the fetal rat, interlimb synchrony is a prominent form of temporally organized spontaneous motor activity in which movement of different limbs occurs at nearly the same instant. In the present study, synchrony profiles were created for different pairwise combinations of limbs over the last 5 days of gestation. Observed rates of synchrony differentiated from randomized time series from Gestational Day 19 to Day 21 (E19-E21), with forelimb synchrony emerging earlier than that of other limb pairs. Synchrony profiles were elevated at the shortest intervals between successive limb movements, indicating that movements became more tightly coupled toward the end of gestation. Interlimb synchrony appears to be a robust method of quantifying fetal movement and may prove useful as a tool for assessing prenatal nervous system functioning.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Embrionário e Fetal/fisiologia , Extremidades/embriologia , Movimento Fetal/fisiologia , Prenhez , Análise de Variância , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Comportamento Animal , Extremidades/fisiologia , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Idade Gestacional , Masculino , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Gravidez , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley
16.
J Appl Physiol (1985) ; 95(5): 1824-32, 2003 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12819214

RESUMO

Chronic altitude residence improves muscular performance at altitude, but the effect of intermittent altitude exposures (IAE) on muscular performance at altitude has not been defined. The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of 3 wk of IAE, in combination with rest and cycle training, on muscular performance at altitude. Six lowlanders (23 +/- 2 yr, 77 +/- 6 kg; means +/- SE) completed a cycle time trial and adductor pollicis endurance test at sea level and during a 30-h acute exposure to 4,300 m altitude equivalent (barometric pressure = 446 mmHg) once before (pre-IAE) and once after (post-IAE) a 3-wk period of IAE (4 h/day, 5 days/wk, 4,300 m). During each IAE, three subjects cycled for 45-60 min/day at 60%-70% of maximal O2 uptake and three subjects rested. Cycle training during each IAE did not appear to affect muscular performance at altitude. Thus data from all six subjects were combined. Three weeks of IAE resulted in 1) a 21 +/- 6% improvement (P < 0.05) in cycle time-trial performance (min) from pre-IAE (32.8 +/- 3.7) to post-IAE (24.8 +/- 1.2), 2) a 63 +/- 26% improvement (P < 0.05) in adductor pollicis endurance (min) from pre-IAE (9.2 +/- 2.8) to post-IAE (14.8 +/- 4.2), and 3) a 10 +/- 4% increase (P < 0.05) in resting arterial O2 saturation (%) from pre-IAE (82 +/- 2) to post-IAE (90 +/- 1). These improvements in muscular performance after IAE correlated strongly with increases in resting arterial O2 saturation and were comparable to those reported previously after chronic altitude residence. IAE may therefore be used as an alternative to chronic altitude residence to facilitate improvements in muscular performance in athletes, soldiers, mountaineers, shift workers, and others that are deployed to altitude.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Altitude , Câmaras de Exposição Atmosférica , Hipóxia/fisiopatologia , Músculo Esquelético/fisiologia , Adulto , Pressão Atmosférica , Ciclismo/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Montanhismo/fisiologia , Contração Muscular/fisiologia , Consumo de Oxigênio/fisiologia , Resistência Física/fisiologia
17.
J Comp Psychol ; 102(1): 78-82, 1988 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3365946

RESUMO

Rat fetuses were observed on Day 20 of gestation using a technique that permits direct observation of fetal behavior. The resulting time series was analyzed to assess cyclic organization in fetal movement. Fetal activity did not occur randomly but showed significant cyclic variation with a mean frequency of 0.61 cycle/min. This finding agrees with studies of human fetuses, which also exhibit short-period cyclicity in motor activity.


Assuntos
Movimento Fetal , Atividade Motora , Periodicidade , Ratos Endogâmicos/embriologia , Animais , Feminino , Idade Gestacional , Gravidez , Ratos
18.
Neurotoxicol Teratol ; 26(5): 663-71, 2004.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15315815

RESUMO

Although there is considerable interest in identifying methods to detect central nervous system impairment early in development, few behavioral assessment tools are available for detecting CNS deficits in the fetus. In the present study, methylazoxymethanol [MAM; Midwest Research Institute, (MRI)] was used to induce deficits in CNS development in fetal rats to assess effects on coordinated fetal behavior. Fetuses were exposed by administering MAM to pregnant rats on E17 of gestation via intraperitoneal injection and then were prepared for behavioral testing 3 days later on E20. After externalization from the uterus into a warm saline bath, fetal subjects received either an intraoral infusion of lemon extract to evoke a facial wiping response or were presented with an artificial nipple to evoke an oral grasping response. Interlimb coordination and paw-face contact during facial wiping were disrupted in MAM-exposed fetuses. Similarly, MAM exposure diminished the ability of fetuses to grasp or maintain oral contact with the artificial nipple. Although clear disruptions of movement coordination were seen in the MAM-treated subjects, there were no significant differences from saline controls in weight or anatomical measures. Together, these findings suggest that behavioral assessments of fetal motor coordination may be useful in identifying neural insult during prenatal development.


Assuntos
Doenças do Sistema Nervoso Central/induzido quimicamente , Acetato de Metilazoximetanol/análogos & derivados , Acetato de Metilazoximetanol/toxicidade , Movimento/efeitos dos fármacos , Efeitos Tardios da Exposição Pré-Natal , Teratogênicos/toxicidade , Análise de Variância , Animais , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Embrião de Mamíferos/efeitos dos fármacos , Embrião de Mamíferos/fisiopatologia , Face/fisiologia , Feminino , Injeções Intraperitoneais , Masculino , Boca/efeitos dos fármacos , Boca/fisiologia , Estimulação Física , Gravidez , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Tempo de Reação/efeitos dos fármacos
19.
PLoS One ; 9(7): e101758, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25029457

RESUMO

Since 2005, an extensive literature documents individuals from several families afflicted with "Uner Tan Syndrome (UTS)," a condition that in its most extreme form is characterized by cerebellar hypoplasia, loss of balance and coordination, impaired cognitive abilities, and habitual quadrupedal gait on hands and feet. Some researchers have interpreted habitual use of quadrupedalism by these individuals from an evolutionary perspective, suggesting that it represents an atavistic expression of our quadrupedal primate ancestry or "devolution." In support of this idea, individuals with "UTS" are said to use diagonal sequence quadrupedalism, a type of quadrupedal gait that distinguishes primates from most other mammals. Although the use of primate-like quadrupedal gait in humans would not be sufficient to support the conclusion of evolutionary "reversal," no quantitative gait analyses were presented to support this claim. Using standard gait analysis of 518 quadrupedal strides from video sequences of individuals with "UTS", we found that these humans almost exclusively used lateral sequence-not diagonal sequence-quadrupedal gaits. The quadrupedal gait of these individuals has therefore been erroneously described as primate-like, further weakening the "devolution" hypothesis. In fact, the quadrupedalism exhibited by individuals with UTS resembles that of healthy adult humans asked to walk quadrupedally in an experimental setting. We conclude that quadrupedalism in healthy adults or those with a physical disability can be explained using biomechanical principles rather than evolutionary assumptions.


Assuntos
Marcha/fisiologia , Primatas , Caminhada/fisiologia , Adulto , Animais , Criança , Evolução Molecular , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Postura/fisiologia , Síndrome
20.
J Cogn Dev ; 12(4): 411-423, 2011 Jan 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22140355

RESUMO

Research in developmental psychology requires sampling at different time points. Accurate depictions of developmental change provide a foundation for further empirical studies and theories about developmental mechanisms. However, overreliance on widely spaced sampling intervals in cross-sectional and longitudinal designs threatens the validity of the enterprise. This article discusses how to sample development in order to accurately discern the shape of developmental change. The ideal solution is daunting: to summarize behavior over 24-hour intervals and collect daily samples over the critical periods of change. We discuss the magnitude of errors due to undersampling, and the risks associated with oversampling. When daily sampling is not feasible, we offer suggestions for sampling methods that can provide preliminary reference points and provisional sketches of the general shape of a developmental trajectory. Denser sampling then can be applied strategically during periods of enhanced variability, inflections in the rate of developmental change, or in relation to key events or processes that may affect the course of change. Despite the challenges of dense repeated sampling, researchers must take seriously the problem of sampling on a developmental time scale if we are to know the true shape of developmental change.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA